首页 > 最新文献

Learning & Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Narrative framing may increase human suboptimal choice behavior. 叙事框架可能会增加人类的次优选择行为。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-25 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00587-z
Jessica Stagner Bodily, Kent D Bodily, Robert A Southern, Erin E Baum, Vincent M Edwards

Under certain conditions, multiple nonhuman species have been observed engaging in choice behavior that resulted in less food earned when compared to the amount of food that was available to be earned over the course of a session. This phenomenon is particularly strong in pigeons, but has also been observed in rats and nonhuman primates. Conversely, human participants have demonstrated a propensity to choose more optimally. However, human participants do not exclusively choose the alternative associated with more reinforcement. Framing a task in a real-world narrative has been effective in improving problem-solving on other tasks such as the Wason Four-Card problem. The present study gave human participants a choice task with either abstract stimuli or with a real-world narrative. In addition, participants were given terminal stimuli that were either predictive or unpredictive of reinforcement. Thus, participants were assigned to one of four conditions: Abstract Predictive, Abstract Unpredictive, Narrative Predictive, or Narrative Unpredictive. In contrast to the improved performance on the Wason Four-Card task, the current study found no evidence that the addition of a real-world narrative improved optimal choice performance. Rather, it may have interfered with optimal choice selection in that participants who received the narrative and unpredictive terminal stimuli were at chance performance at the end of the experimental session. Conversely, participants in the Abstract Unpredictive, Abstract Predictive, and Narrative Predictive conditions all demonstrated a preference for the optimal alternative. Possible mechanisms for these findings and future directions are discussed.

在某些条件下,人们观察到多种非人类物种的选择行为会导致获得的食物量少于在整个过程中可获得的食物量。这种现象在鸽子身上尤为明显,但在大鼠和非人灵长类动物身上也有观察到。相反,人类参与者也表现出了更优化选择的倾向。然而,人类参与者并不只选择与更多强化相关的选择。在真实世界的叙述中设置任务框架对提高其他任务(如瓦松四张牌问题)的问题解决能力非常有效。本研究给人类受试者布置了一项选择任务,任务内容可以是抽象刺激,也可以是真实世界的叙述。此外,参与者还被给予了可预测或不可预测强化的终端刺激。因此,参与者被分配到四个条件之一:抽象预测、抽象非预测、叙述预测或叙述非预测。与瓦森四张牌任务的成绩提高形成鲜明对比的是,本研究没有发现任何证据表明添加现实世界的叙述会提高最佳选择成绩。相反,它可能干扰了最优选择,因为接受叙述性和非预测性终端刺激的被试在实验结束时的表现是偶然的。相反,抽象非预测、抽象预测和叙述预测条件下的参与者都表现出了对最优选择的偏好。本文讨论了这些发现的可能机制和未来发展方向。
{"title":"Narrative framing may increase human suboptimal choice behavior.","authors":"Jessica Stagner Bodily, Kent D Bodily, Robert A Southern, Erin E Baum, Vincent M Edwards","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00587-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00587-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Under certain conditions, multiple nonhuman species have been observed engaging in choice behavior that resulted in less food earned when compared to the amount of food that was available to be earned over the course of a session. This phenomenon is particularly strong in pigeons, but has also been observed in rats and nonhuman primates. Conversely, human participants have demonstrated a propensity to choose more optimally. However, human participants do not exclusively choose the alternative associated with more reinforcement. Framing a task in a real-world narrative has been effective in improving problem-solving on other tasks such as the Wason Four-Card problem. The present study gave human participants a choice task with either abstract stimuli or with a real-world narrative. In addition, participants were given terminal stimuli that were either predictive or unpredictive of reinforcement. Thus, participants were assigned to one of four conditions: Abstract Predictive, Abstract Unpredictive, Narrative Predictive, or Narrative Unpredictive. In contrast to the improved performance on the Wason Four-Card task, the current study found no evidence that the addition of a real-world narrative improved optimal choice performance. Rather, it may have interfered with optimal choice selection in that participants who received the narrative and unpredictive terminal stimuli were at chance performance at the end of the experimental session. Conversely, participants in the Abstract Unpredictive, Abstract Predictive, and Narrative Predictive conditions all demonstrated a preference for the optimal alternative. Possible mechanisms for these findings and future directions are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9522838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bumblebees show capacity for behavioral traditions. 大黄蜂显示出行为传统的能力。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-10 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00594-0
Felicity Muth

A new study shows that bumblebees learn socially, and this resulted in a novel behavior becoming dominant across a group. These findings highlight the opportunity going forward to use social insects to address how simple cognitive mechanisms can underpin the development of complex behavioral phenomena.

