首页 > 最新文献

Psychology and Aging最新文献

英文 中文
Age-related differences in memory encoding and retrieval during referential processing: A time-frequency analysis. 参照加工过程中记忆编码和检索的年龄差异:时频分析
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000857
Hossein Karimi, Megan A Boudewyn, David Vandenheever, Michele T Diaz

We investigated how lexical form similarity of referential candidates and ambiguity of following pronouns impact the encoding and retrieval of words from memory during sentence processing in younger and older adults. Critical sentences included two noun phrases (henceforth NPs) that were either phonologically and orthographically similar (Jason and Jacob/Jade) or dissimilar (Jason and Matt/Hannah), followed by a pronoun (e.g., he) that was either ambiguous or unambiguous (depending on the genders of the preceding NPs). We analyzed brain activity time-locked to the onsets of the second NP (NP2) and the pronoun to investigate the encoding and the retrieval of the NPs, respectively. During encoding NP2, older adults exhibited greater alpha power when NP1 had the same-gender, whereas younger adults showed no such effect, suggesting an increased need for inhibition for older adults during encoding. Moreover, although both groups exhibited an increase in alpha power for similar NPs, only younger adults exhibited a theta power increase, suggesting similarity-induced inhibition for both groups, but an additional maintenance cost only for younger adults. During retrieval (i.e., on the pronoun), we found that both pronominal ambiguity and form similarity resulted in greater theta power for younger adults, suggesting full pronominal processing and therefore more difficult retrieval, but smaller theta/alpha power for older adults, suggesting good-enough processing and therefore easier retrieval. Together with complementary behavioral results, our findings suggest that older adults resort to good-enough referential processing when the retrieval of relevant representations is cognitively demanding. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们研究了指代候选词的词形相似性和后续代词的模糊性如何影响年轻人和老年人在句子处理过程中对单词的编码和记忆检索。关键句子包括两个在语音和正字法上相似(Jason 和 Jacob/Jade)或不相似(Jason 和 Matt/Hannah)的名词短语(以下简称 NPs),后面跟着一个模糊或不模糊(取决于前面 NPs 的性别)的代词(例如,他)。我们分析了与第二个 NP(NP2)和代词的出现时间锁定的大脑活动,以分别研究 NP 的编码和检索。在编码 NP2 的过程中,当 NP1 具有相同的性别时,老年人表现出更大的α功率,而年轻人则没有这种效应,这表明老年人在编码过程中需要更多的抑制。此外,虽然两组人在相似的 NPs 时都表现出 alpha 功率的增加,但只有年轻成人表现出 theta 功率的增加,这表明两组人都有相似性引起的抑制,但只有年轻成人有额外的维持成本。在检索(即对代词的检索)过程中,我们发现年轻成人的代词模糊性和形式相似性都会导致更强的θ功率,这表明代词加工过程很充分,因此检索更困难;而年长成人的θ/α功率较小,这表明加工过程足够充分,因此检索更容易。结合补充行为结果,我们的研究结果表明,当对相关表征的检索有认知要求时,老年人会进行足够好的指代加工。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age-related differences in memory encoding and retrieval during referential processing: A time-frequency analysis.","authors":"Hossein Karimi, Megan A Boudewyn, David Vandenheever, Michele T Diaz","doi":"10.1037/pag0000857","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000857","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated how lexical form similarity of referential candidates and ambiguity of following pronouns impact the encoding and retrieval of words from memory during sentence processing in younger and older adults. Critical sentences included two noun phrases (henceforth NPs) that were either phonologically and orthographically similar (Jason and Jacob/Jade) or dissimilar (Jason and Matt/Hannah), followed by a pronoun (e.g., he) that was either ambiguous or unambiguous (depending on the genders of the preceding NPs). We analyzed brain activity time-locked to the onsets of the second NP (NP2) and the pronoun to investigate the encoding and the retrieval of the NPs, respectively. During encoding NP2, older adults exhibited greater alpha power when NP1 had the same-gender, whereas younger adults showed no such effect, suggesting an increased need for inhibition for older adults during encoding. Moreover, although both groups exhibited an increase in alpha power for similar NPs, only younger adults exhibited a theta power increase, suggesting similarity-induced inhibition for both groups, but an additional maintenance cost only for younger adults. During retrieval (i.e., on the pronoun), we found that both pronominal ambiguity and form similarity resulted in greater theta power for younger adults, suggesting full pronominal processing and therefore more difficult retrieval, but smaller theta/alpha power for older adults, suggesting good-enough processing and therefore easier retrieval. Together with complementary behavioral results, our findings suggest that older adults resort to good-enough referential processing when the retrieval of relevant representations is cognitively demanding. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":"39 7","pages":"731-749"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11537493/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142574883","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age differences in context use during reading and downstream effects on recognition memory. 阅读过程中语境使用的年龄差异及其对识别记忆的下游影响
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-29 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000845
Katja I Haeuser, Jutta Kray

