首页 > 最新文献

Psychology and Aging最新文献

英文 中文
Effects of age and word frequency on Korean visual word recognition: Evidence from a web-based large-scale lexical-decision task. 年龄和单词频率对韩语视觉单词识别的影响:基于网络的大规模词汇决策任务提供的证据。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-18 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000793
Hyunah Baek, Peter C Gordon, Wonil Choi

The present study examined age differences in word-frequency effects in Korean visual word recognition through a large-scale, web-based lexical-decision task. Four hundred ninety-seven adult Korean speakers in their 20s through 60s participated in the task, in which they decided the lexicality of 120 Korean words varying in frequency and 120 nonwords. Overall, both lexical-decision accuracy and response times increased with age, and more frequent words were recognized more rapidly than less frequent words. We also found significant effects of participants' reading skill as well as age of acquisition of words. Crucially, despite older adults' generally slower reaction times, there was no hint of any interaction between participant age and word frequency on lexical-decision times. This result adds to the literature on age-related changes in visual word recognition and provides evidence for stable word-frequency effect across the adult age spectrum. These findings are discussed with different hypotheses of lexical access and aging proposed in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究通过一项大规模、基于网络的词汇判断任务,研究了韩语视觉词汇识别中词频效应的年龄差异。497 名 20 多岁到 60 多岁的成年韩语使用者参加了这项任务,他们在这项任务中决定了 120 个不同词频的韩语单词和 120 个非单词的词性。总体而言,词性判断的准确率和反应时间都随着年龄的增长而增加,识别频率较高的词比识别频率较低的词更快。我们还发现,受试者的阅读技能和掌握单词的年龄也有明显的影响。最重要的是,尽管老年人的反应时间普遍较慢,但受试者的年龄和单词频率对词汇判断时间没有任何交互作用。这一结果补充了有关视觉单词识别中与年龄相关变化的文献,并为单词频率效应在成人年龄范围内的稳定性提供了证据。这些发现与文献中提出的词汇访问和衰老的不同假说进行了讨论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Effects of age and word frequency on Korean visual word recognition: Evidence from a web-based large-scale lexical-decision task.","authors":"Hyunah Baek, Peter C Gordon, Wonil Choi","doi":"10.1037/pag0000793","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000793","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study examined age differences in word-frequency effects in Korean visual word recognition through a large-scale, web-based lexical-decision task. Four hundred ninety-seven adult Korean speakers in their 20s through 60s participated in the task, in which they decided the lexicality of 120 Korean words varying in frequency and 120 nonwords. Overall, both lexical-decision accuracy and response times increased with age, and more frequent words were recognized more rapidly than less frequent words. We also found significant effects of participants' reading skill as well as age of acquisition of words. Crucially, despite older adults' generally slower reaction times, there was no hint of any interaction between participant age and word frequency on lexical-decision times. This result adds to the literature on age-related changes in visual word recognition and provides evidence for stable word-frequency effect across the adult age spectrum. These findings are discussed with different hypotheses of lexical access and aging proposed in the literature. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139485931","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Audiovisual speech perception in noise in younger and older bilinguals. 年轻和年长双语者在噪音中的视听语音感知。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000799
Alexandre Chauvin, Sophie Pellerin, Anna-Francesca Boatswain-Jacques, Jean-Louis René, Natalie A Phillips

Speech perception in noise becomes increasingly difficult with age. Similarly, bilinguals often have difficulty with speech perception in noise in their second language (L2) due to less developed language knowledge in L2. Little is known about older bilinguals, who experience age-related sensory and cognitive changes but have extensive L2 experience. Furthermore, while audiovisual (AV) speech cues and supportive sentence context facilitate speech perception in noise in native listeners, much less is known for bilingual listeners, particularly older bilinguals. This study investigated how much young (n = 30) and older (n = 31) French-English/English-French bilinguals benefit from AV speech cues and sentence context in their first (L1) and second language during speech perception in noise. Participants completed the task in L1 and L2. Importantly, the level of background noise was individually calibrated for each participant to equate performance during the baseline task. The results suggest that there were no age differences in overall performance during the speech perception in noise task. However, older adults showed smaller language effects and smaller modality effects (i.e., comparing auditory-only speech perception to audiovisual