首页 > 最新文献

Learning & Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
A search pattern for the engram. 雕刻的搜索模式。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00562-0
Chelsey C Damphousse

Roy and colleagues (Nature Communications, 13.1, 1-16, 2022) examined neuronal ensembles associated with contextual fear conditioning memory across multiple brain regions, referred to as a unified engram complex. Their four-step approach incorporating brain-wide mapping of activated neurons, engram indexing, and optogenetic and chemogenetic manipulations could offer a novel, holistic approach to implement in our continued search for the engram.

Roy及其同事(Nature Communications,13.1,1-162022)研究了与多个大脑区域的上下文恐惧条件记忆相关的神经元集合,称为统一的记忆印记复合体。他们的四步方法结合了激活神经元的全脑映射、印迹索引以及光遗传学和化学遗传学操作,可以为我们继续寻找印迹提供一种新的、全面的方法。
{"title":"A search pattern for the engram.","authors":"Chelsey C Damphousse","doi":"10.3758/s13420-022-00562-0","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-022-00562-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Roy and colleagues (Nature Communications, 13.1, 1-16, 2022) examined neuronal ensembles associated with contextual fear conditioning memory across multiple brain regions, referred to as a unified engram complex. Their four-step approach incorporating brain-wide mapping of activated neurons, engram indexing, and optogenetic and chemogenetic manipulations could offer a novel, holistic approach to implement in our continued search for the engram.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10297092","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Properties of iconic and visuospatial working memory in pigeons and humans using a location change-detection procedure. 鸽子和人类使用位置变化检测程序的标志性和视觉空间工作记忆的特性。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-26 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00539-z
Ken Leising, John Magnotti, Cheyenne Elliott, Jordan Nerz, Anthony Wright

Tests of visuospatial memory following short (<1 s) and medium (1 to 30 s) delays have revealed characteristically different patterns of behavior in humans. These data have been interpreted as evidence for different memory systems operating during short (iconic memory) and long delays (working memory). Leising et al. (2019, Behavioural Processes, 169, Article 103957 ) found evidence for both systems in pigeons and humans completing a location change-detection task using a visual mask that disrupted accuracy following a short (100 ms), but not a long (1,000 ms) delay. Another common finding is that adding to-be-remembered items should disrupt accuracy after a long, but not short, delay. Experiments 1a and 1b reported this memory system crossover effect in pigeons and people, respectively, tested on location change detection with delays of 0, 100, and 1,000 ms and displays of two to 16 items. Experiments 2a and 2b reported that the color of the items had little (pigeons) or no (humans) effect on change-detection accuracy. Pigeons tested in Experiment 3 with longer delays (2,000, 4,000, and 8,000 ms) and large set sizes demonstrated the crossover effect with most displays but did not demonstrate an abrupt drop in accuracy characteristic of iconic memory. In Experiment 4, accuracy with novel types of change (color, shape, and size) was better after a 0-ms delay and above-chance levels on color and shape trials. These data demonstrate the memory system crossover effect in both humans and pigeons and expand our knowledge of the properties of memory systems across species.

短时视觉空间记忆测试(
{"title":"Properties of iconic and visuospatial working memory in pigeons and humans using a location change-detection procedure.","authors":"Ken Leising,&nbsp;John Magnotti,&nbsp;Cheyenne Elliott,&nbsp;Jordan Nerz,&nbsp;Anthony Wright","doi":"10.3758/s13420-022-00539-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-022-00539-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Tests of visuospatial memory following short (<1 s) and medium (1 to 30 s) delays have revealed characteristically different patterns of behavior in humans. These data have been interpreted as evidence for different memory systems operating during short (iconic memory) and long delays (working memory). Leising et al. (2019, Behavioural Processes, 169, Article 103957 ) found evidence for both systems in pigeons and humans completing a location change-detection task using a visual mask that disrupted accuracy following a short (100 ms), but not a long (1,000 ms) delay. Another common finding is that adding to-be-remembered items should disrupt accuracy after a long, but not short, delay. Experiments 1a and 1b reported this memory system crossover effect in pigeons and people, respectively, tested on location change detection with delays of 0, 100, and 1,000 ms and displays of two to 16 items. Experiments 2a and 2b reported that the color of the items had little (pigeons) or no (humans) effect on change-detection accuracy. Pigeons tested in Experiment 3 with longer delays (2,000, 4,000, and 8,000 ms) and large set sizes demonstrated the crossover effect with most displays but did not demonstrate an abrupt drop in accuracy characteristic of iconic memory. In Experiment 4, accuracy with novel types of change (color, shape, and size) was better after a 0-ms delay and above-chance levels on color and shape trials. These data demonstrate the memory system crossover effect in both humans and pigeons and expand our knowledge of the properties of memory systems across species.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10287001","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Calibrating your own fears: Feasibility of a remote fear conditioning paradigm with semi-subjective stimulus calibration and differences in fear learning. 校准你自己的恐惧:具有半主观刺激校准和恐惧学习差异的远程恐惧条件反射范式的可行性。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-01 Epub Date: 2022-10-14 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00545-1
Frederic Berg, Jürgen Margraf, André Wannemüller