It is well-known that sentential context modulates sentence processing. But does context also have effects that extend beyond the immediate moment, for example, by impacting the memory representations that people store? And are there age-related differences in this process? Here, we investigated this question. German readers who varied in age self-paced through constraining sentences that continued in a predictable or less predictable fashion. Participants' recognition memory was then tested for previously seen (i.e., "old") words and for initially predictable but not actually presented words (i.e., "lures"). The results showed that readers of all ages slowed down when reading unpredictable sentences. However, aging individuals maintained less sentence-specific information than younger adults: They not only understood sentential materials less correctly on the fly, but they also showed disproportionate rates of false remembering and less successful old-new discrimination in the recognition memory test. Of note, rates of false remembering were reduced in those aging readers who allocated more time toward reading unpredictable sentence continuations. Together, our results show that aging increases reliance on gist or schema-congruent processing but that more attentive encoding of text can buffer against some of the resulting memory distortions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

众所周知,句子语境会调节句子处理过程。但是,语境是否也会产生超越当下的影响,例如,影响人们存储的记忆表征?这一过程是否存在与年龄相关的差异?在此,我们对这一问题进行了研究。不同年龄段的德国读者通过限制性句子以可预测或不太可预测的方式自我调整步调。然后,测试了参与者对之前看到过的(即 "旧")单词和最初可预测但未实际呈现的单词(即 "诱饵")的识别记忆。结果显示,所有年龄段的读者在阅读无法预测的句子时都会放慢速度。然而,与年轻人相比,老年人对特定句子信息的保持较少:他们不仅即时理解句子材料的正确率较低,而且在辨认记忆测试中表现出不成比例的错误记忆率和较低的新旧辨别率。值得注意的是,那些花更多时间阅读不可预测句子的老年读者的错误记忆率有所降低。总之,我们的研究结果表明,衰老会增加对要点或模式一致性处理的依赖,但对文本进行更专注的编码可以缓冲由此产生的一些记忆失真。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age differences in context use during reading and downstream effects on recognition memory.","authors":"Katja I Haeuser, Jutta Kray","doi":"10.1037/pag0000845","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000845","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is well-known that sentential context modulates sentence processing. But does context also have effects that extend beyond the immediate moment, for example, by impacting the memory representations that people store? And are there age-related differences in this process? Here, we investigated this question. German readers who varied in age self-paced through constraining sentences that continued in a predictable or less predictable fashion. Participants' recognition memory was then tested for previously seen (i.e., \"old\") words and for initially predictable but not actually presented words (i.e., \"lures\"). The results showed that readers of all ages slowed down when reading unpredictable sentences. However, aging individuals maintained less sentence-specific information than younger adults: They not only understood sentential materials less correctly on the fly, but they also showed disproportionate rates of false remembering and less successful old-new discrimination in the recognition memory test. Of note, rates of false remembering were reduced in those aging readers who allocated more time toward reading unpredictable sentence continuations. Together, our results show that aging increases reliance on gist or schema-congruent processing but that more attentive encoding of text can buffer against some of the resulting memory distortions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"715-730"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113529","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Moderators of curiosity and information seeking in younger and older adults. 年轻人和老年人好奇心和信息搜寻的调节因素。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-19 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000847
Liyana T Swirsky, Julia Spaniol

The present study examined age differences in the influence of informational value cues on curiosity and information seeking. In two experiments, younger and older adults (total N = 514) rated their curiosity about content before having the opportunity to seek out more information. Experiment 1 examined the impact of social value on curiosity and information seeking about trivia. Online popularity metrics served as social value cues. Metric visibility increased engagement with high-popularity information for older adults, whereas it decreased engagement with low-popularity information for younger adults. Experiment 2 examined the impact of practical value on curiosity and information seeking about science facts. Personal and collective practical value were highlighted by linking the information to the domains of medicine and the environment, respectively. Patterns of curiosity and information seeking revealed greater sensitivity to collective practical value in older than younger adults. In both experiments, the relationship between curiosity and information seeking was stronger in older adults than in younger adults. Overall, these findings suggest that age differences in motivational priorities may lead to age differences in curiosity and information seeking. In addition to highlighting strategies for fostering curiosity in older learners, these findings may also inform digital literacy interventions aimed at reducing engagement with clickbait and misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究考察了信息价值线索对好奇心和信息搜寻影响的年龄差异。在两个实验中,年轻和年长的成年人(总人数= 514)在有机会寻求更多信息之前对他们对内容的好奇心进行了评分。实验 1 考察了社会价值对琐事的好奇心和信息搜索的影响。网络流行度量指标作为社会价值线索。对于老年人来说,指标的可见度提高了他们对高人气信息的参与度,而对于年轻人来说,指标的可见度降低了他们对低人气信息的参与度。实验 2 考察了实用价值对科学事实的好奇心和信息搜索的影响。通过将信息分别与医学和环境领域联系起来,强调了个人和集体的实用价值。从好奇心和信息搜寻的模式来看,老年人比年轻人对集体实用价值更敏感。在这两项实验中,老年人的好奇心与信息搜寻之间的关系比年轻人更密切。总之,这些研究结果表明,动机优先级的年龄差异可能会导致好奇心和信息搜寻的年龄差异。除了强调培养老年学习者好奇心的策略外,这些发现还可以为旨在减少点击诱饵和错误信息的数字扫盲干预提供参考。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Moderators of curiosity and information seeking in younger and older adults.","authors":"Liyana T Swirsky, Julia Spaniol","doi":"10.1037/pag0000847","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000847","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study examined age differences in the influence of informational value cues on curiosity and information seeking. In two experiments, younger and older adults (total <i>N</i> = 514) rated their curiosity about content before having the opportunity to seek out more information. Experiment 1 examined the impact of social value on curiosity and information seeking about trivia. Online popularity metrics served as social value cues. Metric visibility increased engagement with high-popularity information for older adults, whereas it decreased engagement with low-popularity information for younger adults. Experiment 2 examined the impact of practical value on curiosity and information seeking about science facts. Personal and collective practical value were highlighted by linking the information to the domains of medicine and the environment, respectively. Patterns of curiosity and information seeking revealed greater sensitivity to collective practical value in older than younger adults. In both experiments, the relationship between curiosity and information seeking was stronger in older adults than in younger adults. Overall, these findings suggest that age differences in motivational priorities may lead to age differences in curiosity and information seeking. In addition to highlighting strategies for fostering curiosity in older learners, these findings may also inform digital literacy interventions aimed at reducing engagement with clickbait and misinformation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"701-714"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142298839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Men and women transitioning to singlehood in young adulthood and midlife. 在青年期和中年期向单身过渡的男性和女性。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-17 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000859
Iris V Wahring, Franz J Neyer, Christiane A Hoppmann, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf

