首页 > 最新文献

Learning & Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
Bee reasonable: Do bumblebees reason by exclusion? 蜜蜂是理性的:大黄蜂是通过排斥来推理的吗?
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-09 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00661-0
Donna Kean, Alex H Taylor

In a clever adaptation of the two-cups task, a recent paper tested for reasoning by exclusion in bees. Although further work is necessary to rule out competing hypotheses, this study advances our ability to test cognitive capacities in invertebrates.

最近的一篇论文对蜜蜂的排斥性推理进行了测试,这是对两杯任务的巧妙改编。虽然需要进一步的工作来排除相互矛盾的假设,但这项研究提高了我们测试无脊椎动物认知能力的能力。
{"title":"Bee reasonable: Do bumblebees reason by exclusion?","authors":"Donna Kean, Alex H Taylor","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00661-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00661-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a clever adaptation of the two-cups task, a recent paper tested for reasoning by exclusion in bees. Although further work is necessary to rule out competing hypotheses, this study advances our ability to test cognitive capacities in invertebrates.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142958008","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting episodic-like memory in scrub jays: Is there more we can still learn from what-where-when caching behaviour? 重新审视丛鸦的情景记忆:我们还能从何时何地的缓存行为中学到更多东西吗?
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-08 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00665-w
Ella Worsfold, Nicola S Clayton, Lucy G Cheke

Professor Nicola Clayton is perhaps best known for her work on food-caching scrub jays. Her seminal 1998 paper, together with Anthony Dickinson, showed that scrub jays could remember what food they had cached, where and how long ago, suggesting memory ability that is 'episodic-like' in nature. Here, we present data from a previously unpublished study that sought to replicate and extend these findings. The results replicate previous findings and address potential alternative explanations for earlier results. We argue that the controlled behavioural analyses introduced in this study have the potential to add nuance to our understanding of memory in scrub jay cache retrieval, and to inspire new studies exploring this phenomenon, about which we still have so much to learn.

尼古拉·克莱顿教授最出名的可能是她对贮藏食物的灌丛鸦的研究。1998年,她与安东尼·迪金森(Anthony Dickinson)共同发表了一篇具有开创性意义的论文,表明灌木丛鸦能够记住它们储存的食物、地点和时间,这表明它们的记忆能力本质上是“情景式”的。在这里,我们提供了一项先前未发表的研究的数据,该研究试图复制和扩展这些发现。这些结果重复了之前的发现,并对早期结果提出了可能的替代解释。我们认为,本研究中引入的控制行为分析有可能增加我们对灌丛鸦缓存检索记忆的理解的细微差别,并激发探索这一现象的新研究,关于这一现象,我们还有很多需要学习。
{"title":"Revisiting episodic-like memory in scrub jays: Is there more we can still learn from what-where-when caching behaviour?","authors":"Ella Worsfold, Nicola S Clayton, Lucy G Cheke","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00665-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00665-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Professor Nicola Clayton is perhaps best known for her work on food-caching scrub jays. Her seminal 1998 paper, together with Anthony Dickinson, showed that scrub jays could remember what food they had cached, where and how long ago, suggesting memory ability that is 'episodic-like' in nature. Here, we present data from a previously unpublished study that sought to replicate and extend these findings. The results replicate previous findings and address potential alternative explanations for earlier results. We argue that the controlled behavioural analyses introduced in this study have the potential to add nuance to our understanding of memory in scrub jay cache retrieval, and to inspire new studies exploring this phenomenon, about which we still have so much to learn.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142958025","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examination of hierarchical form perception in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). 非洲灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)等级形式感知的研究。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00664-x
Muhammad A J Qadri, Suzanne L Gray

The perception of objects is a challenging task that requires recognizing visual elements and integrating them into a whole. While human vision prioritizes attention to the overall configuration, data from other species suggests this bias towards global form perception is not universal. Studies with pigeons indicate preferential attention to local details when both local and global information may be diagnostic, but studies with other bird species are more limited. To examine whether this local bias is class-wide or potentially species-specific, we studied whether African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) have a bias towards local elements or the global configuration when processing Navon-like hierarchical form displays. Two parrots were tested using a computerized touch-screen two-alternative choice task that presented displays that were local-relevant or global-relevant. The results of several successive acquisition phases suggest that these parrots have no local or global bias, indicating differing evolutionary or ecological drives for visual processing among avian species.

