首页 > 最新文献

Psychology and Aging最新文献

英文 中文
Loneliness, epigenetic age acceleration, and chronic health conditions. 孤独、表观遗传年龄加速和慢性健康问题。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000822
Colin D. Freilich, K. Markon, Steve W. Cole, Robert F. Krueger
Having associations with a range of adverse physical health outcomes including mortality, loneliness is increasingly recognized as a pressing public health concern, but the mechanisms studied to date do not yet explain all loneliness-related health risk. We sought to evaluate whether epigenetic influences on DNA methylation could help explain the relationship between loneliness and health. To do so, we first estimated associations between loneliness and epigenetic age acceleration (EAA) in a subsample of participants in the study of midlife in the United States (n = 1,310), before testing whether EAA mediated and/or moderated the association between loneliness and the onset of chronic health conditions in older adulthood (n = 445 completing longitudinal follow-ups). Greater loneliness was weakly associated with greater EAA in the Horvath, DunedinPACE, and GrimAge measures after accounting for demographic (0.08 ≤ β ≤ 0.11) and behavioral (0.06 ≤ β ≤ 0.08) covariates. Loneliness also predicted increases in chronic condition counts and these effects were more pronounced for individuals with higher DunedinPACE EAA values (interaction term β = 0.09, p = .009), suggesting possible synergistic impacts. EAA measures appear to be promising in helping to understand individual variations in the health impacts of loneliness, but the specific mechanisms involved require further research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
孤独与包括死亡率在内的一系列不良身体健康结果有关,因此越来越多的人认为孤独是一个紧迫的公共健康问题,但迄今为止所研究的机制还不能解释所有与孤独有关的健康风险。我们试图评估对DNA甲基化的表观遗传影响是否有助于解释孤独与健康之间的关系。为此,我们首先估算了美国中年研究参与者子样本(n = 1,310)中孤独感与表观遗传年龄加速(EAA)之间的关联,然后测试了EAA是否介导和/或调节了孤独感与老年期慢性病发病之间的关联(n = 445,完成纵向随访)。在考虑了人口统计学(0.08 ≤ β ≤ 0.11)和行为学(0.06 ≤ β ≤ 0.08)协变量后,在 Horvath、DunedinPACE 和 GrimAge 测量中,更强的孤独感与更强的 EAA 有微弱的相关性。孤独感也预示着慢性病数量的增加,这些影响对于达尼丁PACE EAA值较高的个体更为明显(交互项 β = 0.09,p = .009),表明可能存在协同影响。EAA测量似乎有望帮助了解孤独对健康影响的个体差异,但其中涉及的具体机制还需要进一步研究。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Loneliness, epigenetic age acceleration, and chronic health conditions.","authors":"Colin D. Freilich, K. Markon, Steve W. Cole, Robert F. Krueger","doi":"10.1037/pag0000822","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000822","url":null,"abstract":"Having associations with a range of adverse physical health outcomes including mortality, loneliness is increasingly recognized as a pressing public health concern, but the mechanisms studied to date do not yet explain all loneliness-related health risk. We sought to evaluate whether epigenetic influences on DNA methylation could help explain the relationship between loneliness and health. To do so, we first estimated associations between loneliness and epigenetic age acceleration (EAA) in a subsample of participants in the study of midlife in the United States (n = 1,310), before testing whether EAA mediated and/or moderated the association between loneliness and the onset of chronic health conditions in older adulthood (n = 445 completing longitudinal follow-ups). Greater loneliness was weakly associated with greater EAA in the Horvath, DunedinPACE, and GrimAge measures after accounting for demographic (0.08 ≤ β ≤ 0.11) and behavioral (0.06 ≤ β ≤ 0.08) covariates. Loneliness also predicted increases in chronic condition counts and these effects were more pronounced for individuals with higher DunedinPACE EAA values (interaction term β = 0.09, p = .009), suggesting possible synergistic impacts. EAA measures appear to be promising in helping to understand individual variations in the health impacts of loneliness, but the specific mechanisms involved require further research. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140686458","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging predicts changes in mental health: Evidence from a 10-year longitudinal study. 认知老化本质论信念的变化可预测心理健康的变化:一项为期 10 年的纵向研究提供的证据。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000823
Gabriele Prati