Research has long shown that men suffer more from romantic breakups than women. We predicted that men would on average be less inclined to initiate separation, decline with the separation more in well-being and increase more in loneliness, are less satisfied with singlehood, and desire a new partner more than women. We theorized that these gender differences in separation adaptation could be linked to men's higher reliance on their partners for emotional support. Because socioemotional selectivity theory suggests that with age people shift toward more fulfilling social connections, we also expected men's dependency on their partners for emotional support to be smaller in midlife than in young adulthood. To examine our hypotheses, we analyzed multiyear within-person longitudinal change data from 1,530 mostly unmarried participants from the annual German pairfam study who had experienced a relationship dissolution. We applied propensity score matching to compare separation-related changes in well-being and loneliness to case-matched controls who remained in a romantic relationship. Results showed that men relative to women were less likely to initiate separation, less satisfied with singlehood, and wished for a partner more. In contrast to our expectations, the gender differences observed did not differ by age, and no gender differences were found in separation-related changes in well-being and loneliness. Dissolution-related effects on well-being were only evident for marital relationships, while dissolution-related effects on loneliness were equally strong for marital and nonmarital dissolutions. Our study suggests that previous findings on gender-specific divorce-induced changes in well-being may not generalize to nonmarital dissolutions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

长期以来的研究表明,男性比女性更容易因感情破裂而痛苦。我们预测,与女性相比,男性平均较少主动提出分居,分居后幸福感下降更多,孤独感增加更多,对单身的满意度较低,更渴望找到新伴侣。我们推测,在分离适应方面的这些性别差异可能与男性更依赖其伴侣的情感支持有关。社会情感选择性理论认为,随着年龄的增长,人们会转向更充实的社会关系,因此我们也预期男性在中年时对伴侣情感支持的依赖程度会小于青年时期。为了验证我们的假设,我们分析了德国配对家庭年度研究中 1530 名经历过感情解体的参与者的多年人内纵向变化数据,这些参与者大多未婚。我们采用倾向得分匹配法,将与分离相关的幸福感和孤独感变化与保持恋爱关系的病例匹配对照组进行比较。结果显示,与女性相比,男性更不可能主动提出分居,对单身的满意度更低,也更希望有伴侣。与我们的预期相反,观察到的性别差异并不因年龄而异,而且在与分离相关的幸福感和孤独感变化方面也没有发现性别差异。与解体相关的幸福感影响仅在婚姻关系中明显,而与解体相关的孤独感影响在婚姻解体和非婚姻解体中同样强烈。我们的研究表明,以前关于离婚引起的幸福感变化的性别特异性研究结果可能无法推广到非婚姻解体中。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Men and women transitioning to singlehood in young adulthood and midlife.","authors":"Iris V Wahring, Franz J Neyer, Christiane A Hoppmann, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf","doi":"10.1037/pag0000859","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000859","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research has long shown that men suffer more from romantic breakups than women. We predicted that men would on average be less inclined to initiate separation, decline with the separation more in well-being and increase more in loneliness, are less satisfied with singlehood, and desire a new partner more than women. We theorized that these gender differences in separation adaptation could be linked to men's higher reliance on their partners for emotional support. Because socioemotional selectivity theory suggests that with age people shift toward more fulfilling social connections, we also expected men's dependency on their partners for emotional support to be smaller in midlife than in young adulthood. To examine our hypotheses, we analyzed multiyear within-person longitudinal change data from 1,530 mostly unmarried participants from the annual German pairfam study who had experienced a relationship dissolution. We applied propensity score matching to compare separation-related changes in well-being and loneliness to case-matched controls who remained in a romantic relationship. Results showed that men relative to women were less likely to initiate separation, less satisfied with singlehood, and wished for a partner more. In contrast to our expectations, the gender differences observed did not differ by age, and no gender differences were found in separation-related changes in well-being and loneliness. Dissolution-related effects on well-being were only evident for marital relationships, while dissolution-related effects on loneliness were equally strong for marital and nonmarital dissolutions. Our study suggests that previous findings on gender-specific divorce-induced changes in well-being may not generalize to nonmarital dissolutions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142478020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Which predicts longevity better: Satisfaction with life or purpose in life? 哪个更能预测长寿?生活满意度还是人生目标?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-15 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000802
Frank Martela, Elmeri Laitinen, Christian Hakulinen