对物体的感知是一项具有挑战性的任务,它需要识别视觉元素并将它们整合成一个整体。虽然人类的视觉优先关注整体结构,但来自其他物种的数据表明,这种对整体形式感知的偏见并非普遍存在。对鸽子的研究表明,当局部和全局信息都可以诊断时,鸽子更倾向于关注局部细节,但对其他鸟类的研究则更为有限。为了检验这种局部偏见是类范围的还是物种特异性的,我们研究了非洲灰鹦鹉(Psittacus erithacus)在处理类似navon的分层形式显示时,是对局部元素还是全局配置有偏见。研究人员用电脑触摸屏对两只鹦鹉进行了两种选择任务的测试,这些选择任务显示的是与当地相关的还是与全球相关的。连续几个获取阶段的结果表明,这些鹦鹉没有局部或全局偏见,这表明鸟类物种之间视觉处理的进化或生态驱动不同。
{"title":"Examination of hierarchical form perception in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus).","authors":"Muhammad A J Qadri, Suzanne L Gray","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00664-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00664-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The perception of objects is a challenging task that requires recognizing visual elements and integrating them into a whole. While human vision prioritizes attention to the overall configuration, data from other species suggests this bias towards global form perception is not universal. Studies with pigeons indicate preferential attention to local details when both local and global information may be diagnostic, but studies with other bird species are more limited. To examine whether this local bias is class-wide or potentially species-specific, we studied whether African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) have a bias towards local elements or the global configuration when processing Navon-like hierarchical form displays. Two parrots were tested using a computerized touch-screen two-alternative choice task that presented displays that were local-relevant or global-relevant. The results of several successive acquisition phases suggest that these parrots have no local or global bias, indicating differing evolutionary or ecological drives for visual processing among avian species.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142928452","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do marmosets really have names? 狨猴真的有名字吗?
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2025-01-02 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00662-z
Kelly Jaakkola

A recent study demonstrated that marmoset "phee calls" include information specific to the intended receiver of the call, and that receivers respond more to calls that are specifically directed at them. The authors interpret this as showing that these calls are name-like vocal labels for individual marmosets, but there is at least one other possibility that would equally explain these data.

最近的一项研究表明,狨猴的“phee呼叫”包含了特定于呼叫接收者的信息,并且接收者对专门针对他们的呼叫做出更多反应。作者解释说,这表明这些叫声是单个狨猴的类似名字的声音标签,但至少还有另一种可能性可以同样解释这些数据。
{"title":"Do marmosets really have names?","authors":"Kelly Jaakkola","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00662-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00662-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A recent study demonstrated that marmoset \"phee calls\" include information specific to the intended receiver of the call, and that receivers respond more to calls that are specifically directed at them. The authors interpret this as showing that these calls are name-like vocal labels for individual marmosets, but there is at least one other possibility that would equally explain these data.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2025-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142923676","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Complex relationship between response rate and preference in pigeons: Williams (1992) revisited. 鸽子的反应率和偏好之间的复杂关系:Williams(1992)。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-12-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00660-1
Thomas R Zentall, Daniel N Peng, Laiba Rasul

A decrease in the rate of reinforcement associated with one component of a multiple schedule is typically associated with a decrease in responding in that component as well as with an increase in responding in the unchanged component. This increase in responding, referred to as positive contrast, is thought to result from an increase in the subjective value associated with the unchanged component. Williams Animal Learning & Behavior, 19, 337-344, (1991) challenged this hypothesis in an experiment with pigeons in which Stimulus A, associated with a variable interval schedule, was always followed by Stimulus X, associated with extinction, while Stimulus B, associated with the same variable interval schedule, was always followed by Stimulus Y, also associated with a variable interval schedule. Although Williams found that most of the pigeons pecked more at Stimulus A than at Stimulus B (behavioral contrast), when the pigeons were given a choice between Stimulus A and B, they showed a preference for Stimulus B. In the present experiment (a slight modification from Williams's), we confirmed this finding. Although our pigeons pecked more at Stimulus A than at Stimulus B, they generally preferred Stimulus B, the stimulus that was not followed by extinction. This result suggests that positive contrast may not result from an increase in the subjective value of the unchanged component. Instead, it suggests that this version of positive contrast may result at least in part from the pigeons' attempt to get all of the reinforcers possible in the presence of Stimulus A before the extinction schedule begins.