The belief that aging-related cognitive decline is inevitable is associated with impaired cognitive performance of older adults. Little is, however, known about the association between changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging and mental health in the long term and among both younger and older adults. From a theoretical perspective, it would be expected that changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging predict changes in mental health among older adults compared to younger adults. These differential associations have not yet been prospectively investigated. Using longitudinal data, the association between changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging and mental health outcomes and the moderation effect of age were assessed. Mental health includes well-being and mental illness (depression, anxiety, and panic symptoms). Data from adult participants (n = 4,963 with a mean age of 55.41 years, range 28-84 years) in a U.S. national longitudinal study were analyzed. A series of two-wave latent change score models indicated that changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging are associated with changes in well-being and mental illness. Age did not moderate the relationship between changes in essentialist beliefs about aging and changes in mental health (i.e., mental illness and well-being). The present study highlights change-to-change dynamics between essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging and mental health, regardless of age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
认为与衰老相关的认知能力衰退不可避免与老年人认知能力受损有关。然而,对于认知老化本质论信念的变化与心理健康之间的长期联系,以及年轻人和老年人之间的联系,人们知之甚少。从理论角度来看,与年轻人相比,认知老化的本质主义信念的变化会预测老年人心理健康的变化。这些不同的关联尚未得到前瞻性研究。本研究利用纵向数据,评估了认知老化的本质主义信念变化与心理健康结果之间的关联以及年龄的调节作用。心理健康包括幸福感和精神疾病(抑郁、焦虑和恐慌症状)。研究分析了一项美国国家纵向研究的成年参与者(n = 4,963 人,平均年龄为 55.41 岁,年龄范围为 28-84 岁)的数据。一系列两波潜在变化得分模型表明,认知老化本质主义信念的变化与幸福感和精神疾病的变化有关。年龄并没有缓和有关衰老的本质主义信念的变化与心理健康变化(即精神疾病和幸福感)之间的关系。本研究强调了认知老化的本质主义信念与心理健康之间的动态变化,与年龄无关。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging predicts changes in mental health: Evidence from a 10-year longitudinal study.","authors":"Gabriele Prati","doi":"10.1037/pag0000823","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000823","url":null,"abstract":"The belief that aging-related cognitive decline is inevitable is associated with impaired cognitive performance of older adults. Little is, however, known about the association between changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging and mental health in the long term and among both younger and older adults. From a theoretical perspective, it would be expected that changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging predict changes in mental health among older adults compared to younger adults. These differential associations have not yet been prospectively investigated. Using longitudinal data, the association between changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging and mental health outcomes and the moderation effect of age were assessed. Mental health includes well-being and mental illness (depression, anxiety, and panic symptoms). Data from adult participants (n = 4,963 with a mean age of 55.41 years, range 28-84 years) in a U.S. national longitudinal study were analyzed. A series of two-wave latent change score models indicated that changes in essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging are associated with changes in well-being and mental illness. Age did not moderate the relationship between changes in essentialist beliefs about aging and changes in mental health (i.e., mental illness and well-being). The present study highlights change-to-change dynamics between essentialist beliefs about cognitive aging and mental health, regardless of age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140686699","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Loneliness, Epigenetic Age Acceleration, and Chronic Health Conditions 孤独、表观遗传年龄加速和慢性健康状况》补充材料
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000822.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Loneliness, Epigenetic Age Acceleration, and Chronic Health Conditions","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pag0000822.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000822.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140702748","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Efficient word segmentation is preserved in older adult readers: Evidence from eye movements during Chinese reading. 老年读者仍能保持高效的分词能力:中文阅读时眼球运动的证据
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000813
Lin Li, Lingshan Bao, Zhuoer Li, Sha Li, Jingyi Liu, Pin Wang, Kayleigh L. Warrington, Sarah C. Gunn, K. Paterson