Fear conditioning studies have occurred mostly in the laboratory, but recently researchers have started to adapt fear conditioning procedures for remote application. Standardization of aversive stimulus material not causing unnecessarily strong discomfort remains an issue especially relevant to research without experimental supervision. The present study introduces a novel semi-subjective method to calibrate aversive sounds in a remotely conducted fear conditioning paradigm. To demonstrate feasibility and proof of concept, 165 participants completed the paradigm, calibrating the loudness of an aversive sound without the guidance of an experimental instructor. This study also aimed to replicate existing findings of participant groups that differed in their early CS-UCS contingency awareness. Participants were classified as Accurate (UCS more likely after the CS+ than CS-), Poor (UCS more likely after the CS- than CS+, or UCS unlikely after either CS), and Threat Biased (UCS equally likely after the CS+ and CS-). Results indicated both the feasibility and efficacy of the paradigm, with participants showing typical patterns of fear learning. Threat Biased participants showed significantly higher uncertainty towards safety signals. There were no differences between the groups in terms of personality traits, thus questioning whether these attributes mediate differences in fear learning and the emergence of anxiety disorders. Using semi-subjective sound calibration appears to be functional, and future studies may consider implementing the new method when remotely administering fear conditioning paradigms.

恐惧条件反射研究大多在实验室进行,但最近研究人员开始将恐惧条件反射程序应用于远程应用。不引起不必要的强烈不适的厌恶性刺激材料的标准化仍然是一个问题,尤其与没有实验监督的研究有关。本研究介绍了一种新的半主观方法来校准远程传导恐惧条件反射范式中的厌恶声音。为了证明可行性和概念验证,165名参与者完成了该范式,在没有实验教师指导的情况下校准厌恶声音的响度。本研究还旨在复制参与者群体在早期CS-UCS应急意识方面存在差异的现有发现。参与者被分为准确(CS+后UCS比CS-更有可能)、差(CS-后UCS比CS+更有可能,或CS后UCS不太可能)和威胁偏好(CS+和CS-后无UCS同样可能)。结果表明了该范式的可行性和有效性,参与者表现出典型的恐惧学习模式。有威胁偏见的参与者对安全信号表现出明显更高的不确定性。两组之间在性格特征方面没有差异,因此质疑这些特征是否在恐惧学习和焦虑症的出现方面起到了中介作用。使用半主观声音校准似乎是可行的,未来的研究可能会考虑在远程管理恐惧条件反射范式时实施新方法。
{"title":"Calibrating your own fears: Feasibility of a remote fear conditioning paradigm with semi-subjective stimulus calibration and differences in fear learning.","authors":"Frederic Berg,&nbsp;Jürgen Margraf,&nbsp;André Wannemüller","doi":"10.3758/s13420-022-00545-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-022-00545-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Fear conditioning studies have occurred mostly in the laboratory, but recently researchers have started to adapt fear conditioning procedures for remote application. Standardization of aversive stimulus material not causing unnecessarily strong discomfort remains an issue especially relevant to research without experimental supervision. The present study introduces a novel semi-subjective method to calibrate aversive sounds in a remotely conducted fear conditioning paradigm. To demonstrate feasibility and proof of concept, 165 participants completed the paradigm, calibrating the loudness of an aversive sound without the guidance of an experimental instructor. This study also aimed to replicate existing findings of participant groups that differed in their early CS-UCS contingency awareness. Participants were classified as Accurate (UCS more likely after the CS+ than CS-), Poor (UCS more likely after the CS- than CS+, or UCS unlikely after either CS), and Threat Biased (UCS equally likely after the CS+ and CS-). Results indicated both the feasibility and efficacy of the paradigm, with participants showing typical patterns of fear learning. Threat Biased participants showed significantly higher uncertainty towards safety signals. There were no differences between the groups in terms of personality traits, thus questioning whether these attributes mediate differences in fear learning and the emergence of anxiety disorders. Using semi-subjective sound calibration appears to be functional, and future studies may consider implementing the new method when remotely administering fear conditioning paradigms.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9568901/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10298077","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
A model for recovery-from-extinction effects in Pavlovian conditioning and exposure therapy. 巴甫洛夫条件调节和暴露治疗中消光效应的恢复模型。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-03-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00578-0
Masato Nihei, Daiki Hojo, Tsunehiko Tanaka, Kosuke Sawa