speech perception) compared to younger adults. Furthermore, both young and older bilinguals benefited from AV speech cues and sentence context in both of their languages. However, during L2 speech perception in noise, visual speech cues were particularly important for young adults. In contrast, older adults show equal benefit from visual speech cues in L1 and L2. Taken together, these findings extend current models of AV speech perception to a second language in both younger and older adults. Furthermore, the findings suggest that bilinguals are likely to benefit from visual speech cues and supportive sentence context, although this benefit may differ slightly between younger and older bilinguals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

随着年龄的增长,噪音中的语音感知变得越来越困难。同样,双语者由于对第二语言(L2)的语言知识掌握较少,往往难以在噪声中感知语音。老年双语者经历着与年龄相关的感官和认知变化,但却拥有丰富的第二语言经验,因此人们对他们的情况知之甚少。此外,虽然视听(AV)语音线索和支持性句子语境有助于母语听者在噪声中感知语音,但对双语听者,尤其是老年双语听者的了解却少得多。本研究调查了年轻(n = 30)和年长(n = 31)的法-英/英-法双语者在噪声中感知语音时从第一语言(L1)和第二语言的视听语音提示和句子语境中获益的程度。参与者用第一语言和第二语言完成任务。重要的是,每位受试者的背景噪音水平都经过单独校准,以便与基线任务中的表现相等。结果表明,在噪声中的语音感知任务中,总体表现没有年龄差异。不过,与年轻人相比,老年人表现出的语言效应和模式效应(即比较纯听觉语音感知和视听语音感知)较小。此外,年轻和年长的双语者都能从两种语言的视听语音线索和句子语境中获益。然而,在噪音中进行第二语言语音感知时,视觉语音线索对年轻人尤为重要。相反,老年双语者在 L1 和 L2 中从视觉语音线索中获益相同。综上所述,这些发现将目前的视听语音感知模型扩展到了年轻人和老年人的第二语言。此外,研究结果表明,双语者很可能从视觉语音提示和支持性句子语境中获益,尽管这种获益在年轻双语者和老年双语者之间可能略有不同。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Audiovisual speech perception in noise in younger and older bilinguals.","authors":"Alexandre Chauvin, Sophie Pellerin, Anna-Francesca Boatswain-Jacques, Jean-Louis René, Natalie A Phillips","doi":"10.1037/pag0000799","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000799","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Speech perception in noise becomes increasingly difficult with age. Similarly, bilinguals often have difficulty with speech perception in noise in their second language (L2) due to less developed language knowledge in L2. Little is known about older bilinguals, who experience age-related sensory and cognitive changes but have extensive L2 experience. Furthermore, while audiovisual (AV) speech cues and supportive sentence context facilitate speech perception in noise in native listeners, much less is known for bilingual listeners, particularly older bilinguals. This study investigated how much young (<i>n</i> = 30) and older (<i>n</i> = 31) French-English/English-French bilinguals benefit from AV speech cues and sentence context in their first (L1) and second language during speech perception in noise. Participants completed the task in L1 and L2. Importantly, the level of background noise was individually calibrated for each participant to equate performance during the baseline task. The results suggest that there were no age differences in overall performance during the speech perception in noise task. However, older adults showed smaller language effects and smaller modality effects (i.e., comparing auditory-only speech perception to audiovisual speech perception) compared to younger adults. Furthermore, both young and older bilinguals benefited from AV speech cues and sentence context in both of their languages. However, during L2 speech perception in noise, visual speech cues were particularly important for young adults. In contrast, older adults show equal benefit from visual speech cues in L1 and L2. Taken together, these findings extend current models of AV speech perception to a second language in both younger and older adults. Furthermore, the findings suggest that bilinguals are likely to benefit from visual speech cues and supportive sentence context, although this benefit may differ slightly between younger and older bilinguals. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139651905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adult age differences in noninstrumental information-seeking strategies. 成人非工具性信息搜索策略的年龄差异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000806
Greta M Fastrich, Lily FitzGibbon, Johnny K Lau, Sumeyye Aslan, Michiko Sakaki, Kou Murayama

We often seek information without any explicit incentives or goals (i.e., noninstrumental information seeking, often noted as a manifestation of curiosity). Does noninstrumental information-seeking change with age? We tried to answer the question by making a critical distinction between two information-seeking behaviors: diversive information seeking (i.e., information seeking for topics a person knows little about) and specific information seeking (i.e., information seeking to deepen a person's existing knowledge of a topic). Five hundred participants (age range: 12-79 years old) spontaneously read new facts about different topics. After reading each fact, participants were given the choice to read more facts about the current topic or return to the selection menu to learn about a new topic. We found that with increasing age, participants chose to explore more facts within a topic (i.e., increased specific information seeking) and switched less frequently to new topics (i.e., decreased diversive information seeking). These results indicate that while young people seek out a broader range of information, as people grow older, they develop a preference to deepen their existing knowledge. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