College-aged readers use efficient strategies to segment and recognize words in naturally unspaced Chinese text. Whether this capability changes across the adult lifespan is unknown, although segmenting words in unspaced text may be challenging for older readers due to visual and cognitive declines in older age, including poorer parafoveal processing of upcoming characters. Accordingly, we conducted two eye movement experiments to test for age differences in word segmentation, each with 48 young (18-30 years) and 36 older (65+ years) native Chinese readers. Following Zhou and Li (2021), we focused on the processing of "incremental" three-character words, like (meaning "kindergartens"), which contain an embedded two-character word (e.g., , meaning "children"). In Experiment 1, either the three-character word or its embedded word was presented as the target word in sentence contexts where the three-character word always was plausible, and the embedded word was either plausible or implausible. Both age groups produced similar plausibility effects, suggesting age constancy in accessing the embedded word early during ambiguity processing before ultimately assigning an incremental word analysis. Experiment 2 provided further evidence that both younger and older readers access the embedded word early during ambiguity processing, but rapidly select the appropriate (incremental) word. Crucially, the findings suggest that word segmentation strategies do not differ with age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
大学年龄段的读者使用高效的策略来分割和识别自然无间隔中文文本中的单词。这种能力是否会在成年人的一生中发生变化尚不清楚,不过,由于老年人的视觉和认知能力下降,包括对即将出现的字符的视网膜旁处理能力较差,在无间隔文本中分词对老年读者来说可能具有挑战性。因此,我们进行了两项眼动实验,分别测试 48 名年轻(18-30 岁)和 36 名老年(65 岁以上)母语为中文的读者在分词方面的年龄差异。按照周和李的研究(2021 年),我们重点研究了对 "递增 "三字词的处理,如(意为 "幼儿园"),其中包含一个嵌入的两字词(如 ,意为 "孩子")。在实验 1 中,三字词或其嵌入词在句子语境中作为目标词出现,其中三字词总是可信的,而嵌入词要么可信要么不可信。两个年龄组都产生了相似的可信度效应,这表明在模糊处理过程中,在最终进行增量词分析之前,早期进入嵌入词的年龄是一致的。实验 2 提供了进一步的证据,证明年龄较小和年龄较大的读者都会在模棱两可处理过程中及早接触嵌入词,但很快就会选择适当的(增量)词。最重要的是,实验结果表明,单词分割策略并不因年龄而异。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Efficient word segmentation is preserved in older adult readers: Evidence from eye movements during Chinese reading.","authors":"Lin Li, Lingshan Bao, Zhuoer Li, Sha Li, Jingyi Liu, Pin Wang, Kayleigh L. Warrington, Sarah C. Gunn, K. Paterson","doi":"10.1037/pag0000813","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000813","url":null,"abstract":"College-aged readers use efficient strategies to segment and recognize words in naturally unspaced Chinese text. Whether this capability changes across the adult lifespan is unknown, although segmenting words in unspaced text may be challenging for older readers due to visual and cognitive declines in older age, including poorer parafoveal processing of upcoming characters. Accordingly, we conducted two eye movement experiments to test for age differences in word segmentation, each with 48 young (18-30 years) and 36 older (65+ years) native Chinese readers. Following Zhou and Li (2021), we focused on the processing of \"incremental\" three-character words, like (meaning \"kindergartens\"), which contain an embedded two-character word (e.g., , meaning \"children\"). In Experiment 1, either the three-character word or its embedded word was presented as the target word in sentence contexts where the three-character word always was plausible, and the embedded word was either plausible or implausible. Both age groups produced similar plausibility effects, suggesting age constancy in accessing the embedded word early during ambiguity processing before ultimately assigning an incremental word analysis. Experiment 2 provided further evidence that both younger and older readers access the embedded word early during ambiguity processing, but rapidly select the appropriate (incremental) word. Crucially, the findings suggest that word segmentation strategies do not differ with age. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140713398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Facing off-time mortality: Leaving a legacy. 面对非正常死亡:留下遗产
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000815
Mary Kate Koch, Susan Bluck, Sophia Maggiore, Harvey Max Chochinov, Kiana Cogdill-Richardson, Carma Bylund