Exposure therapy is an effective intervention for anxiety-related problems. The mechanism of this intervention has been the extinction procedure in Pavlovian conditioning, and this application has provided many successful instances for the prevention of relapse. However, traditional associative theories cannot comprehensively explain many findings. In particular, it is difficult to explain the recovery-from-extinction effects, which is the reappearance of the conditioned response following extinction. In this paper, we propose an associative model that is a mathematical extension of Bouton's (1993, Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80-99) model for the extinction procedure. The core of our model is that the asymptotic strength of the inhibitory association depends on the degree of excitatory association retrieved in a context in which a conditioned stimulus (CS) is presented and that the retrieval is determined by the similarity between contexts during both reinforcement and non-reinforcement and the retrieval context. Our model provides an explanation of the recovery-from-extinction effects, and implications for exposure therapy.

暴露疗法是治疗焦虑相关问题的有效手段。这种干预的机制是巴甫洛夫条件反射中的消退程序,这种应用为预防复发提供了许多成功的例子。然而,传统的联想理论并不能全面地解释许多发现。特别是,很难解释灭绝效应的恢复,即灭绝后条件反应的再现。在本文中,我们提出了一个联想模型,它是Bouton(1993,Psychological Bulletin,11480-99)灭绝过程模型的数学扩展。我们模型的核心是,抑制性联想的渐近强度取决于在条件刺激(CS)出现的上下文中检索到的兴奋性联想的程度,并且检索是由强化和非强化过程中的上下文与检索上下文之间的相似性决定的。我们的模型提供了从消光效应中恢复的解释,以及对暴露治疗的启示。
{"title":"A model for recovery-from-extinction effects in Pavlovian conditioning and exposure therapy.","authors":"Masato Nihei,&nbsp;Daiki Hojo,&nbsp;Tsunehiko Tanaka,&nbsp;Kosuke Sawa","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00578-0","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00578-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Exposure therapy is an effective intervention for anxiety-related problems. The mechanism of this intervention has been the extinction procedure in Pavlovian conditioning, and this application has provided many successful instances for the prevention of relapse. However, traditional associative theories cannot comprehensively explain many findings. In particular, it is difficult to explain the recovery-from-extinction effects, which is the reappearance of the conditioned response following extinction. In this paper, we propose an associative model that is a mathematical extension of Bouton's (1993, Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80-99) model for the extinction procedure. The core of our model is that the asymptotic strength of the inhibitory association depends on the degree of excitatory association retrieved in a context in which a conditioned stimulus (CS) is presented and that the retrieval is determined by the similarity between contexts during both reinforcement and non-reinforcement and the retrieval context. Our model provides an explanation of the recovery-from-extinction effects, and implications for exposure therapy.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10295802","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
1-Back reinforcement symbolic-matching by humans: How do they learn it? 1-人类的背部强化符号匹配:他们是如何学习的?
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-01-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00558-w
Thomas R Zentall, Peyton M Mueller, Daniel N Peng