与多个计划的一个组成部分相关的强化率的降低通常与该组成部分的响应减少以及未改变的组成部分的响应增加有关。这种反应的增加,被称为积极对比,被认为是由于与不变成分相关的主观价值的增加。Williams动物学习与行为,19,337-344,(1991)在一个鸽子实验中挑战了这一假设,在这个实验中,与可变间隔计划相关的刺激A总是紧随与灭绝相关的刺激X,而与相同可变间隔计划相关的刺激B总是紧随与可变间隔计划相关的刺激Y。虽然Williams发现大多数鸽子啄刺激A的次数多于啄刺激B(行为对比),但当鸽子在刺激A和刺激B之间做出选择时,它们表现出对刺激B的偏好。在本实验中(对Williams的实验进行了轻微修改),我们证实了这一发现。虽然我们的鸽子啄刺激A的次数比啄刺激B的次数多,但它们通常更喜欢刺激B,因为刺激B没有灭绝。这一结果表明,积极的对比可能不是由于增加的主观价值不变的组成部分。相反,它表明,这种积极对比的版本可能至少部分是由于鸽子试图在灭绝计划开始之前在刺激A存在的情况下获得所有可能的强化物。
{"title":"Complex relationship between response rate and preference in pigeons: Williams (1992) revisited.","authors":"Thomas R Zentall, Daniel N Peng, Laiba Rasul","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00660-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-024-00660-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A decrease in the rate of reinforcement associated with one component of a multiple schedule is typically associated with a decrease in responding in that component as well as with an increase in responding in the unchanged component. This increase in responding, referred to as positive contrast, is thought to result from an increase in the subjective value associated with the unchanged component. Williams Animal Learning & Behavior, 19, 337-344, (1991) challenged this hypothesis in an experiment with pigeons in which Stimulus A, associated with a variable interval schedule, was always followed by Stimulus X, associated with extinction, while Stimulus B, associated with the same variable interval schedule, was always followed by Stimulus Y, also associated with a variable interval schedule. Although Williams found that most of the pigeons pecked more at Stimulus A than at Stimulus B (behavioral contrast), when the pigeons were given a choice between Stimulus A and B, they showed a preference for Stimulus B. In the present experiment (a slight modification from Williams's), we confirmed this finding. Although our pigeons pecked more at Stimulus A than at Stimulus B, they generally preferred Stimulus B, the stimulus that was not followed by extinction. This result suggests that positive contrast may not result from an increase in the subjective value of the unchanged component. Instead, it suggests that this version of positive contrast may result at least in part from the pigeons' attempt to get all of the reinforcers possible in the presence of Stimulus A before the extinction schedule begins.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142872953","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rats show up to 72 h of significant retention for spatial memory in the radial maze. 大鼠在径向迷宫中的空间记忆保持时间长达 72 小时。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00633-4
Chiaki Tanaka, Tohru Taniuchi

This study explored long-term retention of spatial memory in rats using an eight-arm radial maze. Crystal and Babb (Learning and motivation, 39(4), 278-284, 2008) previously demonstrated that rats could retain spatial memory for up to 25 h in the radial maze. Notably, they found performance improved with 48-h intertrial intervals compared with 24-h intervals. Our study investigated the effects of extending intertrial intervals on long-term retention of spatial memory by reducing the potential for proactive interference. Each trial comprised a learning phase, during which subjects were required to sequentially visit four randomly selected arms, followed by a free-choice test that included all eight arms, conducted after increasing the retention and intertrial intervals. The retention intervals were systematically increased from 1 h to 24, 48, and, ultimately, 72 h, with corresponding intertrial intervals expanding from 24 to 48, 120, and 144 h. Performance significantly surpassed chance levels across all conditions, demonstrating that rats are capable of retaining spatial memory for up to 72 h.