Life satisfaction and purpose in life are fundamental yet separate ways to evaluate one's life. Both positively predict physical health and longevity, making them key factors for length and quality of life. However, we do not know which of them predicts mortality, when controlling for the influence of each other. Given that purpose in life involves a more active engagement with life and can help to cope with suffering, we hypothesize that purpose in life could be a more direct prospective predictor of longevity, overshadowing any effect of life satisfaction, when the two are pitted against each other as prospective predictors of longevity. To examine these hypotheses, we utilized Midlife in the U.S. survey, which is a 23-year follow-up study, (N = 5,993) and Cox proportional hazards models, repeating the analyses both without covariates and when controlling for various demographic and health-related variables. We show that both life satisfaction and purpose in life predict mortality when modeled separately. When life satisfaction, purpose in life and self-rated health were entered as simultaneous predictors of mortality, purpose in life remained a slightly more robust predictor of mortality, while life satisfaction became only marginally significant, suggesting that some of the factors that connect it to mortality are covered by the other two subjective evaluations. Overall, the results demonstrate that purpose in life is a robust predictor of mortality, and thus a key dimension of well-being to attend to as people age, while the predictive power of life satisfaction is more dependent on the choice of covariates. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

生活满意度和人生目标是评价一个人生活的基本但又不同的方式。两者都能积极预测身体健康和长寿,是决定生命长度和质量的关键因素。然而,我们并不知道在控制两者相互影响的情况下,哪一个能预测死亡率。鉴于生活目的涉及更积极地参与生活,并有助于应对痛苦,我们假设,当生活目的和生活满意度作为长寿的前瞻性预测因素相互对立时,生活目的可能是更直接的长寿前瞻性预测因素,其作用会盖过生活满意度。为了检验这些假设,我们利用了美国中年调查(一项为期 23 年的跟踪研究)(N = 5993)和 Cox 比例危险模型,在不考虑协变量以及控制各种人口和健康相关变量的情况下重复进行了分析。我们的研究表明,如果将生活满意度和生活目的分别建模,它们都能预测死亡率。当生活满意度、生活目的和自评健康同时作为死亡率的预测因子时,生活目的仍然是死亡率的一个略微稳健的预测因子,而生活满意度仅有轻微的显著性,这表明生活满意度与死亡率相关的一些因素已被其他两个主观评价所涵盖。总之,研究结果表明,生活目标是预测死亡率的有力指标,因此也是随着人们年龄的增长而需要关注的幸福感的一个关键维度,而生活满意度的预测能力则更多地取决于协变量的选择。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Which predicts longevity better: Satisfaction with life or purpose in life?","authors":"Frank Martela, Elmeri Laitinen, Christian Hakulinen","doi":"10.1037/pag0000802","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000802","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Life satisfaction and purpose in life are fundamental yet separate ways to evaluate one's life. Both positively predict physical health and longevity, making them key factors for length and quality of life. However, we do not know which of them predicts mortality, when controlling for the influence of each other. Given that purpose in life involves a more active engagement with life and can help to cope with suffering, we hypothesize that purpose in life could be a more direct prospective predictor of longevity, overshadowing any effect of life satisfaction, when the two are pitted against each other as prospective predictors of longevity. To examine these hypotheses, we utilized Midlife in the U.S. survey, which is a 23-year follow-up study, (<i>N</i> = 5,993) and Cox proportional hazards models, repeating the analyses both without covariates and when controlling for various demographic and health-related variables. We show that both life satisfaction and purpose in life predict mortality when modeled separately. When life satisfaction, purpose in life and self-rated health were entered as simultaneous predictors of mortality, purpose in life remained a slightly more robust predictor of mortality, while life satisfaction became only marginally significant, suggesting that some of the factors that connect it to mortality are covered by the other two subjective evaluations. Overall, the results demonstrate that purpose in life is a robust predictor of mortality, and thus a key dimension of well-being to attend to as people age, while the predictive power of life satisfaction is more dependent on the choice of covariates. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"589-598"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are social interactions perceived as more meaningful in older adulthood? 成年后的社会交往是否更有意义?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000827
Jana Nikitin, Martina Luchetti, Nejla Alimanovic, Fiona S Rupprecht, Angelina R Sutin

Social relationships accompany us throughout adulthood and are among the most important sources of meaning in a person's life. However, little is known about age differences in meaningfulness of social interactions across adulthood. According to socioemotional selectivity theory, as people age, they develop relatively stronger preferences for social relationships that are emotionally meaningful. Consequently, older adults may perceive social interactions in everyday life as more meaningful than younger adults. To test this hypothesis, the present study examined age-related differences in the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions using experience sampling data. Three-hundred six participants (56.9% women, 18-88 years) completed a total of 6,407 entries over 3 days. Results of the multilevel analyses showed that age was positively associated with perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions, controlling for relationship closeness and situation valence. In addition, the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions was positively related to subjective well-being both between and within all participants, indicating that meaningful social interactions are beneficial for well-being regardless of age. Thus, perceiving social interactions as meaningful could be one way that older people maintain a high level of well-being in their daily lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