For humans, a distinction has been made between implicit and explicit learning. Implicit learning is thought to involve automatic processes of the kind involved in much Pavlovian conditioning, while explicit learning is thought to involve conscious hypothesis testing and rule formation, in which the subject's statement of the rule has been taken as evidence of explicit learning. Various methods have been used to determine if nonverbal animals are able to learn a task explicitly - among these is the 1-back reinforcement task in which feedback from performance on the current conditional discrimination trial is provided only after completion of the following trial. We propose that it is not whether an organism can learn the task, but whether they learn it rapidly, all-or-none, that provides a better distinction between the two kinds of learning. We had humans learn a symbolic matching, 1-back reinforcement task. Almost half of the subjects failed to learn the task, and of those who did, none described the 1-back rule. Thus, it is possible to learn this task without learning the 1-back rule. Furthermore, the backward learning functions for humans differ from those of pigeons. Human subjects who learned the task did so all-or-none, suggesting explicit learning. In earlier research with pigeons, they too showed significant learning of this task; however, backward learning functions suggested that they did so gradually over the course of several sessions of training and to a lower level of asymptotic accuracy than the humans, a result suggesting implicit learning was involved.

对于人类来说,内隐学习和外显学习是有区别的。内隐学习被认为涉及巴甫洛夫条件反射中所涉及的那种自动过程,而外显学习被认为包括有意识的假设检验和规则形成,其中受试者对规则的陈述被视为外显学习的证据。已经使用了各种方法来确定非语言动物是否能够明确地学习一项任务,其中包括单背强化任务,在该任务中,只有在完成以下试验后,才能提供当前条件歧视试验的表现反馈。我们提出,不是生物体是否能学习任务,而是它们是否快速学习,全部或全部不学习,才能更好地区分这两种学习。我们让人类学习了一种符号匹配的单背强化任务。几乎一半的受试者没有学会这项任务,而在那些学会的人中,没有一个人描述了一背规则。因此,可以在不学习1-back规则的情况下学习该任务。此外,人类的后向学习功能与鸽子的不同。学习该任务的人类受试者要么全部完成,要么全部不完成,这表明他们进行了显性学习。在早期对鸽子的研究中,它们也表现出了对这项任务的显著学习;然而,后向学习函数表明,他们在几次训练中逐渐做到了这一点,并且其渐近精度低于人类,这一结果表明他们参与了内隐学习。
{"title":"1-Back reinforcement symbolic-matching by humans: How do they learn it?","authors":"Thomas R Zentall,&nbsp;Peyton M Mueller,&nbsp;Daniel N Peng","doi":"10.3758/s13420-022-00558-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-022-00558-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>For humans, a distinction has been made between implicit and explicit learning. Implicit learning is thought to involve automatic processes of the kind involved in much Pavlovian conditioning, while explicit learning is thought to involve conscious hypothesis testing and rule formation, in which the subject's statement of the rule has been taken as evidence of explicit learning. Various methods have been used to determine if nonverbal animals are able to learn a task explicitly - among these is the 1-back reinforcement task in which feedback from performance on the current conditional discrimination trial is provided only after completion of the following trial. We propose that it is not whether an organism can learn the task, but whether they learn it rapidly, all-or-none, that provides a better distinction between the two kinds of learning. We had humans learn a symbolic matching, 1-back reinforcement task. Almost half of the subjects failed to learn the task, and of those who did, none described the 1-back rule. Thus, it is possible to learn this task without learning the 1-back rule. Furthermore, the backward learning functions for humans differ from those of pigeons. Human subjects who learned the task did so all-or-none, suggesting explicit learning. In earlier research with pigeons, they too showed significant learning of this task; however, backward learning functions suggested that they did so gradually over the course of several sessions of training and to a lower level of asymptotic accuracy than the humans, a result suggesting implicit learning was involved.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10297102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Motivated to time: Effects of reinforcer devaluation and opportunity cost on interval timing. 时间动机:强化货币贬值和机会成本对间隔时间的影响。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-13 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00572-6
Tanya A Gupta, Federico Sanabria

Prior research suggests that interval timing performance is sensitive to reinforcer devaluation effects and to the rate of competing sources of reinforcement. The present study sought to replicate and account for these findings in rats. A self-paced concurrent fixed-interval (FI) random-ratio (RR) schedule of reinforcement was implemented in which the FI requirement varied across training conditions (12, 24, 48 s). The RR requirement-which imposed an opportunity cost to responding on the FI component-was adjusted so that it took about twice the FI requirement, on average, to complete it. Probe reinforcer devaluation (prefeeding) sessions were conducted at the end of each condition. To assess the effect of contextual reinforcement on timing performance, the RR requirement was removed before the end of the experiment. Consistent with prior findings, performance on the FI component tracked schedule requirement and displayed scalar invariance; the removal of the RR component yielded more premature FI responses. For some rats, prefeeding reduced the number of trials initiated without affecting timing performance; for other rats, prefeeding delayed responding on the FI component but had a weaker effect on trial initiation. These results support the notion that timing and motivational processes are separable, suggesting novel explanations for ostensible motivational effects on timing performance.