Considering one's legacy is usual in later life but may be accentuated after receiving a serious and terminal cancer diagnosis. This may be particularly true when timing of the diagnosis is nonnormatively early, evoking the sense of losing future years of life. Acknowledging the severity of one's illness may also promote focus on legacy. We investigated the extent to which older individuals diagnosed with cancer narrated communion (i.e., loving, caring themes) when telling their legacy, including narration of aftermath concerns (i.e., concern for how others will fare after one's death). Communion was assessed in relation to individuals' potential years of life to lose and illness acknowledgment. Participants were a national sample of adults (N = 203; M = 65.80 years; 66% women; 77.94% White; 48.53% college-educated) with serious and terminal cancer receiving outpatient palliative care. They narrated legacies in semistructured interviews and completed measures of illness acknowledgment. We developed a novel construct, potential years of life to lose, calculated as the difference between chronological age and national life expectancy at birth. Coders, trained to high reliability, content-analyzed legacy narratives for communion with follow-up coding for aftermath concerns. Hierarchical regression indicated that for those with more potential years of life to lose, acknowledging the severity of their illness was critical to narrating communion-rich legacies. Similarly, aftermath concerns were common in those with the most years of life to lose who were able to acknowledge the severity of their illness. Findings affirm the psychological richness of individuals' legacies in the second half of life and highlight one way they adaptively respond to the nonnormative timing of serious and terminal cancer. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
在晚年生活中,考虑自己的遗产是很平常的事,但在被诊断出患有严重的晚期癌症后,考虑遗产的问题可能会更加突出。当确诊时间非正常提前时,这种情况可能尤为明显,让人感觉失去了未来的生命岁月。承认自己病情的严重性也会促进人们对遗产的关注。我们调查了被诊断出患有癌症的老年人在讲述自己的遗产时,包括讲述对后事的关注(即关注自己死后他人的生活状况)时,在多大程度上叙述了共融(即爱与关怀的主题)。对 "共融 "的评估与个人可能失去的生命年限和对疾病的认识有关。参与者是全国范围内接受门诊姑息治疗的重症晚期癌症成人样本(N = 203;M = 65.80 岁;66% 为女性;77.94% 为白人;48.53% 接受过大学教育)。他们在半结构化访谈中讲述了遗产,并完成了疾病认知测量。我们提出了一个新的概念,即 "可能失去的生命年数",计算方法是出生时的实际年龄与国家预期寿命之间的差值。经过高可靠性培训的编码员对遗产叙述的内容进行了分析,以便与后续的后遗症问题编码保持一致。层次回归结果表明,对于那些可能失去更多寿命的人来说,承认自己疾病的严重性对于叙述富有共融性的遗产至关重要。同样,对于那些能够承认自己疾病严重性的人来说,在他们失去生命的年限最多的时候,对后事的关注也很普遍。研究结果肯定了个人在后半生留下的丰富心理遗产,并强调了他们对重症和晚期癌症的非正常时间做出适应性反应的一种方式。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Facing off-time mortality: Leaving a legacy.","authors":"Mary Kate Koch, Susan Bluck, Sophia Maggiore, Harvey Max Chochinov, Kiana Cogdill-Richardson, Carma Bylund","doi":"10.1037/pag0000815","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000815","url":null,"abstract":"Considering one's legacy is usual in later life but may be accentuated after receiving a serious and terminal cancer diagnosis. This may be particularly true when timing of the diagnosis is nonnormatively early, evoking the sense of losing future years of life. Acknowledging the severity of one's illness may also promote focus on legacy. We investigated the extent to which older individuals diagnosed with cancer narrated communion (i.e., loving, caring themes) when telling their legacy, including narration of aftermath concerns (i.e., concern for how others will fare after one's death). Communion was assessed in relation to individuals' potential years of life to lose and illness acknowledgment. Participants were a national sample of adults (N = 203; M = 65.80 years; 66% women; 77.94% White; 48.53% college-educated) with serious and terminal cancer receiving outpatient palliative care. They narrated legacies in semistructured interviews and completed measures of illness acknowledgment. We developed a novel construct, potential years of life to lose, calculated as the difference between chronological age and national life expectancy at birth. Coders, trained to high reliability, content-analyzed legacy narratives for communion with follow-up coding for aftermath concerns. Hierarchical regression indicated that for those with more potential years of life to lose, acknowledging the severity of their illness was critical to narrating communion-rich legacies. Similarly, aftermath concerns were common in those with the most years of life to lose who were able to acknowledge the severity of their illness. Findings affirm the psychological richness of individuals' legacies in the second half of life and highlight one way they adaptively respond to the nonnormative timing of serious and terminal cancer. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140715072","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for The Role of Social Interaction Modality for Well-Being in Older Adults 社会交往模式对老年人幸福感的作用》补充材料