本研究利用八臂径向迷宫探索大鼠空间记忆的长期保持。Crystal 和 Babb(《学习与动机》,39(4), 278-284, 2008 年)曾证明,大鼠在径向迷宫中可以保持长达 25 小时的空间记忆。值得注意的是,他们发现与 24 小时的试验间隔相比,48 小时的试验间隔能提高大鼠的表现。我们的研究通过减少潜在的主动干扰,探讨了延长试验间隔对空间记忆长期保持的影响。每次试验由学习阶段和自由选择测试组成,前者要求受试者依次访问随机选择的四个臂膀,后者包括所有八个臂膀。在所有条件下,大鼠的表现都大大超过了偶然水平,这证明大鼠能够将空间记忆保持长达 72 小时。
{"title":"Rats show up to 72 h of significant retention for spatial memory in the radial maze.","authors":"Chiaki Tanaka, Tohru Taniuchi","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00633-4","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-024-00633-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study explored long-term retention of spatial memory in rats using an eight-arm radial maze. Crystal and Babb (Learning and motivation, 39(4), 278-284, 2008) previously demonstrated that rats could retain spatial memory for up to 25 h in the radial maze. Notably, they found performance improved with 48-h intertrial intervals compared with 24-h intervals. Our study investigated the effects of extending intertrial intervals on long-term retention of spatial memory by reducing the potential for proactive interference. Each trial comprised a learning phase, during which subjects were required to sequentially visit four randomly selected arms, followed by a free-choice test that included all eight arms, conducted after increasing the retention and intertrial intervals. The retention intervals were systematically increased from 1 h to 24, 48, and, ultimately, 72 h, with corresponding intertrial intervals expanding from 24 to 48, 120, and 144 h. Performance significantly surpassed chance levels across all conditions, demonstrating that rats are capable of retaining spatial memory for up to 72 h.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"330-338"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11628574/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141762114","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Octopus toss-up: Is debris throwing driven by intent? 章鱼扔垃圾:扔垃圾是有目的的吗?
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-14 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00611-2
Alexandra K Schnell

In a noteworthy observation, Godfrey-Smith and colleagues report the first evidence of debris throwing in wild octopuses, including instances where they target conspecifics. Proposing parallels with behaviours observed in select social mammals, this discovery prompts inquiries into the extent of their similarity and the potential role of cognition.

在一项值得注意的观察中,戈弗雷-史密斯和他的同事报告了野生章鱼投掷碎片的第一个证据,包括它们瞄准同种章鱼的例子。这一发现与在某些社会性哺乳动物中观察到的行为相似,促使人们对它们的相似性程度和认知的潜在作用进行探讨。
{"title":"Octopus toss-up: Is debris throwing driven by intent?","authors":"Alexandra K Schnell","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00611-2","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00611-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a noteworthy observation, Godfrey-Smith and colleagues report the first evidence of debris throwing in wild octopuses, including instances where they target conspecifics. Proposing parallels with behaviours observed in select social mammals, this discovery prompts inquiries into the extent of their similarity and the potential role of cognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"285-286"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"92157123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Disentangling the evolution of cognition: Learning in Cnidaria. 解析认知的进化:蛇尾目动物的学习
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-11 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-023-00621-0
Jose Prados

Bielecki et al. Current Biology, 33, 4150-4159, (2023) described new behavioral and physiological paradigms to study associative learning and its neural basis in the Cnidaria Tripedalia cystophora. We discuss the relevance of these findings to further our understanding of the intertwined evolution of cognition and the nervous systems.

Bielecki等人在《当代生物学》(Current Biology)第33卷第4150-4159页(2023年)中描述了研究栉水母纲(Cnidaria Tripedalia cystophora)联想学习及其神经基础的新行为和生理范式。我们讨论了这些发现对于进一步理解认知与神经系统交织进化的意义。
{"title":"Disentangling the evolution of cognition: Learning in Cnidaria.","authors":"Jose Prados","doi":"10.3758/s13420-023-00621-0","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-023-00621-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Bielecki et al. Current Biology, 33, 4150-4159, (2023) described new behavioral and physiological paradigms to study associative learning and its neural basis in the Cnidaria Tripedalia cystophora. We discuss the relevance of these findings to further our understanding of the intertwined evolution of cognition and the nervous systems.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"289-290"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138807305","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Serial pattern learning: The anticipation of worsening conditions by pigeons. 连续模式学习:鸽子对恶化条件的预期。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00628-1
Thomas R Zentall, Daniel N Peng