先前的研究表明,区间时机表现对补强货币贬值效应和竞争补强来源的比率敏感。本研究试图在大鼠身上复制并解释这些发现。实施了自定步的同时固定间隔(FI)随机比率(RR)强化计划,其中FI要求随训练条件(12、24、48 s)而变化。RR要求对FI部分施加了回应的机会成本,因此平均需要大约两倍于FI要求的时间才能完成。在每种情况结束时都会进行探针强化贬值(优先接种)会话。为了评估情境强化对时间表现的影响,在实验结束前取消了RR要求。与先前的研究结果一致,FI组件的性能跟踪了进度要求,并显示出标量不变性;RR成分的去除产生了更多过早的FI反应。对于一些大鼠来说,在不影响定时性能的情况下,优先喂食减少了试验次数;对于其他大鼠,优先喂食延迟了对FI成分的反应,但对试验开始的影响较弱。这些结果支持了时间和动机过程是可分离的这一观点,为表面上的动机对时间表现的影响提供了新的解释。
{"title":"Motivated to time: Effects of reinforcer devaluation and opportunity cost on interval timing.","authors":"Tanya A Gupta,&nbsp;Federico Sanabria","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00572-6","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00572-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research suggests that interval timing performance is sensitive to reinforcer devaluation effects and to the rate of competing sources of reinforcement. The present study sought to replicate and account for these findings in rats. A self-paced concurrent fixed-interval (FI) random-ratio (RR) schedule of reinforcement was implemented in which the FI requirement varied across training conditions (12, 24, 48 s). The RR requirement-which imposed an opportunity cost to responding on the FI component-was adjusted so that it took about twice the FI requirement, on average, to complete it. Probe reinforcer devaluation (prefeeding) sessions were conducted at the end of each condition. To assess the effect of contextual reinforcement on timing performance, the RR requirement was removed before the end of the experiment. Consistent with prior findings, performance on the FI component tracked schedule requirement and displayed scalar invariance; the removal of the RR component yielded more premature FI responses. For some rats, prefeeding reduced the number of trials initiated without affecting timing performance; for other rats, prefeeding delayed responding on the FI component but had a weaker effect on trial initiation. These results support the notion that timing and motivational processes are separable, suggesting novel explanations for ostensible motivational effects on timing performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10297598","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
If Proust had whiskers: Recalling locations with smells. 如果普鲁斯特有胡须:回忆有气味的地方。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00549-x
Ann-Sophie Barwich

Recent research suggests that the piriform cortex simultaneously represents spatial and olfactory information. These findings may provide further insight into the non-topographic principles of odor coding.

最近的研究表明梨状皮质同时代表空间和嗅觉信息。这些发现可能会进一步深入了解气味编码的非地形原理。
{"title":"If Proust had whiskers: Recalling locations with smells.","authors":"Ann-Sophie Barwich","doi":"10.3758/s13420-022-00549-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-022-00549-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent research suggests that the piriform cortex simultaneously represents spatial and olfactory information. These findings may provide further insight into the non-topographic principles of odor coding.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9650761","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Social diffusion of new foraging techniques in the Southern ground-hornbill (Bucorvus leadbeateri). 南方地犀鸟(Bucorvus leadbeateri)新觅食技术的社会传播。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00518-4
Samara Danel, Nancy Rebout, Lucy Kemp

Social learning during foraging has been found in a wide range of animals, including numerous bird species. Still, the mechanisms underlying this cognitive capacity remain largely unstudied and the use of divergent methods limits our understanding of their taxonomic distribution. Using an ecologically relevant design, the open diffusion experiment, we tested whether 11 Southern ground-hornbills (Bucorvus leadbeateri) were able to show imitation on the two-action task. Three experimental groups were created. In the slide and pull group, subjects ('observers') watched a trained conspecific ('demonstrator') opening a box using a specific technique. Naïve individuals from the control group, however, did not receive a social demonstration. All birds of the slide and pull group succeeded in opening the box, whereas all subjects of the control group failed the task. We found consistent inter-individual differences among some observers, with only two birds (one in each group) using the same technique and part of the box contacted by the demonstrator. Our results suggest that at least fine-tune enhancement underlies behavioural diffusion in this novel model species, which provides new research opportunities with direct implications for conservation.