我们经常在没有任何明确动机或目标的情况下寻求信息(即非工具性信息寻求,通常被视为好奇心的一种表现)。非工具性信息搜寻会随着年龄的增长而改变吗?为了回答这个问题,我们对两种信息搜寻行为进行了重要的区分:分散性信息搜寻(即针对个人知之甚少的主题进行信息搜寻)和特定信息搜寻(即为加深个人对某一主题的现有知识而进行的信息搜寻)。五百名参与者(年龄在 12-79 岁之间)自发阅读了关于不同主题的新事实。在阅读完每个事实后,参与者可以选择阅读更多关于当前主题的事实,或者返回选择菜单了解新的主题。我们发现,随着年龄的增长,参与者会选择在一个主题中探索更多的事实(即增加了特定信息的寻求),而较少切换到新的主题(即减少了多元化信息的寻求)。这些结果表明,虽然年轻人寻求更广泛的信息,但随着年龄的增长,他们会更倾向于加深已有的知识。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Adult age differences in noninstrumental information-seeking strategies.","authors":"Greta M Fastrich, Lily FitzGibbon, Johnny K Lau, Sumeyye Aslan, Michiko Sakaki, Kou Murayama","doi":"10.1037/pag0000806","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000806","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We often seek information without any explicit incentives or goals (i.e., noninstrumental information seeking, often noted as a manifestation of curiosity). Does noninstrumental information-seeking change with age? We tried to answer the question by making a critical distinction between two information-seeking behaviors: diversive information seeking (i.e., information seeking for topics a person knows little about) and specific information seeking (i.e., information seeking to deepen a person's existing knowledge of a topic). Five hundred participants (age range: 12-79 years old) spontaneously read new facts about different topics. After reading each fact, participants were given the choice to read more facts about the current topic or return to the selection menu to learn about a new topic. We found that with increasing age, participants chose to explore more facts within a topic (i.e., increased specific information seeking) and switched less frequently to new topics (i.e., decreased diversive information seeking). These results indicate that while young people seek out a broader range of information, as people grow older, they develop a preference to deepen their existing knowledge. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adult age differences in language, communication, and learning from text. 成人在语言、交流和从文本中学习方面的年龄差异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000819
Lise Abrams, Elizabeth A L Stine-Morrow

This is an introduction to the special issue "Adult Age Differences in Language, Communication, and Learning from Text." These articles illustrate the great variety of language use through the adult lifespan, tell us a little more-and invite further inquiry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

这是特刊 "成人在语言、交流和文本学习方面的年龄差异 "的导言。这些文章展示了成人一生中语言使用的巨大差异,告诉我们更多的信息,并邀请我们进一步探究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Adult age differences in language, communication, and learning from text.","authors":"Lise Abrams, Elizabeth A L Stine-Morrow","doi":"10.1037/pag0000819","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000819","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This is an introduction to the special issue \"Adult Age Differences in Language, Communication, and Learning from Text.\" These articles illustrate the great variety of language use through the adult lifespan, tell us a little more-and invite further inquiry. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age effects on prosodic boundary perception. 年龄对前音边界感知的影响
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000811
Jinxin Ji, Xinxian Zhao, Yang Li, Xiaohu Yang

The redundancy hypothesis proposes that older listeners need a larger array of acoustic cues than younger listeners for effective speech perception. This research investigated this hypothesis by examining the aging effects on the use of prosodic cues in speech segmentation in Mandarin Chinese. We examined how younger and older listeners perceived prosodic boundaries using three main prosodic cues (pause, final lengthening, and pitch change) across eight conditions involving different cue combinations. The stimuli consisted of syntactically ambiguous phrase pairs, each containing two or three objects. Participants (22 younger listeners and 22 older listeners) performed a speech recognition task to judge the number of objects they heard. Both groups primarily relied on the pause cue for identifying prosodic boundaries, using final lengthening and pitch change as secondary cues. However, older listeners showed reduced sensitivity to these cues, compensating by integrating the primary cue pause with the secondary cue pitch change for more precise segmentation. The present study reveals older listeners' integration strategy in using prosodic cues for speech segmentation, supporting the redundancy hypothesis. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