一项新的研究表明,熊蜂会进行社会学习,这导致一种新行为在整个群体中占主导地位。这些发现凸显了未来利用社会性昆虫来研究简单的认知机制如何支撑复杂行为现象发展的机会。
{"title":"Bumblebees show capacity for behavioral traditions.","authors":"Felicity Muth","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00594-0","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00594-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A new study shows that bumblebees learn socially, and this resulted in a novel behavior becoming dominant across a group. These findings highlight the opportunity going forward to use social insects to address how simple cognitive mechanisms can underpin the development of complex behavioral phenomena.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10143803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Going back to "basics": Harlow's learning set task with wolves and dogs. 回到 "基础":哈洛与狼和狗的学习任务。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-23 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00631-6
Dániel Rivas-Blanco, Tiago Monteiro, Zsófia Virányi, Friederike Range

To survive and reproduce, animals need to behave adaptively by adjusting their behavior to their environment, with learning facilitating some of these processes. Dogs have become a go-to model species in comparative cognition studies, making our understanding of their learning skills paramount at multiple levels, not only with regards to basic research on their cognitive skills and the effects of domestication, but also with applied purposes such as training. In order to tackle these issues, we tested similarly raised wolves and dogs in a serial learning task inspired by Harlow's "learning set." In Phase 1, different pairs of objects were presented to the animals, one of which was baited while the other was not. Both species' performance gradually improved with each new set of objects, showing that they "learnt to learn," but no differences were found between the species in their learning speed. In Phase 2, once subjects had learned the association between one of the objects and the food reward, the contingencies were reversed and the previously unrewarded object of the same pair was now rewarded. Dogs' performance in this task seemed to be better than wolves', albeit only when considering just the first session of each reversal, suggesting that the dogs might be more flexible than wolves. Further research (possibly with the aid of refined methods such as computer-based tasks) would help ascertain whether these differences between wolves and dogs are persistent across different learning tasks.

为了生存和繁衍,动物需要通过调整自己的行为来适应环境,而学习可以促进其中的一些过程。狗已成为比较认知研究中的一个常用模式物种,这使得我们对其学习技能的了解在多个层面上都变得至关重要,这不仅关系到对其认知技能和驯化效果的基础研究,还关系到培训等应用目的。为了解决这些问题,我们在哈洛 "学习集 "的启发下,对同样饲养的狼和狗进行了连续学习任务测试。在第一阶段,我们向动物展示了一对不同的物体,其中一个是有诱饵的,而另一个则没有。随着每组新物体的出现,两种动物的表现都在逐渐提高,这表明它们 "学会了学习",但在学习速度上并没有发现物种间的差异。在第二阶段,一旦受试者学会了其中一个物体与食物奖励之间的联系,就会将条件颠倒过来,让同一对物体中之前没有奖励的那个物体获得奖励。狗在这项任务中的表现似乎优于狼,尽管只是在每次逆转的第一阶段,这表明狗可能比狼更灵活。进一步的研究(可能需要借助改进的方法,如基于计算机的任务)将有助于确定狼和狗之间的这些差异是否会在不同的学习任务中持续存在。
{"title":"Going back to \"basics\": Harlow's learning set task with wolves and dogs.","authors":"Dániel Rivas-Blanco, Tiago Monteiro, Zsófia Virányi, Friederike Range","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00631-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00631-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To survive and reproduce, animals need to behave adaptively by adjusting their behavior to their environment, with learning facilitating some of these processes. Dogs have become a go-to model species in comparative cognition studies, making our understanding of their learning skills paramount at multiple levels, not only with regards to basic research on their cognitive skills and the effects of domestication, but also with applied purposes such as training. In order to tackle these issues, we tested similarly raised wolves and dogs in a serial learning task inspired by Harlow's \"learning set.\" In Phase 1, different pairs of objects were presented to the animals, one of which was baited while the other was not. Both species' performance gradually improved with each new set of objects, showing that they \"learnt to learn,\" but no differences were found between the species in their learning speed. In Phase 2, once subjects had learned the association between one of the objects and the food reward, the contingencies were reversed and the previously unrewarded object of the same pair was now rewarded. Dogs' performance in this task seemed to be better than wolves', albeit only when considering just the first session of each reversal, suggesting that the dogs might be more flexible than wolves. Further research (possibly with the aid of refined methods such as computer-based tasks) would help ascertain whether these differences between wolves and dogs are persistent across different learning tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141081440","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Different memory systems in food-hoarding birds: A response to Pravosudov. 惜食鸟类的不同记忆系统:对普拉沃苏多夫的回应
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00630-7
Tom V. Smulders, Jenny C A Read
{"title":"Different memory systems in food-hoarding birds: A response to Pravosudov.","authors":"Tom V. Smulders, Jenny C A Read","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00630-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00630-7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140969697","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Different memory systems in food-hoarding birds: A response to Pravosudov. 惜食鸟类的不同记忆系统:对普拉沃苏多夫的回应
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00630-7
Tom V Smulders, Jenny C A Read