社会关系伴随着我们整个成年期,是人一生中最重要的意义来源之一。然而,人们对整个成年期社会交往意义的年龄差异知之甚少。根据社会情感选择理论,随着年龄的增长,人们会对有情感意义的社会关系产生相对较强的偏好。因此,老年人可能会认为日常生活中的社会交往比年轻人更有意义。为了验证这一假设,本研究利用经验抽样数据,考察了与年龄相关的日常社会交往意义感知差异。三百六名参与者(56.9% 为女性,18-88 岁)在三天内共完成了 6,407 个条目。多层次分析的结果表明,年龄与日常社交互动的意义感呈正相关,与关系亲密程度和情境价值相关。此外,在所有参与者之间和参与者内部,日常社交互动的意义感都与主观幸福感呈正相关,这表明无论年龄大小,有意义的社交互动都对幸福感有益。因此,认为社交互动有意义可能是老年人在日常生活中保持高水平幸福感的一种方式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Are social interactions perceived as more meaningful in older adulthood?","authors":"Jana Nikitin, Martina Luchetti, Nejla Alimanovic, Fiona S Rupprecht, Angelina R Sutin","doi":"10.1037/pag0000827","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000827","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social relationships accompany us throughout adulthood and are among the most important sources of meaning in a person's life. However, little is known about age differences in meaningfulness of social interactions across adulthood. According to socioemotional selectivity theory, as people age, they develop relatively stronger preferences for social relationships that are emotionally meaningful. Consequently, older adults may perceive social interactions in everyday life as more meaningful than younger adults. To test this hypothesis, the present study examined age-related differences in the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions using experience sampling data. Three-hundred six participants (56.9% women, 18-88 years) completed a total of 6,407 entries over 3 days. Results of the multilevel analyses showed that age was positively associated with perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions, controlling for relationship closeness and situation valence. In addition, the perceived meaningfulness of daily social interactions was positively related to subjective well-being both between and within all participants, indicating that meaningful social interactions are beneficial for well-being regardless of age. Thus, perceiving social interactions as meaningful could be one way that older people maintain a high level of well-being in their daily lives. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"599-607"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do early midlife work characteristics predict 20-year change in control beliefs? 中年早期的工作特征能否预测 20 年后控制信念的变化?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000830
Johanna Hartung, Lena Stahlhofen, Oliver K Schilling, Hans-Werner Wahl, Gizem Hülür

Previous research showed the importance of control beliefs for many life outcomes. The present study examines associations between subjectively perceived work environment and objectively measured work activities at the beginning of midlife as a central developmental phase in the context of work, with control beliefs across the subsequent 20 years. We analyzed four-wave longitudinal data from N = 374 participants (born 1950-1952; Mage baseline = 44 years, SD = 1; 44% women) from the Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study of Adult Development and Aging within a structural equation modeling framework. Over 20 years and overall, internal control beliefs were stable, while external control beliefs decreased. Individuals who reported higher task variety and better social integration at work at baseline also reported higher levels of control beliefs for positive life outcomes. In addition, higher social integration at work at baseline was related to lower levels of external control beliefs. Work characteristics at baseline were not associated with individual differences in change in control beliefs across the 20-year observational interval. In summary, our findings suggest that work experiences at the prime of job-related development around the midst of the fifth decade of life do not play a major role in subsequent control beliefs development across 20 years. However, investigations measuring control beliefs as well as work characteristics continuously over a long period of time are necessary to shed light on reciprocal influences between work and personality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