觅食过程中的社会学习在很多动物中都有发现,包括许多鸟类。然而,这种认知能力背后的机制在很大程度上仍未得到研究,不同方法的使用限制了我们对其分类分布的理解。采用与生态学相关的开放扩散实验设计,我们测试了11只南方地犀鸟(Bucorvus leadbeateri)是否能够在双动作任务中表现出模仿。建立了三个实验组。在滑动和拉扯组中,受试者(“观察者”)观看训练有素的受试者(“演示者”)使用特定技术打开一个盒子。Naïve然而,控制组的个体没有得到社会示范。滑拉组的所有鸟都成功打开了盒子,而对照组的所有受试者都失败了。我们在一些观察者中发现了一致的个体间差异,只有两只鸟(每组一只)使用相同的技术和演示者接触的部分盒子。我们的研究结果表明,至少微调增强是这种新型模式物种行为扩散的基础,这为保护提供了新的研究机会。
{"title":"Social diffusion of new foraging techniques in the Southern ground-hornbill (Bucorvus leadbeateri).","authors":"Samara Danel,&nbsp;Nancy Rebout,&nbsp;Lucy Kemp","doi":"10.3758/s13420-022-00518-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-022-00518-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Social learning during foraging has been found in a wide range of animals, including numerous bird species. Still, the mechanisms underlying this cognitive capacity remain largely unstudied and the use of divergent methods limits our understanding of their taxonomic distribution. Using an ecologically relevant design, the open diffusion experiment, we tested whether 11 Southern ground-hornbills (Bucorvus leadbeateri) were able to show imitation on the two-action task. Three experimental groups were created. In the slide and pull group, subjects ('observers') watched a trained conspecific ('demonstrator') opening a box using a specific technique. Naïve individuals from the control group, however, did not receive a social demonstration. All birds of the slide and pull group succeeded in opening the box, whereas all subjects of the control group failed the task. We found consistent inter-individual differences among some observers, with only two birds (one in each group) using the same technique and part of the box contacted by the demonstrator. Our results suggest that at least fine-tune enhancement underlies behavioural diffusion in this novel model species, which provides new research opportunities with direct implications for conservation.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9677746","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
A timely glimpse of memories to come. 对即将到来的回忆的及时一瞥。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-06-01 Epub Date: 2022-12-15 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-022-00557-x
Catalin V Buhusi, Mona Buhusi

Research in the last century has provided insight into the systems, cellular, and molecular processes involved in the formation, storage, recall, and update of memory engrams - the physical manifestation of the long sought-after philosophical and psychological concept of memory traces. Recent technologies allow scientists to visualize the key molecular players involved in segregating, ordering, and linking memories close in time, for future treatment of "disorders of the engram" where memory linking is deficient (e.g., cognitive aging or Alzheimer's) or excessive (e.g., PTSD).

上个世纪的研究深入了解了记忆印记的形成、储存、回忆和更新所涉及的系统、细胞和分子过程,记忆印记是长期以来备受追捧的哲学和心理学记忆痕迹概念的物理表现。最近的技术使科学家能够可视化参与在时间上近距离分离、排序和连接记忆的关键分子参与者,以用于未来治疗记忆连接不足(如认知衰老或阿尔茨海默氏症)或过度(如创伤后应激障碍)的“记忆记忆障碍”。
{"title":"A timely glimpse of memories to come.","authors":"Catalin V Buhusi, Mona Buhusi","doi":"10.3758/s13420-022-00557-x","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-022-00557-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research in the last century has provided insight into the systems, cellular, and molecular processes involved in the formation, storage, recall, and update of memory engrams - the physical manifestation of the long sought-after philosophical and psychological concept of memory traces. Recent technologies allow scientists to visualize the key molecular players involved in segregating, ordering, and linking memories close in time, for future treatment of \"disorders of the engram\" where memory linking is deficient (e.g., cognitive aging or Alzheimer's) or excessive (e.g., PTSD).</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10593987/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9850929","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Response to Hansen Wheat et al.: Additional analysis further supports the early emergence of cooperative communication in dogs compared to wolves raised with more human exposure. 对 Hansen Wheat 等人的回应:额外的分析进一步证实,与更多接触人类的狼相比,狗更早出现合作交流。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-02-21 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00576-2
Hannah Salomons, Kyle C M Smith, Megan Callahan-Beckel, Margaret Callahan, Kerinne Levy, Brenda S Kennedy, Emily E Bray, Gitanjali E Gnanadesikan, Daniel J Horschler, Margaret Gruen, Jingzhi Tan, Philip White, Bridgett M vonHoldt, Evan L MacLean, Brian Hare