We recently showed that food-hoarding birds use familiarity processes more than recollection processes when remembering the spatial location of their caches (Smulders et al., Animal Cognition 26:1929-1943, 2023). Pravosudov (Learning & Behavior, https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-023-00616-x , 2023) called our findings into question, claiming that our method is unable to distinguish between recollection and familiarity, and that associative learning tasks are a better way to study the memory for cache sites. In this response, we argue that our methods would have been more likely to detect recollection than familiarity, if Pravosudov's assertions were correct. We also point out that associative learning mechanisms may be good for building semantic knowledge, but are incompatible with the needs of cache site memory, which requires the unique encoding of caching events.

我们最近的研究表明,囤积食物的鸟类在记忆藏匿地点的空间位置时,更多使用的是熟悉过程,而不是回忆过程(Smulders 等,《动物认知》26:1929-1943, 2023)。Pravosudov(《学习与行为》,https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-023-00616-x ,2023)对我们的研究结果提出质疑,认为我们的方法无法区分回忆与熟悉,联想学习任务是研究藏匿地点记忆的更好方法。在这篇回应中,我们认为,如果 Pravosudov 的论断是正确的,我们的方法更有可能检测出回忆而非熟悉。我们还指出,联想学习机制可能有利于建立语义知识,但不符合缓存站点记忆的需要,因为缓存站点记忆需要对缓存事件进行独特的编码。
{"title":"Different memory systems in food-hoarding birds: A response to Pravosudov.","authors":"Tom V Smulders, Jenny C A Read","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00630-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00630-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We recently showed that food-hoarding birds use familiarity processes more than recollection processes when remembering the spatial location of their caches (Smulders et al., Animal Cognition 26:1929-1943, 2023). Pravosudov (Learning & Behavior, https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-023-00616-x , 2023) called our findings into question, claiming that our method is unable to distinguish between recollection and familiarity, and that associative learning tasks are a better way to study the memory for cache sites. In this response, we argue that our methods would have been more likely to detect recollection than familiarity, if Pravosudov's assertions were correct. We also point out that associative learning mechanisms may be good for building semantic knowledge, but are incompatible with the needs of cache site memory, which requires the unique encoding of caching events.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140959749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring boundary conditions of the single-code/default strategy in pigeons 探索鸽子单一代码/默认策略的边界条件
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-26 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00629-0
Carlos Pinto, João Queiroz

To investigate the extent of adoption of more efficient coding strategies, pigeons learned, in three experiments, a symbolic matching-to-sample task that featured an asymmetric sample-comparison mapping. In all experiments, one comparison was correct following one of the samples (one-to-one mapping), and another comparison was correct following the remaining samples (many-to-one mapping). The experiments differed in sample number; Experiment 1 featured three samples, Experiment 2 five samples, and Experiment 3 seven samples. Our goal was to assess the adoption of a single-code/default coding strategy, which establishes two response rules: one rule specific to the sample mapped one-to-one (the single code), and another rule to be applied following any other sample (the default rule). Alternatively, the animals could establish more response rules, one per sample. Thus, the single-code/default strategy allows learning a task via a reduced number of response rules, and the more samples are mapped many-to-one, the greater the savings it allows. As such, the three experiments should progressively be more amenable to the adoption of this strategy. Overall, the adoption of a single-code/default strategy was not widespread. When taken together with previous results, the present study suggests that the amount of training may affect the coding strategy pigeons adopt. Additionally, our results underscore that individual differences are a fundamental aspect to consider when studying learning flexibility.