以往的研究表明,控制信念对许多人生结果都很重要。本研究探讨了中年之初的主观感知工作环境和客观测量的工作活动与随后 20 年的控制信念之间的关联,中年是工作方面的一个核心发展阶段。我们在结构方程建模框架内分析了 "成人发展与老龄化跨学科纵向研究"(Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study of Adult Development and Aging)中 N = 374 名参与者(1950-1952 年出生;年龄基线 = 44 岁,SD = 1;44% 为女性)的四波纵向数据。在 20 年的总体时间里,内部控制信念保持稳定,而外部控制信念则有所下降。那些在基线时报告工作任务多样性更高、社会融合度更好的人,也报告他们对积极生活结果的控制信念水平更高。此外,基线时工作中社会融合度较高的人与外部控制信念水平较低有关。基线时的工作特征与 20 年观察期间控制信念变化的个体差异无关。总之,我们的研究结果表明,在人生的第五个十年中期左右,在与工作相关的发展的黄金时期的工作经历并不会对随后跨越 20 年的控制信念的发展起主要作用。然而,有必要对控制信念以及工作特征进行长期持续的测量,以揭示工作与人格之间的相互影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do early midlife work characteristics predict 20-year change in control beliefs?","authors":"Johanna Hartung, Lena Stahlhofen, Oliver K Schilling, Hans-Werner Wahl, Gizem Hülür","doi":"10.1037/pag0000830","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000830","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research showed the importance of control beliefs for many life outcomes. The present study examines associations between subjectively perceived work environment and objectively measured work activities at the beginning of midlife as a central developmental phase in the context of work, with control beliefs across the subsequent 20 years. We analyzed four-wave longitudinal data from <i>N</i> = 374 participants (born 1950-1952; <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> baseline = 44 years, <i>SD</i> = 1; 44% women) from the Interdisciplinary Longitudinal Study of Adult Development and Aging within a structural equation modeling framework. Over 20 years and overall, internal control beliefs were stable, while external control beliefs decreased. Individuals who reported higher task variety and better social integration at work at baseline also reported higher levels of control beliefs for positive life outcomes. In addition, higher social integration at work at baseline was related to lower levels of external control beliefs. Work characteristics at baseline were not associated with individual differences in change in control beliefs across the 20-year observational interval. In summary, our findings suggest that work experiences at the prime of job-related development around the midst of the fifth decade of life do not play a major role in subsequent control beliefs development across 20 years. However, investigations measuring control beliefs as well as work characteristics continuously over a long period of time are necessary to shed light on reciprocal influences between work and personality. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"672-687"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to "Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis" by Cheng et al. (2022). Correction to "Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment?Cheng等人(2022年)所作的 "全面系统回顾和荟萃分析"。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000840

Reports an error in "Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis" by Sheung-Tak Cheng, Kin-Kit Li, Peggy P. L. Or and Andrés Losada (Psychology and Aging, 2022[Dec], Vol 37[8], 929-953). In the original article, two of the labels in the first column of Table 6 were incorrect. The first Recommended intervention should have been "Education with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b)," and the first Moderately recommended intervention should have been "Education with probable psychological components (psychoeducation-a)." Despite the misnomers, the nature of the intervention type can be clearly discerned from the description in the far-right column. The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2022-76749-001). Some reviews suggest benefits of nonpharmacological interventions for informal caregivers of people with dementia and mild cognitive impairment. These benefits may transfer to the care-recipients (CRs) through increased caregiving capability, reduced burden and depression among caregivers, and decreased negative mood contagion. However, large-scale review on these effects on the CRs is lacking. We searched PsycINFO, CINAHL with Full Text, MEDLINE, and PubMed from inception to end of 2020 and found 142 articles that reported randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of caregiver interventions using CR outcomes. Interventions were found to reduce neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in general and behavioral and mood disturbance specifically, enhance cognition and quality of life, and delay institutionalization and mortality, with care coordination/case management, educational intervention with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b), and direct training of the CR (with caregiver involvement) being the more potent interventions. The kinds of benefit depend on the types of intervention. NPS was reduced by psychoeducation-b, care coordination/case management, and CR training. Cognition and quality of life were enhanced by CR training and care coordination/case management, respectively. Institutionalization was delayed by multicomponent interventions and respite (based on one study). However, the effects were generally small to very small. Together with existing findings on caregiver outcomes, a tripartite scaffolding model of caregiver support is proposed. The model is composed of three components: (a) care coordination/case management (i.e., enhanced usual care), (b) psychoeducation-b, and (c) CR training. Future directions in terms of developing consensual guidelines, a registry of intervention manuals, and family-centered programs with flexibility in delivery are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