In general, animals are known to be sensitive to the immediacy of reinforcers. That is, they are generally impulsive and outcomes that occur in the future are generally heavily discounted. Furthermore, they should prefer alternatives that provide reinforcers that require less rather than greater effort to obtain. In the present research, pigeons were given a choice between (1) obtaining reinforcers on a progressively more difficult schedule of reinforcement; starting with four pecks, then eight pecks, then 16 pecks, then 32 pecks, and finally 64 pecks on each trial, and (2) a color signaling a number of pecks for a single reinforcer: red = six, green = 11, blue = 23, or yellow = 45. If pigeons choose optimally, most of the time they should choose the progressive schedule to obtain five reinforcers rather than switch to a color to receive only one. However, if they are sensitive primarily to the number of pecks to the next reinforcer, they should choose the progressive schedule once before switching to red, twice before switching to green, three times before switching to blue, and four times before switching to yellow. Instead, they systematically switched too early. Rather than choose based on the rate of reinforcement or even based on the time or effort to the next reinforcer, they appear to anticipate that the progressive schedule is going to get more difficult, and they base their choice suboptimally on the serial pattern of the worsening progressive schedule.

一般来说,众所周知,动物对强化物的即时性很敏感。也就是说,它们通常是冲动型的,对未来发生的结果通常会大打折扣。此外,它们应该更喜欢那些提供强化物的替代品,而这些强化物需要较少而不是较大的努力才能获得。在本研究中,鸽子可以在以下两种情况中做出选择:(1)在难度逐渐增加的强化计划中获得强化物;每次试验从啄4下开始,然后是啄8下,接着是啄16下,然后是啄32下,最后是啄64下;(2)用一种颜色表示单个强化物的啄击次数:红色=6次,绿色=11次,蓝色=23次,黄色=45次。如果鸽子的选择是最优的,那么在大多数情况下,它们应该选择渐进式时间表来获得五个强化物,而不是切换到只获得一个强化物的颜色。然而,如果鸽子主要对啄下一个强化物的次数敏感,那么它们应该在切换到红色之前选择一次渐进式时间表,在切换到绿色之前选择两次渐进式时间表,在切换到蓝色之前选择三次渐进式时间表,在切换到黄色之前选择四次渐进式时间表。相反,他们却过早地进行了系统切换。他们的选择不是基于强化率,甚至不是基于距离下一个强化物的时间或努力程度,而是似乎预料到渐进式时间表会变得越来越难,于是他们根据渐进式时间表不断恶化的序列模式做出了次优选择。
{"title":"Serial pattern learning: The anticipation of worsening conditions by pigeons.","authors":"Thomas R Zentall, Daniel N Peng","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00628-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-024-00628-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In general, animals are known to be sensitive to the immediacy of reinforcers. That is, they are generally impulsive and outcomes that occur in the future are generally heavily discounted. Furthermore, they should prefer alternatives that provide reinforcers that require less rather than greater effort to obtain. In the present research, pigeons were given a choice between (1) obtaining reinforcers on a progressively more difficult schedule of reinforcement; starting with four pecks, then eight pecks, then 16 pecks, then 32 pecks, and finally 64 pecks on each trial, and (2) a color signaling a number of pecks for a single reinforcer: red = six, green = 11, blue = 23, or yellow = 45. If pigeons choose optimally, most of the time they should choose the progressive schedule to obtain five reinforcers rather than switch to a color to receive only one. However, if they are sensitive primarily to the number of pecks to the next reinforcer, they should choose the progressive schedule once before switching to red, twice before switching to green, three times before switching to blue, and four times before switching to yellow. Instead, they systematically switched too early. Rather than choose based on the rate of reinforcement or even based on the time or effort to the next reinforcer, they appear to anticipate that the progressive schedule is going to get more difficult, and they base their choice suboptimally on the serial pattern of the worsening progressive schedule.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"296-301"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential effect of training impure tacts versus pure tacts plus intraverbal on the emergence of new verbal operants: A conceptual and methodological study. 训练不纯触觉与纯触觉加内部言语对出现新言语操作的不同影响:概念和方法研究。
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-12-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.3758/s13420-024-00636-1
Miguel A Maldonado, José Andrés Lorca-Marín, María Sheila Velo-Ramírez, Francisco J Alós