为了研究鸽子在多大程度上采用了更有效的编码策略,在三次实验中,鸽子学习了一项以非对称样本比较映射为特征的符号匹配到样本任务。在所有实验中,其中一个样本后的一个比较是正确的(一对一映射),而其余样本后的另一个比较是正确的(多对一映射)。实验在样本数量上有所不同:实验 1 有三个样本,实验 2 有五个样本,实验 3 有七个样本。我们的目标是评估采用单一代码/默认编码策略的情况,这种策略建立了两种反应规则:一种规则专门针对一对一映射的样本(单一代码),另一种规则适用于任何其他样本(默认规则)。或者,动物可以建立更多的响应规则,每个样本一个。因此,单一代码/默认策略可以通过减少反应规则的数量来学习任务,而且多对一映射的样本越多,节省的费用就越多。因此,这三个实验应该会逐渐更适合采用这种策略。总体而言,采用单一代码/默认策略的情况并不普遍。结合之前的研究结果,本研究表明,训练量可能会影响鸽子采用的编码策略。此外,我们的研究结果还强调,个体差异是研究学习灵活性时需要考虑的一个基本方面。
{"title":"Exploring boundary conditions of the single-code/default strategy in pigeons","authors":"Carlos Pinto, João Queiroz","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00629-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00629-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>To investigate the extent of adoption of more efficient coding strategies, pigeons learned, in three experiments, a symbolic matching-to-sample task that featured an asymmetric sample-comparison mapping. In all experiments, one comparison was correct following one of the samples (one-to-one mapping), and another comparison was correct following the remaining samples (many-to-one mapping). The experiments differed in sample number; Experiment 1 featured three samples, Experiment 2 five samples, and Experiment 3 seven samples. Our goal was to assess the adoption of a single-code/default coding strategy, which establishes two response rules: one rule specific to the sample mapped one-to-one (the single code), and another rule to be applied following any other sample (the default rule). Alternatively, the animals could establish more response rules, one per sample. Thus, the single-code/default strategy allows learning a task via a reduced number of response rules, and the more samples are mapped many-to-one, the greater the savings it allows. As such, the three experiments should progressively be more amenable to the adoption of this strategy. Overall, the adoption of a single-code/default strategy was not widespread. When taken together with previous results, the present study suggests that the amount of training may affect the coding strategy pigeons adopt. Additionally, our results underscore that individual differences are a fundamental aspect to consider when studying learning flexibility.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140805859","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Serial pattern learning: The anticipation of worsening conditions by pigeons. 连续模式学习:鸽子对恶化条件的预期。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00628-1
Thomas R Zentall, Daniel N Peng

In general, animals are known to be sensitive to the immediacy of reinforcers. That is, they are generally impulsive and outcomes that occur in the future are generally heavily discounted. Furthermore, they should prefer alternatives that provide reinforcers that require less rather than greater effort to obtain. In the present research, pigeons were given a choice between (1) obtaining reinforcers on a progressively more difficult schedule of reinforcement; starting with four pecks, then eight pecks, then 16 pecks, then 32 pecks, and finally 64 pecks on each trial, and (2) a color signaling a number of pecks for a single reinforcer: red = six, green = 11, blue = 23, or yellow = 45. If pigeons choose optimally, most of the time they should choose the progressive schedule to obtain five reinforcers rather than switch to a color to receive only one. However, if they are sensitive primarily to the number of pecks to the next reinforcer, they should choose the progressive schedule once before switching to red, twice before switching to green, three times before switching to blue, and four times before switching to yellow. Instead, they systematically switched too early. Rather than choose based on the rate of reinforcement or even based on the time or effort to the next reinforcer, they appear to anticipate that the progressive schedule is going to get more difficult, and they base their choice suboptimally on the serial pattern of the worsening progressive schedule.