冗余假说认为,老年听者需要比年轻听者更多的声学线索才能有效地感知语音。本研究通过考察汉语普通话语音分段中使用前音线索的老龄化效应来研究这一假说。我们研究了年轻听者和年长听者如何使用三种主要的前音线索(停顿、末尾延长和音高变化)在涉及不同线索组合的八种条件下感知前音边界。刺激由句法模糊的短语对组成,每个短语对包含两个或三个对象。受试者(22 名年轻听者和 22 名年长听者)通过语音识别任务来判断他们听到的对象数量。两组听者都主要依靠停顿线索来识别前音边界,并将末尾延长和音高变化作为辅助线索。不过,老年听者对这些线索的敏感度有所降低,他们会将主要线索停顿与次要线索音高变化结合起来,以进行更精确的分段。本研究揭示了老年听者在使用前音线索进行语音分段时的整合策略,支持了冗余假说。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age effects on prosodic boundary perception.","authors":"Jinxin Ji, Xinxian Zhao, Yang Li, Xiaohu Yang","doi":"10.1037/pag0000811","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000811","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The redundancy hypothesis proposes that older listeners need a larger array of acoustic cues than younger listeners for effective speech perception. This research investigated this hypothesis by examining the aging effects on the use of prosodic cues in speech segmentation in Mandarin Chinese. We examined how younger and older listeners perceived prosodic boundaries using three main prosodic cues (pause, final lengthening, and pitch change) across eight conditions involving different cue combinations. The stimuli consisted of syntactically ambiguous phrase pairs, each containing two or three objects. Participants (22 younger listeners and 22 older listeners) performed a speech recognition task to judge the number of objects they heard. Both groups primarily relied on the pause cue for identifying prosodic boundaries, using final lengthening and pitch change as secondary cues. However, older listeners showed reduced sensitivity to these cues, compensating by integrating the primary cue pause with the secondary cue pitch change for more precise segmentation. The present study reveals older listeners' integration strategy in using prosodic cues for speech segmentation, supporting the redundancy hypothesis. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do pictures' emotional valence and arousal affect younger and older adults' narratives? 图片的情感价位和唤醒程度会影响年轻人和老年人的叙述吗?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000808
Lise Abrams, Benjamin P Cote, María José Najas, Aysha H Gsibat, Katherine K White

Emotional content, specifically negative valence, can differentially influence speech production in younger and older adults' autobiographical narratives, which have been interpreted as reflecting age differences in emotion regulation. However, age differences in emotional reactivity are another possible explanation, as younger and older adults frequently differ in their affective responses to negative and positive pictures. The present experiment investigated whether a picture's valence (pleasantness) and arousal (intensity) influenced older adults' production of narratives about those pictures. Thirty younger and 30 older participants produced narratives about pictures that varied in valence (positive, negative, and neutral) and arousal (high, low). Narratives were recorded via Zoom, transcribed, and analyzed with Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count-22 to get measures of emotional word use, disfluencies, and linguistic distance. Results showed that negative valence increased age differences in speech production independent of picture arousal: Relative to younger adults, older adults used more positive words, fewer negative words, and had more silent pauses when telling narratives about negative pictures. In contrast, high arousal decreased age differences such that older adults used fewer positive words in narratives about positive pictures and more linguistically distant words evidenced by fewer present-tense verbs, relative to narratives about low-arousal pictures. Contrary to an explanation of enhanced regulation or control over emotions in older adulthood, these findings support the idea that older adults' speech production is influenced by their reactivity or affective response to emotional stimuli even when the task is not to communicate one's emotions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