Here, we address Hansen Wheat et al.'s commentary in this journal in response to Salomons et al. Current Biology, 31(14), 3137-3144.E11, (2021). We conduct additional analyses in response to Hansen Wheat et al.'s two main questions. First, we examine the claim that it was the move to a human home environment which enabled the dog puppies to outperform the wolf puppies in gesture comprehension tasks. We show that the youngest dog puppies who had not yet been individually placed in raisers' homes were still highly skilled, and outperformed similar-aged wolf puppies who had higher levels of human interaction. Second, we address the claim that willingness to approach a stranger can explain the difference between dog and wolf pups' ability to succeed in gesture comprehension tasks. We explain the various controls in the original study that render this explanation insufficient, and demonstrate via model comparison that the covariance of species and temperament also make this parsing impossible. Overall, our additional analyses and considerations support the domestication hypothesis as laid out by Salomons et al. Current Biology, 31(14), 3137-3144.E11, (2021).

在此,我们对 Hansen Wheat 等人在本刊上针对 Salomons 等人在 Current Biology, 31(14), 3137-3144.E11, (2021) 发表的评论做出回应。针对 Hansen Wheat 等人提出的两个主要问题,我们进行了补充分析。首先,我们研究了狗幼犬在手势理解任务中胜过狼幼犬的原因是移居人类家庭环境这一说法。我们的研究表明,尚未被单独安置在饲养者家中的幼犬仍然非常熟练,它们的表现优于与人类互动程度较高的同龄幼狼。其次,我们讨论了 "接近陌生人的意愿 "可以解释幼犬和幼狼在手势理解任务中的能力差异这一说法。我们解释了原始研究中导致这种解释不充分的各种控制因素,并通过模型比较证明,物种和性情的协方差也使这种解析变得不可能。总之,我们的补充分析和考虑支持 Salomons 等人提出的驯化假说,《当代生物学》,31(14), 3137-3144.E11, (2021)。
{"title":"Response to Hansen Wheat et al.: Additional analysis further supports the early emergence of cooperative communication in dogs compared to wolves raised with more human exposure.","authors":"Hannah Salomons, Kyle C M Smith, Megan Callahan-Beckel, Margaret Callahan, Kerinne Levy, Brenda S Kennedy, Emily E Bray, Gitanjali E Gnanadesikan, Daniel J Horschler, Margaret Gruen, Jingzhi Tan, Philip White, Bridgett M vonHoldt, Evan L MacLean, Brian Hare","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00576-2","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00576-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Here, we address Hansen Wheat et al.'s commentary in this journal in response to Salomons et al. Current Biology, 31(14), 3137-3144.E11, (2021). We conduct additional analyses in response to Hansen Wheat et al.'s two main questions. First, we examine the claim that it was the move to a human home environment which enabled the dog puppies to outperform the wolf puppies in gesture comprehension tasks. We show that the youngest dog puppies who had not yet been individually placed in raisers' homes were still highly skilled, and outperformed similar-aged wolf puppies who had higher levels of human interaction. Second, we address the claim that willingness to approach a stranger can explain the difference between dog and wolf pups' ability to succeed in gesture comprehension tasks. We explain the various controls in the original study that render this explanation insufficient, and demonstrate via model comparison that the covariance of species and temperament also make this parsing impossible. Overall, our additional analyses and considerations support the domestication hypothesis as laid out by Salomons et al. Current Biology, 31(14), 3137-3144.E11, (2021).</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9647564","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning & Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1