一般来说,众所周知,动物对强化物的即时性很敏感。也就是说,它们通常是冲动型的,对未来发生的结果通常会大打折扣。此外,它们应该更喜欢那些提供强化物的替代品,而这些强化物需要较少而不是较大的努力才能获得。在本研究中,鸽子可以在以下两种情况中做出选择:(1)在难度逐渐增加的强化计划中获得强化物;每次试验从啄4下开始,然后是啄8下,接着是啄16下,然后是啄32下,最后是啄64下;(2)用一种颜色表示单个强化物的啄击次数:红色=6次,绿色=11次,蓝色=23次,黄色=45次。如果鸽子的选择是最优的,那么在大多数情况下,它们应该选择渐进式时间表来获得五个强化物,而不是切换到只获得一个强化物的颜色。然而,如果鸽子主要对啄下一个强化物的次数敏感,那么它们应该在切换到红色之前选择一次渐进式时间表,在切换到绿色之前选择两次渐进式时间表,在切换到蓝色之前选择三次渐进式时间表,在切换到黄色之前选择四次渐进式时间表。相反,他们却过早地进行了系统切换。他们的选择不是基于强化率,甚至不是基于距离下一个强化物的时间或努力程度,而是似乎预料到渐进式时间表会变得越来越难,于是他们根据渐进式时间表不断恶化的序列模式做出了次优选择。
{"title":"Serial pattern learning: The anticipation of worsening conditions by pigeons.","authors":"Thomas R Zentall, Daniel N Peng","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00628-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00628-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In general, animals are known to be sensitive to the immediacy of reinforcers. That is, they are generally impulsive and outcomes that occur in the future are generally heavily discounted. Furthermore, they should prefer alternatives that provide reinforcers that require less rather than greater effort to obtain. In the present research, pigeons were given a choice between (1) obtaining reinforcers on a progressively more difficult schedule of reinforcement; starting with four pecks, then eight pecks, then 16 pecks, then 32 pecks, and finally 64 pecks on each trial, and (2) a color signaling a number of pecks for a single reinforcer: red = six, green = 11, blue = 23, or yellow = 45. If pigeons choose optimally, most of the time they should choose the progressive schedule to obtain five reinforcers rather than switch to a color to receive only one. However, if they are sensitive primarily to the number of pecks to the next reinforcer, they should choose the progressive schedule once before switching to red, twice before switching to green, three times before switching to blue, and four times before switching to yellow. Instead, they systematically switched too early. Rather than choose based on the rate of reinforcement or even based on the time or effort to the next reinforcer, they appear to anticipate that the progressive schedule is going to get more difficult, and they base their choice suboptimally on the serial pattern of the worsening progressive schedule.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Iterative learning experiments can help elucidate music's origins. 迭代学习实验有助于阐明音乐的起源。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-11 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00627-2
Marisa Hoeschele

Anglada-Tort et al. Current Biology, 33, 1472-1486.e12, (2023) conducted a large-scale iterative learning study with cross-cultural human participants to understand how musical structure emerges. Together with archaeological, developmental, historical cross-cultural music data, and cross-species studies we can begin to elucidate the origins of music.

Anglada-Tort等人(《当代生物学》,33, 1472-1486.e12,(2023年))对跨文化人类参与者进行了大规模迭代学习研究,以了解音乐结构是如何产生的。结合考古、发展、跨文化音乐历史数据以及跨物种研究,我们可以开始阐明音乐的起源。
{"title":"Iterative learning experiments can help elucidate music's origins.","authors":"Marisa Hoeschele","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00627-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00627-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Anglada-Tort et al. Current Biology, 33, 1472-1486.e12, (2023) conducted a large-scale iterative learning study with cross-cultural human participants to understand how musical structure emerges. Together with archaeological, developmental, historical cross-cultural music data, and cross-species studies we can begin to elucidate the origins of music.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140102712","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rat spatial memory and foraging on dual radial mazes. 大鼠在双径向迷宫中的空间记忆和觅食能力
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00592-2
William A Roberts, Krista Macpherson, Sophia Robinson, Abagail Hennessy, Bram Richmond

Three experiments are reported that used a new test of spatial memory in rats. The apparatus used was dual eight-arm radial mazes that were connected at one arm of each maze, with a start arm and doors to each maze. Rats could be forced to go to one maze or the other or could make a free choice between mazes. In Experiment 1, rats formed reference memory for the arm containing food on one maze but had food randomly placed on different arms over trials on the other maze. In Experiment 2, rats formed working memory for the arm containing food on one maze but not the other. In Experiment 3, food location changed randomly among trials on both mazes, but one maze contained a cue for the location of food. Rats used reference and working memory to go directly to the food arm on one maze but found food only after searching several arms on the other maze. Most importantly, when given free-choice trials rats developed a significant preference for the maze where they knew the location of food reward or found the cue indicating the location of reward. We suggest these findings may be best interpreted by rats applying two successive rules: (1) choose the maze that leads to the most immediate reward, and (2) use extramaze or intramaze cues to find reward location on the maze.