情绪内容,特别是负面情绪,会对年轻人和老年人自传体叙述中的语音产生不同的影响,这被解释为反映了情绪调节方面的年龄差异。然而,情绪反应能力的年龄差异也是另一种可能的解释,因为年轻人和老年人对负面和正面图片的情绪反应经常不同。本实验研究了图片的情价(愉快度)和唤醒(强度)是否会影响老年人对这些图片的叙述。分别有 30 名年轻人和 30 名老年人对不同情绪(积极、消极和中性)和唤醒(高、低)的图片进行了叙述。通过 Zoom 对叙述进行录制、转录,并使用 Linguistic Inquiry 和 Word Count-22 进行分析,以获得情感用词、语句不通顺和语言距离的测量结果。结果表明,负情态增加了语言表达的年龄差异,而与图像唤醒无关:与年轻人相比,老年人在讲述负面图片时使用更多的正面词语、更少的负面词语和更多的无声停顿。与此相反,高唤醒度缩小了年龄差异,因此相对于低唤醒度图片的叙述,老年人在叙述积极图片时使用的积极词汇更少,而语言上的疏远词汇则更多,表现为使用的现在时动词更少。与老年人对情绪的调节或控制能力增强的解释相反,这些研究结果支持这样一种观点,即老年人的语言表达会受到他们对情绪刺激的反应性或情感反应的影响,即使他们的任务不是传达自己的情绪。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Do pictures' emotional valence and arousal affect younger and older adults' narratives?","authors":"Lise Abrams, Benjamin P Cote, María José Najas, Aysha H Gsibat, Katherine K White","doi":"10.1037/pag0000808","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000808","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotional content, specifically negative valence, can differentially influence speech production in younger and older adults' autobiographical narratives, which have been interpreted as reflecting age differences in emotion regulation. However, age differences in emotional reactivity are another possible explanation, as younger and older adults frequently differ in their affective responses to negative and positive pictures. The present experiment investigated whether a picture's valence (pleasantness) and arousal (intensity) influenced older adults' production of narratives about those pictures. Thirty younger and 30 older participants produced narratives about pictures that varied in valence (positive, negative, and neutral) and arousal (high, low). Narratives were recorded via Zoom, transcribed, and analyzed with Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count-22 to get measures of emotional word use, disfluencies, and linguistic distance. Results showed that negative valence increased age differences in speech production independent of picture arousal: Relative to younger adults, older adults used more positive words, fewer negative words, and had more silent pauses when telling narratives about negative pictures. In contrast, high arousal decreased age differences such that older adults used fewer positive words in narratives about positive pictures and more linguistically distant words evidenced by fewer present-tense verbs, relative to narratives about low-arousal pictures. Contrary to an explanation of enhanced regulation or control over emotions in older adulthood, these findings support the idea that older adults' speech production is influenced by their reactivity or affective response to emotional stimuli even when the task is not to communicate one's emotions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201145","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-related differences in delay discounting: Income matters. 延迟贴现中与年龄有关的差异:收入问题。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000818
Haoran Wan, J. Myerson, Leonard Green, Michael J Strube, S. Hale
Although the authors of a recent meta-analysis concluded there were no age-related differences in the discounting of delayed rewards, they did not examine the effects of income (Seaman et al., 2022). Accordingly, the present study compared discounting by younger and older adults (Ages 35-50 and 65-80) differing in household income. Two procedures were used: the Monetary Choice Questionnaire and the Adjusting-Amount procedure. Whereas no age difference was observed between the higher income (> $80,000) age groups, a significant difference was observed between younger and older adults with lower incomes (< $50,000): The younger adults discounted more steeply than the older adults. These findings, which were observed with both discounting procedures, support our buffering hypothesis, which assumes that the scarcity associated with a lower income is a stressor that can lead to steeper discounting, but that age-related increases in emotional stability can buffer such stressors, leading to age-related differences between lower income age groups. In contrast, no age difference was observed in higher income adults who experience much less scarcity. Further support for the buffering hypothesis comes from the finding that there was no age-related difference in discounting by the lower income groups when distress was statistically controlled. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
尽管最近一项荟萃分析的作者得出结论,延迟奖励的折现与年龄无关,但他们没有研究收入的影响(Seaman 等人,2022 年)。因此,本研究比较了不同家庭收入的年轻人和老年人(年龄分别为 35-50 岁和 65-80 岁)的贴现率。研究采用了两种程序:货币选择问卷和调整金额程序。虽然在收入较高(> 80,000 美元)的年龄组之间没有观察到年龄差异,但在收入较低(< 50,000 美元)的年轻人和老年人之间观察到了显著差异:年轻人比老年人的折扣幅度更大。这些结果在两种贴现程序中都能观察到,支持了我们的缓冲假说,即与较低收入相关的稀缺性是一种压力源,可导致较陡的贴现,但与年龄相关的情绪稳定性的增加可缓冲这种压力源,从而导致较低收入年龄组之间与年龄相关的差异。与此相反,在收入较高的成年人中没有观察到年龄差异,因为他们经历的稀缺性要少得多。