The aim of this research was to test the effect of training impure tact versus pure tact and intraverbals on the emergence of new verbal operants (impure tacts), thus establishing a conceptual and methodological differentiation on these operants. This was done by varying the training order of intraverbal or impure tact to analyze and confirm whether or not impure tact is the mere sum of pure tact plus intraverbal and therefore has different functions and consequences in learning. An experiment was conducted with 30 participants randomly assigned to three groups. In Group 1, pure tact plus intraverbal and then impure tact were trained. In Group 3 the training order of these operants was counterbalanced. Group 2 was the control group, training only pure tact plus intraverbal. After the training phases, the emergence of impure tacts was tested. The results of this research indicate that the training of impure tacts favors the emergence of new impure tacts to a greater extent than the training of pure tact plus intraverbal and that they therefore have different functions. It is also shown that variation in the order of presentation of the type of training influences the subsequent emergence of new operants (impure tacts), so that creating a previous history of learning in impure tacts favors emergence even when the intraverbal alone is subsequently trained. This has important implications at both conceptual and methodological levels as it would contribute to the development of more effective language training technologies.

本研究的目的是测试不纯触觉与纯触觉和语内触觉的训练对新言语动作(不纯触觉)出现的影响,从而建立对这些动作的概念和方法上的区分。为此,我们改变了语内触觉或不纯触觉的训练顺序,以分析和确认不纯触觉是否仅仅是纯触觉和语内触觉的总和,因而在学习中具有不同的功能和后果。实验将 30 名参与者随机分为三组。在第一组中,先训练纯触觉加语内触觉,然后再训练不纯触觉;在第二组中,先训练纯触觉加语内触觉,然后再训练不纯触觉。在第三组中,这些操作数的训练顺序是平衡的。第 2 组为对照组,只训练纯触觉和言内触觉。训练阶段结束后,测试了不纯触觉的出现情况。研究结果表明,不纯触觉训练比纯触觉加言内语训练更有利于新的不纯触觉的出现,因此它们具有不同的功能。研究还表明,训练类型呈现顺序的变化会影响随后新操作物(不纯触觉)的出现,因此,即使随后只训练语内触觉,先前不纯触觉的学习历史也会有利于新操作物的出现。这在概念和方法论层面都具有重要意义,因为它有助于开发更有效的语言训练技术。
{"title":"Differential effect of training impure tacts versus pure tacts plus intraverbal on the emergence of new verbal operants: A conceptual and methodological study.","authors":"Miguel A Maldonado, José Andrés Lorca-Marín, María Sheila Velo-Ramírez, Francisco J Alós","doi":"10.3758/s13420-024-00636-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13420-024-00636-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The aim of this research was to test the effect of training impure tact versus pure tact and intraverbals on the emergence of new verbal operants (impure tacts), thus establishing a conceptual and methodological differentiation on these operants. This was done by varying the training order of intraverbal or impure tact to analyze and confirm whether or not impure tact is the mere sum of pure tact plus intraverbal and therefore has different functions and consequences in learning. An experiment was conducted with 30 participants randomly assigned to three groups. In Group 1, pure tact plus intraverbal and then impure tact were trained. In Group 3 the training order of these operants was counterbalanced. Group 2 was the control group, training only pure tact plus intraverbal. After the training phases, the emergence of impure tacts was tested. The results of this research indicate that the training of impure tacts favors the emergence of new impure tacts to a greater extent than the training of pure tact plus intraverbal and that they therefore have different functions. It is also shown that variation in the order of presentation of the type of training influences the subsequent emergence of new operants (impure tacts), so that creating a previous history of learning in impure tacts favors emergence even when the intraverbal alone is subsequently trained. This has important implications at both conceptual and methodological levels as it would contribute to the development of more effective language training technologies.</p>","PeriodicalId":49914,"journal":{"name":"Learning & Behavior","volume":" ","pages":"339-351"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2024-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11628571/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141894741","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning & Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1