报告中的三项实验采用了一种新的大鼠空间记忆测试方法。使用的仪器是双八臂径向迷宫,每个迷宫的一臂相连,每个迷宫都有一个起始臂和门。大鼠可以被迫进入其中一个迷宫,也可以在两个迷宫之间自由选择。在实验 1 中,大鼠在一个迷宫中对装有食物的臂形成了参考记忆,但在另一个迷宫的试验中,食物被随机放置在不同的臂上。在实验 2 中,大鼠在一个迷宫中对装有食物的手臂形成了工作记忆,而在另一个迷宫中则没有。在实验 3 中,食物的位置在两个迷宫的试验中随机改变,但其中一个迷宫包含食物位置的提示。在一个迷宫中,大鼠利用参考记忆和工作记忆直接前往食物臂,但在另一个迷宫中,大鼠只有在搜索了几个食物臂后才找到食物。最重要的是,在进行自由选择试验时,大鼠明显偏好知道食物奖励位置或找到指示奖励位置线索的迷宫。我们认为,大鼠应用两个连续规则可以最好地解释这些发现:(1)选择能带来最直接奖励的迷宫;(2)利用迷宫外或迷宫内的线索来寻找迷宫中的奖励位置。
{"title":"Rat spatial memory and foraging on dual radial mazes.","authors":"William A Roberts, Krista Macpherson, Sophia Robinson, Abagail Hennessy, Bram Richmond","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00592-2","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00592-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments are reported that used a new test of spatial memory in rats. The apparatus used was dual eight-arm radial mazes that were connected at one arm of each maze, with a start arm and doors to each maze. Rats could be forced to go to one maze or the other or could make a free choice between mazes. In Experiment 1, rats formed reference memory for the arm containing food on one maze but had food randomly placed on different arms over trials on the other maze. In Experiment 2, rats formed working memory for the arm containing food on one maze but not the other. In Experiment 3, food location changed randomly among trials on both mazes, but one maze contained a cue for the location of food. Rats used reference and working memory to go directly to the food arm on one maze but found food only after searching several arms on the other maze. Most importantly, when given free-choice trials rats developed a significant preference for the maze where they knew the location of food reward or found the cue indicating the location of reward. We suggest these findings may be best interpreted by rats applying two successive rules: (1) choose the maze that leads to the most immediate reward, and (2) use extramaze or intramaze cues to find reward location on the maze.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9668713","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Temporal foundations of episodic memory. 情景记忆的时间基础。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-06 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00608-x
Jonathon D Crystal

A fundamental question in the development of animal models of episodic memory concerns the role of temporal processes in episodic memory. Gallistel, (1990) developed a framework in which animals remember specific features about an event, including the time of occurrence of the event and its location in space. Gallistel proposed that timing is based on a series of biological oscillators, spanning a wide range of periods. Accordingly, a snapshot of the phases of multiple oscillators provides a representation of the time of occurrence of the event. I review research on basic timing mechanisms that may support memory for times of occurrence. These studies suggest that animals use biological oscillators to represent time. Next, I describe recently developed animal models of episodic memory that highlight the importance of temporal representations in memory. One line of research suggests that an oscillator representation of time supports episodic memory. A second line of research highlights the flow of events in time in episodic memory. Investigations that integrate time and memory may advance the development of animal models of episodic memory.

情节记忆动物模型发展过程中的一个基本问题是时间过程在情节记忆中的作用。Gallistel(1990)开发了一个框架,让动物记住事件的具体特征,包括事件发生的时间及其在空间中的位置。Gallistel提出,计时是基于一系列生物振荡器,跨越广泛的周期。因此,多个振荡器的相位的快照提供了事件发生时间的表示。我回顾了关于可能支持记忆发生时间的基本时间机制的研究。这些研究表明,动物使用生物振荡器来表示时间。接下来,我将介绍最近开发的情景记忆动物模型,这些模型强调了时间表征在记忆中的重要性。一项研究表明,时间的振荡表示支持情景记忆。第二条研究线强调了情节记忆中事件的时间流动。整合时间和记忆的研究可能会促进情节记忆动物模型的发展。
{"title":"Temporal foundations of episodic memory.","authors":"Jonathon D Crystal","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00608-x","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00608-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A fundamental question in the development of animal models of episodic memory concerns the role of temporal processes in episodic memory. Gallistel, (1990) developed a framework in which animals remember specific features about an event, including the time of occurrence of the event and its location in space. Gallistel proposed that timing is based on a series of biological oscillators, spanning a wide range of periods. Accordingly, a snapshot of the phases of multiple oscillators provides a representation of the time of occurrence of the event. I review research on basic timing mechanisms that may support memory for times of occurrence. These studies suggest that animals use biological oscillators to represent time. Next, I describe recently developed animal models of episodic memory that highlight the importance of temporal representations in memory. One line of research suggests that an oscillator representation of time supports episodic memory. A second line of research highlights the flow of events in time in episodic memory. Investigations that integrate time and memory may advance the development of animal models of episodic memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71488051","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning & Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1