进一步支持缓冲假说的研究发现,在对压力进行统计控制后,较低收入群体在折现方面没有与年龄相关的差异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age-related differences in delay discounting: Income matters.","authors":"Haoran Wan, J. Myerson, Leonard Green, Michael J Strube, S. Hale","doi":"10.1037/pag0000818","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000818","url":null,"abstract":"Although the authors of a recent meta-analysis concluded there were no age-related differences in the discounting of delayed rewards, they did not examine the effects of income (Seaman et al., 2022). Accordingly, the present study compared discounting by younger and older adults (Ages 35-50 and 65-80) differing in household income. Two procedures were used: the Monetary Choice Questionnaire and the Adjusting-Amount procedure. Whereas no age difference was observed between the higher income (> $80,000) age groups, a significant difference was observed between younger and older adults with lower incomes (< $50,000): The younger adults discounted more steeply than the older adults. These findings, which were observed with both discounting procedures, support our buffering hypothesis, which assumes that the scarcity associated with a lower income is a stressor that can lead to steeper discounting, but that age-related increases in emotional stability can buffer such stressors, leading to age-related differences between lower income age groups. In contrast, no age difference was observed in higher income adults who experience much less scarcity. Further support for the buffering hypothesis comes from the finding that there was no age-related difference in discounting by the lower income groups when distress was statistically controlled. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140677298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Postponing old age: Evidence for historical change toward a later perceived onset of old age. 推迟进入老年:历史变迁的证据表明,人们开始意识到自己进入老年的时间越来越晚。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000812
Markus Wettstein, Rinseo Park, A. Kornadt, Susanne Wurm, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf
"At what age would you describe someone as old?" Perceptions of when old age begins might be prone to upward shifts because of historical increases in life expectancy and in retirement age, as well as because of better psychosocial functioning in later life. We investigated historical changes in within-person trajectories of the perceived onset of old age using data from 14,056 participants who entered the German Ageing Survey at age 40-85 years and who completed up to eight assessments across 25 years. Using longitudinal multilevel regression models, we found that at age 64, the average perceived onset of old age is at about age 75 years. Longitudinally, this perceived onset age increased by about 1 year for every 4-5 years of actual aging. We also found evidence for historical change. Compared to the earliest-born cohorts, later-born cohorts reported a later perceived onset of old age, yet with decelerating trend among more recent birth cohorts. Within-person increases of the perceived onset of old age were steeper in later-born cohorts. The described cohort trends were only slightly reduced when controlling for covariates. Being younger, male, living in East Germany, feeling older, reporting more loneliness, more chronic diseases, and poorer self-rated health were each associated with a perceived earlier onset of old age. Our results suggest that there is a nonlinear historical trend toward a later perceived onset of old age, which might have meaningful implications for individuals' perspectives on aging and old age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
"您认为什么年龄的人老呢?由于历史上预期寿命和退休年龄的增加,以及晚年生活中更好的社会心理功能,人们对老年开始时间的看法可能容易发生上移。我们利用 14,056 名参加德国老龄化调查(German Ageing Survey)、年龄在 40-85 岁之间、并在 25 年间完成了多达八次评估的参与者的数据,研究了感知到的老年开始时间的历史变化轨迹。通过纵向多层次回归模型,我们发现在 64 岁时,平均感知到的老年发病年龄约为 75 岁。纵向来看,实际老龄化每增加 4-5 年,感知到的开始老龄化年龄就会增加约 1 岁。我们还发现了历史变化的证据。与最早出生的人群相比,较晚出生的人群报告的老龄感知起始年龄较晚,但在较近出生的人群中,老龄感知起始年龄呈下降趋势。在较晚出生的队列中,感知到的老年期开始时间在个人内部的增长速度更快。在控制协变量后,上述队列趋势仅略有减弱。年龄越小、男性、居住在东德、感觉年龄越大、孤独感越强、慢性病越多、自我评价健康状况越差,这些因素都与老年期提前到来有关。我们的研究结果表明,存在着一种非线性的历史趋势,即认为自己进入老年的时间较晚,这可能会对个人对衰老和老年的看法产生有意义的影响。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Postponing old age: Evidence for historical change toward a later perceived onset of old age.","authors":"Markus Wettstein, Rinseo Park, A. Kornadt, Susanne Wurm, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf","doi":"10.1037/pag0000812","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000812","url":null,"abstract":"\"At what age would you describe someone as old?\" Perceptions of when old age begins might be prone to upward shifts because of historical increases in life expectancy and in retirement age, as well as because of better psychosocial functioning in later life. We investigated historical changes in within-person trajectories of the perceived onset of old age using data from 14,056 participants who entered the German Ageing Survey at age 40-85 years and who completed up to eight assessments across 25 years. Using longitudinal multilevel regression models, we found that at age 64, the average perceived onset of old age is at about age 75 years. Longitudinally, this perceived onset age increased by about 1 year for every 4-5 years of actual aging. We also found evidence for historical change. Compared to the earliest-born cohorts, later-born cohorts reported a later perceived onset of old age, yet with decelerating trend among