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000816.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for The Role of Social Interaction Modality for Well-Being in Older Adults","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pag0000816.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000816.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140731550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality and 10-year personality development among Norwegians in midlife-Do retirement and job type play a role? 挪威中年人的个性和十年个性发展--退休和工作类型是否起作用?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000814
Georg Henning, G. K. Løset, Oliver Huxhold
Our personality develops over the whole lifespan and in particular when our life circumstances change. Retirement is a life event that brings changes in identity, day structures, and social roles of former workers. Therefore, it may affect personality traits such as the Big Five (neuroticism, extraversion, intellect, conscientiousness, and agreeableness). Previous studies have shown conflicting results concerning the question whether and how retirement is associated with changes in personality traits. Furthermore, there is little knowledge about the role of the job people leave behind when retiring. In the present study, we compared personality development over a 10-year period, based on two waves of a Norwegian survey, between retiring and continuously working blue-collar versus white-collar workers (n = 1,263, Mage = 56.58). Latent change score models showed that neuroticism and openness declined in the sample, but to a comparable degree in all groups. We further found differences in baseline personality traits between blue-collar workers and white-collar workers, as well as between those retiring and not retiring, implying selection into retirement by personality traits. Item level analyses showed declines in some items. We discuss theoretical and methodological implications of our results in light of previous ambiguous findings and emphasize the possible heterogeneity across retirees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
我们的人格是在整个生命周期中形成的,尤其是当我们的生活环境发生变化时。退休是一个人生大事,会带来身份、日间结构和前工作者社会角色的变化。因此,退休可能会影响人格特质,如五大人格(神经质、外向性、智力、自觉性和合群性)。关于退休是否以及如何与人格特质的变化相关联这一问题,以往的研究显示了相互矛盾的结果。此外,人们对退休后所从事工作的作用知之甚少。在本研究中,我们根据挪威的两波调查,比较了即将退休的蓝领工人和连续工作的白领工人(n = 1,263, Mage = 56.58)在10年期间的人格发展情况。潜在变化得分模型显示,样本中的神经质和开放性有所下降,但各组的下降程度相当。我们进一步发现,蓝领工人和白领工人之间,以及退休人员和非退休人员之间的基线人格特质存在差异,这意味着人格特质选择了退休。项目层面的分析表明,某些项目出现了下降。鉴于以前的研究结果模棱两可,我们讨论了研究结果的理论和方法论意义,并强调了退休人员之间可能存在的异质性。 (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved)。
{"title":"Personality and 10-year personality development among Norwegians in midlife-Do retirement and job type play a role?","authors":"Georg Henning, G. K. Løset, Oliver Huxhold","doi":"10.1037/pag0000814","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000814","url":null,"abstract":"Our personality develops over the whole lifespan and in particular when our life circumstances change. Retirement is a life event that brings changes in identity, day structures, and social roles of former workers. Therefore, it may affect personality traits such as the Big Five (neuroticism, extraversion, intellect, conscientiousness, and agreeableness). Previous studies have shown conflicting results concerning the question whether and how retirement is associated with changes in personality traits. Furthermore, there is little knowledge about the role of the job people leave behind when retiring. In the present study, we compared personality development over a 10-year period, based on two waves of a Norwegian survey, between retiring and continuously working blue-collar versus white-collar workers (n = 1,263, Mage = 56.58). Latent change score models showed that neuroticism and openness declined in the sample, but to a comparable degree in all groups. We further found differences in baseline personality traits between blue-collar workers and white-collar workers, as well as between those retiring and not retiring, implying selection into retirement by personality traits. Item level analyses showed declines in some items. We discuss theoretical and methodological implications of our results in light of previous ambiguous findings and emphasize the possible heterogeneity across retirees. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140741856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Efficient Word Segmentation Is Preserved in Older Adult Readers: Evidence From Eye Movements During Chinese Reading 老年读者仍能保持高效分词的补充材料:中文阅读中眼球运动的证据