报告了 Sheung-Tak Cheng、Kin-Kit Li、Peggy P.L. Or 和 Andrés Losada 的 "护理干预是否能改善痴呆症和轻度认知障碍患者亲属的预后?A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis"(《心理学与老龄化》,2022 年 12 月,第 37 卷[8],929-953 页)中的错误。在原文中,表 6 第一栏中有两个标签不正确。第一个推荐干预应该是 "具有心理治疗成分的教育(心理教育-b)",第一个中度推荐干预应该是 "可能具有心理成分的教育(心理教育-a)"。尽管用词不当,但从最右边一栏的描述中可以清楚地看出干预类型的性质。本文网络版已作更正。(原文摘要如下,载于 2022-76749-001 号记录)。一些综述表明,非药物干预对痴呆症和轻度认知障碍患者的非正式照顾者有益。这些益处可能会通过提高护理能力、减轻护理者的负担和抑郁以及减少负面情绪传染等方式传递给护理对象(CRs)。然而,目前还缺乏有关这些对护理对象影响的大规模研究。我们检索了 PsycINFO、CINAHL(含全文)、MEDLINE 和 PubMed(从开始到 2020 年底),发现有 142 篇文章报道了使用 CR 结果对照顾者进行干预的随机对照试验 (RCT)。研究发现,干预措施可以减少一般的神经精神症状(NPS),特别是行为和情绪障碍,提高认知能力和生活质量,延迟入院时间和死亡率,其中护理协调/病例管理、包含心理治疗成分的教育干预(心理教育-b)和直接培训 CR(有护理者参与)是更有效的干预措施。获益的种类取决于干预的类型。心理教育-b、护理协调/个案管理和 CR 培训降低了 NPS。CR 培训和护理协调/个案管理分别提高了认知能力和生活质量。多组分干预和暂休(基于一项研究)推迟了入院时间。然而,效果一般较小或非常小。结合现有的关于照护者成果的研究结果,提出了照护者支持的三方支架模式。该模式由三个部分组成:(a) 护理协调/个案管理(即增强型常规护理),(b) 心理教育-b,以及 (c) CR 培训。本文讨论了未来的发展方向,即制定共识指南、登记干预手册以及以家庭为中心、灵活实施的项目。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Correction to \"Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis\" by Cheng et al. (2022).","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pag0000840","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000840","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Reports an error in \"Do caregiver interventions improve outcomes in relatives with dementia and mild cognitive impairment? A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis\" by Sheung-Tak Cheng, Kin-Kit Li, Peggy P. L. Or and Andrés Losada (<i>Psychology and Aging</i>, 2022[Dec], Vol 37[8], 929-953). In the original article, two of the labels in the first column of Table 6 were incorrect. The first Recommended intervention should have been \"Education with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b),\" and the first Moderately recommended intervention should have been \"Education with probable psychological components (psychoeducation-a).\" Despite the misnomers, the nature of the intervention type can be clearly discerned from the description in the far-right column. The online version of this article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2022-76749-001). Some reviews suggest benefits of nonpharmacological interventions for informal caregivers of people with dementia and mild cognitive impairment. These benefits may transfer to the care-recipients (CRs) through increased caregiving capability, reduced burden and depression among caregivers, and decreased negative mood contagion. However, large-scale review on these effects on the CRs is lacking. We searched PsycINFO, CINAHL with Full Text, MEDLINE, and PubMed from inception to end of 2020 and found 142 articles that reported randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of caregiver interventions using CR outcomes. Interventions were found to reduce neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPS) in general and behavioral and mood disturbance specifically, enhance cognition and quality of life, and delay institutionalization and mortality, with care coordination/case management, educational intervention with psychotherapeutic components (psychoeducation-b), and direct training of the CR (with caregiver involvement) being the more potent interventions. The kinds of benefit depend on the types of intervention. NPS was reduced by psychoeducation-b, care coordination/case management, and CR training. Cognition and quality of life were enhanced by CR training and care coordination/case management, respectively. Institutionalization was delayed by multicomponent interventions and respite (based on one study). However, the effects were generally small to very small. Together with existing findings on caregiver outcomes, a tripartite scaffolding model of caregiver support is proposed. The model is composed of three components: (a) care coordination/case management (i.e., enhanced usual care), (b) psychoeducation-b, and (c) CR training. Future directions in terms of developing consensual guidelines, a registry of intervention manuals, and family-centered programs with flexibility in delivery are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"643"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social and general cognition are uniquely associated with social connectedness in later life. 社会认知和一般认知与晚年的社会联系有着独特的联系。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-20 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000831
Anne C Krendl, Siyun Peng, Lucas J Hamilton, Brea L Perry

The mechanisms by which older adults maintain large, complex social networks are not well understood. Prior work has primarily focused on general cognitive ability (e.g., executive function, episodic memory), largely overlooking social cognition-the ability to process, store, and remember social information. Because social cognition plays a key role in navigating social interactions and is distinct from general cognition, we examined whether general and social cognition uniquely predicted the nature of older adults' personal social networks. Our study leveraged comprehensive measures of general cognition (executive function, episodic memory), social cognition (face memory and dynamic measures of cognitive and affective theory of mind), and a rigorous measure of personal social networks from 143 community-dwelling older adults. We found that, when modeled together and controlling for sociodemographic variables, only executive function and dynamic cognitive theory of mind positively predicted having social networks with relatively unfamiliar, loosely connected others, accounting for 17% of the unique variance in older adults' social connectedness. Interestingly, having a social network comprised primarily of close, tightly knit relationships was negatively associated with affective theory of mind performance. Findings are discussed in the context of the social-cognitive resource framework-which suggests that social cognition may be more engaged in relatively unfamiliar, versus close, interactions. Specifically, our results show that social-cognitive processes may be relatively automatic for individuals whose primary social relationships are very close but may be more strongly engaged for individuals whose interactions include at least some relatively less close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