more recent birth cohorts. Within-person increases of the perceived onset of old age were steeper in later-born cohorts. The described cohort trends were only slightly reduced when controlling for covariates. Being younger, male, living in East Germany, feeling older, reporting more loneliness, more chronic diseases, and poorer self-rated health were each associated with a perceived earlier onset of old age. Our results suggest that there is a nonlinear historical trend toward a later perceived onset of old age, which might have meaningful implications for individuals' perspectives on aging and old age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140677449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of social interaction modality for well-being in older adults. 社会交往模式对老年人幸福感的作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000816
Carlotta E Grünjes, Birthe Macdonald, Gizem Hülür
It is well-established that more frequent social interaction is associated with higher well-being across the lifespan. The present study examines the role of frequency of interactions via different modalities on older adults' weekly well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic, where people had to adapt their communication behavior and reduce in-person contact due to precautionary measures. We use data from 98 participants (age: M = 71, SD = 5), who documented their weekly frequency of communication via four interaction modalities as well as their loneliness, positive affect, and negative affect over up to 64 weeks. Results show that participants with overall higher frequency of face-to-face, telephone, and text-based interaction than others report higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative affect and loneliness than others. Participants report higher levels of well-being during weeks when they report more frequent face-to-face, telephone, and text-based interaction than their individual average. Unexpectedly, participants report higher levels of negative affect during weeks with more video call interaction. Some effects of social interaction frequency on affect and loneliness are higher for face-to-face interactions versus other modalities. In addition, interaction effects at within-person level indicate that the effects of weekly telephone and text-based interaction frequency on loneliness are stronger in weeks with relatively few face-to-face interactions. Taken together, our findings suggest that social interactions via different modalities contribute to well-being, but that face-to-face interactions have the biggest effect. In addition, there is some evidence that telephone and text-based interaction may play a compensatory role. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在人的一生中,更频繁的社会交往与更高的幸福感息息相关,这一点已得到公认。本研究探讨了在 COVID-19 大流行期间,通过不同方式进行交流的频率对老年人每周幸福感的影响。我们使用了 98 名参与者(年龄:M = 71,SD = 5)的数据,他们记录了每周通过四种互动方式进行交流的频率,以及长达 64 周的孤独感、积极情绪和消极情绪。结果显示,与其他人相比,面对面交流、电话交流和短信交流频率较高的参与者的积极情绪水平较高,而消极情绪和孤独感水平较低。在面对面交流、电话交流和短信交流频率高于个人平均水平的几周内,参与者的幸福感水平较高。出乎意料的是,在视频通话互动较多的几周内,参与者的负面情绪水平较高。社交互动频率对情感和孤独感的某些影响在面对面互动时要高于其他方式。此外,人与人之间的互动效应表明,在面对面互动相对较少的几周内,每周电话和短信互动频率对孤独感的影响更大。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,通过不同方式进行的社交互动有助于提高幸福感,但面对面的互动影响最大。此外,还有一些证据表明,电话和基于文本的互动可能起到补偿作用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of social interaction modality for well-being in older adults.","authors":"Carlotta E Grünjes, Birthe Macdonald, Gizem Hülür","doi":"10.1037/pag0000816","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000816","url":null,"abstract":"It is well-established that more frequent social interaction is associated with higher well-being across the lifespan. The present study examines the role of frequency of interactions via different modalities on older adults' weekly well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic, where people had to adapt their communication behavior and reduce in-person contact due to precautionary measures. We use data from 98 participants (age: M = 71, SD = 5), who documented their weekly frequency of communication via four interaction modalities as well as their loneliness, positive affect, and negative affect over up to 64 weeks. Results show that participants with overall higher frequency of face-to-face, telephone, and text-based interaction than others report higher levels of positive affect and lower levels of negative affect and loneliness than others. Participants report higher levels of well-being during weeks when they report more frequent face-to-face, telephone, and text-based interaction than their individual average. Unexpectedly, participants report higher levels of negative affect during weeks with more video call interaction. Some effects of social interaction frequency on affect and loneliness are higher for face-to-face interactions versus other modalities. In addition, interaction effects at within-person level indicate that the effects of weekly telephone and text-based interaction frequency on loneliness are stronger in weeks with relatively few face-to-face interactions. Taken together, our findings suggest that social interactions via different modalities contribute to well-being, but that face-to-face interactions have the biggest effect. In addition, there is some evidence that telephone and text-based interaction may play a compensatory role. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140688956","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nonepisodic autobiographical memory details reflect attempts to tell a good story. 非记忆性自传体记忆的细节反映了讲述一个好故事的尝试。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000805
A. Mair, Marie Poirier, Martin A Conway
A persistent finding in the autobiographical memory (AM) literature is that older adults report more nonepisodic (or generalized/semantic) information than young adults. Since studies are usually focused on memory for episodic (or specific) autobiographical events, the reason for the age difference in nonepisodic AM remains understudied. This experiment investigated whether the higher rate of nonepisodic AM in older adults reflects (a) a difference incommunicative preferences or (b) cognitive decline, by way of either an inhibition deficit or as a means of compensating for a deficit in episodic AM. A sample of 54 young (N = 28, age range = 18-46) and older (N = 26, age range = 62-86) participants retrieved the same AM twice, under two different sets of instructions: to tell a good story for their autobiography, or to provide a detailed police witness statement. Both groups reported more general details when they were aiming to tell a good story. In addition, older adults also reported fewer specific details when the aim was to tell a good story. In a separate ranking task, young and older adults differed in their perceptions of what makes a good story; young adults ranked "detail," "grammar," and "full descriptions" more highly than older adults, whereas older ranked "linking ideas" and "explaining not just describing" more highly than young adults. The results suggest that age-related differences in nonepisodic AM might be explained by communicative preferences rather than cognitive decline. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
自传体记忆(AM)文献中的一个长期发现是,老年人比年轻人报告了更多的非表象(或概括/语义)信息。由于研究通常集中于对情节性(或特定)自传事件的记忆,因此对非情节性自传记忆的年龄差异的原因仍未进行深入研究。本实验研究了老年人较高的非情节性自传记忆率是否反映了(a)非交流偏好的差异或(b)认知能力的衰退,即抑制能力的不足或作为对情节性自传记忆不足的一种补偿手段。54 名年轻(28 人,年龄在 18-46 岁之间)和年长(26 人,年龄在 62-86 岁之间)的参与者在两套不同的指令下两次检索了相同的 AM:为自传讲一个好故事,或提供一份详细的警方证人证词。两组受试者在以讲一个好故事为目标时,都报告了更多的一般细节。此外,当目标是讲一个好故事时,老年人报告的具体细节也较少。在一个单独的排序任务中,年轻人和老年人对什么是好故事的看法不同;年轻人对 "细节"、"语法 "和 "全面描述 "的评价比老年人高,而老年人对 "连接观点 "和 "解释而不仅仅是描述 "的评价比年轻人高。研究结果表明,与年龄相关的非记忆性调幅差异可能是由交流偏好而非认知能力下降造成的。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Nonepisodic autobiographical memory details reflect attempts to tell a good story.","authors":"A. Mair, Marie Poirier, Martin A Conway","doi":"10.1037/pag0000805","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000805","url":null,"abstract":"A persistent finding in the autobiographical memory (AM) literature is that older adults report more nonepisodic (or generalized/semantic) information than young adults. Since studies are usually focused on memory for episodic (or specific) autobiographical events, the reason for the age difference in nonepisodic AM remains understudied. This experiment investigated whether the higher rate of nonepisodic AM in older adults reflects (a) a difference incommunicative preferences or (b) cognitive decline, by way of either an inhibition deficit or as a means of compensating for a deficit in episodic AM. A sample of 54 young (N = 28, age range = 18-46) and older (N = 26, age range = 62-86) participants retrieved the same AM twice, under two different sets of instructions: to tell a good story for their autobiography, or to provide a detailed police witness statement. Both groups reported more general details when they were aiming to tell a good story. In addition, older adults also reported fewer specific details when the aim was to tell a good story. In a separate ranking task, young and older adults differed in their perceptions of what makes a good story; young adults ranked \"detail,\" \"grammar,\" and \"full descriptions\" more highly than older adults, whereas older ranked \"linking ideas\" and \"explaining not just describing\" more highly than young adults. The results suggest that age-related differences in nonepisodic AM might be explained by communicative preferences rather than cognitive decline. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140686938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychology and Aging
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1