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000813.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Efficient Word Segmentation Is Preserved in Older Adult Readers: Evidence From Eye Movements During Chinese Reading","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pag0000813.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000813.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140746131","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-related differences in trait affect: Establishing measurement invariance of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). 特质情感中与年龄有关的差异:建立积极和消极情绪表(PANAS)的测量不变性。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000810
Sophie Hoehne, Daniel Zimprich
The present study examined trajectories of trait positive and trait negative affect as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule across the lifespan. Increasing levels of measurement invariance of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule were tested across a wide age range (18-99 years of age; 10 age groups) in a large sample (N = 3,309; 65.82% women; 74.22% with a university entrance diploma; 92.23% with German as their mother tongue). Strong invariance was established so that the latent factor means of positive and negative affect could be meaningfully compared across age groups. Age had a small effect on positive affect, which showed a reversed U-shaped function with a maximum between the ages of 40 and 50. For negative affect, there was a strong cubic age effect. Specifically, negative affect decreased until about the age of 70 and then increased again. We also found a negative covariance between positive and negative affect, which first increased slightly and then decreased across age groups until becoming positive in the oldest age group. In summary, the present study supports previous findings on age trajectories of positive and negative affect but also highlights the importance of testing for measurement invariance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
本研究考察了积极和消极情绪表所测量的特质积极和特质消极情绪在人一生中的轨迹。在一个大样本(样本数=3,309;65.82%为女性;74.22%拥有大学入学文凭;92.23%以德语为母语)中,测试了积极和消极情绪表在较大年龄范围(18-99 岁;10 个年龄组)内的测量不变性。由于建立了较强的不变性,因此可以对不同年龄组的积极情绪和消极情绪的潜在因子均值进行有意义的比较。年龄对积极情绪的影响较小,积极情绪呈反向 U 型函数,最大值出现在 40 至 50 岁之间。对于消极情绪,则存在强烈的立方年龄效应。具体来说,消极情绪在 70 岁之前会下降,之后又会上升。我们还发现,积极情绪和消极情绪之间存在负协方差,在不同年龄组中,积极情绪和消极情绪之间的协方差先是略有上升,然后下降,直到最年长的年龄组才变为积极情绪。总之,本研究支持了之前关于积极情绪和消极情绪的年龄轨迹的研究结果,但也强调了测试测量不变性的重要性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Age-related differences in trait affect: Establishing measurement invariance of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS).","authors":"Sophie Hoehne, Daniel Zimprich","doi":"10.1037/pag0000810","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000810","url":null,"abstract":"The present study examined trajectories of trait positive and trait negative affect as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule across the lifespan. Increasing levels of measurement invariance of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule were tested across a wide age range (18-99 years of age; 10 age groups) in a large sample (N = 3,309; 65.82% women; 74.22% with a university entrance diploma; 92.23% with German as their mother tongue). Strong invariance was established so that the latent factor means of positive and negative affect could be meaningfully compared across age groups. Age had a small effect on positive affect, which showed a reversed U-shaped function with a maximum between the ages of 40 and 50. For negative affect, there was a strong cubic age effect. Specifically, negative affect decreased until about the age of 70 and then increased again. We also found a negative covariance between positive and negative affect, which first increased slightly and then decreased across age groups until becoming positive in the oldest age group. In summary, the present study supports previous findings on age trajectories of positive and negative affect but also highlights the importance of testing for measurement invariance. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140742731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Facing Off-Time Mortality: Leaving a Legacy 面对非正常死亡的补充材料:留下遗产
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000815.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Facing Off-Time Mortality: Leaving a Legacy","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pag0000815.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000815.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140744680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychology and Aging
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1