人们对老年人维持庞大而复杂的社交网络的机制还不甚了解。之前的研究主要集中于一般认知能力(如执行功能、外显记忆),在很大程度上忽略了社会认知--处理、存储和记忆社会信息的能力。由于社会认知在社会交往中起着关键作用,而且有别于一般认知,因此我们研究了一般认知和社会认知是否能独特地预测老年人个人社交网络的性质。我们的研究利用了对 143 名居住在社区的老年人的一般认知(执行功能、外显记忆)、社会认知(面孔记忆以及认知和情感心智理论的动态测量)和个人社交网络的严格测量的综合测量。我们发现,如果将执行功能和动态认知心智理论放在一起建模并控制社会人口学变量,只有执行功能和动态认知心智理论能积极预测与相对陌生、联系松散的人建立社交网络的情况,占老年人社交联系独特差异的 17%。有趣的是,社交网络主要由关系密切、紧密的人组成,这与情感心智理论的表现呈负相关。研究结果将在社会认知资源框架的背景下进行讨论--该框架认为,相对于亲密的互动,社会认知可能更多参与到相对陌生的互动中。具体来说,我们的研究结果表明,对于那些主要社会关系非常密切的人来说,社会认知过程可能会相对自动,但对于那些互动至少包括一些相对不太密切的关系的人来说,社会认知过程可能会更强烈。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Social and general cognition are uniquely associated with social connectedness in later life.","authors":"Anne C Krendl, Siyun Peng, Lucas J Hamilton, Brea L Perry","doi":"10.1037/pag0000831","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000831","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The mechanisms by which older adults maintain large, complex social networks are not well understood. Prior work has primarily focused on general cognitive ability (e.g., executive function, episodic memory), largely overlooking social cognition-the ability to process, store, and remember social information. Because social cognition plays a key role in navigating social interactions and is distinct from general cognition, we examined whether general and social cognition uniquely predicted the nature of older adults' personal social networks. Our study leveraged comprehensive measures of general cognition (executive function, episodic memory), social cognition (face memory and dynamic measures of cognitive and affective theory of mind), and a rigorous measure of personal social networks from 143 community-dwelling older adults. We found that, when modeled together and controlling for sociodemographic variables, only executive function and dynamic cognitive theory of mind positively predicted having social networks with relatively unfamiliar, loosely connected others, accounting for 17% of the unique variance in older adults' social connectedness. Interestingly, having a social network comprised primarily of close, tightly knit relationships was negatively associated with affective theory of mind performance. Findings are discussed in the context of the social-cognitive resource framework-which suggests that social cognition may be more engaged in relatively unfamiliar, versus close, interactions. Specifically, our results show that social-cognitive processes may be relatively automatic for individuals whose primary social relationships are very close but may be more strongly engaged for individuals whose interactions include at least some relatively less close relationships. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"644-657"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11851866/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141427977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-based stereotype threat in the workplace: A daily diary study of antecedents and mechanisms. 工作场所中基于年龄的刻板印象威胁:关于前因和机制的每日日记研究。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-24 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000839
Sophie Coulon, Courtney von Hippel, Kim Peters

Both older and younger employees are stereotyped at work, making them susceptible to age-based stereotype threat or the concern about being negatively stereotyped based on their age. To date, there is limited understanding of the workplace events that spark age-based stereotype threat and the mechanisms through which they do so. The current daily diary study aimed to assess the frequency with which older and younger employees experience various events that have been identified as potential antecedents of age-based stereotype threat (e.g., being overlooked for training or development opportunities, feeling excluded from informal socializing) as well as their association with experiences of stereotype threat. As predicted, we found that employees who reported more frequent experiences of these events subsequently reported greater feelings of stereotype threat. This association was partially mediated by the capacity of these events to increase the salience of a participant's age. There was also evidence of reciprocal association, whereby employees who felt more stereotype threat were also more likely to subsequently report experiencing these events. Employee age did not moderate the relationship between daily feelings of stereotype threat and daily outcomes, suggesting that everyday feelings of stereotype threat may be equally problematic for both older and younger employees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

无论是年长员工还是年轻员工,在工作中都会受到刻板印象的影响,使他们容易受到年龄刻板印象威胁,或担心因年龄而受到负面刻板印象的影响。迄今为止,人们对引发年龄刻板印象威胁的工作场所事件及其机制的了解还很有限。当前的每日日记研究旨在评估老年员工和年轻员工经历各种事件的频率,这些事件已被确认为年龄刻板印象威胁的潜在前因(如被忽视培训或发展机会、感觉被排除在非正式社交之外),以及它们与刻板印象威胁经历之间的关联。正如我们所预测的那样,我们发现那些报告了更频繁经历这些事件的员工随后报告了更强烈的刻板印象威胁感。这种关联的部分原因是这些事件能够提高参与者年龄的显著性。也有证据表明存在相互关联,即感受到更多刻板印象威胁的员工随后也更有可能报告经历过这些事件。员工年龄并没有调节日常刻板印象威胁感与日常结果之间的关系,这表明日常刻板印象威胁感可能对年龄较大和较年轻的员工造成同样的问题。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age-based stereotype threat in the workplace: A daily diary study of antecedents and mechanisms.","authors":"Sophie Coulon, Courtney von Hippel, Kim Peters","doi":"10.1037/pag0000839","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000839","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Both older and younger employees are stereotyped at work, making them susceptible to age-based stereotype threat or the concern about being negatively stereotyped based on their age. To date, there is limited understanding of the workplace events that spark age-based stereotype threat and the mechanisms through which they do so. The current daily diary study aimed to assess the frequency with which older and younger employees experience various events that have been identified as potential antecedents of age-based stereotype threat (e.g., being overlooked for training or development opportunities, feeling excluded from informal socializing) as well as their association with experiences of stereotype threat. As predicted, we found that employees who reported more frequent experiences of these events subsequently reported greater feelings of stereotype threat. This association was partially mediated by the capacity of these events to increase the salience of a participant's age. There was also evidence of reciprocal association, whereby employees who felt more stereotype threat were also more likely to subsequently report experiencing these events. Employee age did not moderate the relationship between daily feelings of stereotype threat and daily outcomes, suggesting that everyday feelings of stereotype threat may be equally problematic for both older and younger employees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":" ","pages":"688-699"},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141447381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychology and Aging
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1