首页 > 最新文献

The Psychological Record最新文献

英文 中文
A Laboratory Evaluation of the Effects of Empathy Training on Racial Bias 移情训练对种族偏见影响的实验室评估
Pub Date : 2024-09-13 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00606-z
Victoria D. Suarez, Adel C. Najdowski, Angela Persicke, Jonathan Tarbox

The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the effects of behavioral procedures, including relational training and multiple exemplar training on empathic responding of individuals who display racial bias. In particular, this study used a multielement design with five adult participants to evaluate whether the presentation of relational frames of coordination and distinction between the participants’ values and the values of a person belonging to a group for which a bias existed altered empathic responses toward people belonging to such groups. The results showed empathic responding was higher when relational frames of coordination were presented and lower when relational frames of distinction were presented. This study provides preliminary data suggesting that relational training may result in altered patterns of empathic responses toward people belonging to different racial groups for which a bias previously was observed.

本研究的目的是评估行为程序(包括关系训练和多重范例训练)对表现出种族偏见的人的移情反应的影响。具体而言,本研究采用多元素设计,以五名成年参与者为对象,评估参与者的价值观与存在偏见的群体成员的价值观之间的关系协调和区别是否会改变对这些群体成员的移情反应。结果显示,当呈现协调关系框架时,移情反应较高,而当呈现区别关系框架时,移情反应较低。这项研究提供的初步数据表明,关系训练可能会改变对属于不同种族群体的人的移情反应模式,而这些种族群体以前曾被观察到存在偏见。
{"title":"A Laboratory Evaluation of the Effects of Empathy Training on Racial Bias","authors":"Victoria D. Suarez, Adel C. Najdowski, Angela Persicke, Jonathan Tarbox","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00606-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00606-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the effects of behavioral procedures, including relational training and multiple exemplar training on empathic responding of individuals who display racial bias. In particular, this study used a multielement design with five adult participants to evaluate whether the presentation of relational frames of coordination and distinction between the participants’ values and the values of a person belonging to a group for which a bias existed altered empathic responses toward people belonging to such groups. The results showed empathic responding was higher when relational frames of coordination were presented and lower when relational frames of distinction were presented. This study provides preliminary data suggesting that relational training may result in altered patterns of empathic responses toward people belonging to different racial groups for which a bias previously was observed.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142252970","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Replication of a Nonsequential Renewal Model and a Failure to Attenuate Nonsequential Renewal with Extinction Cues 非连续性更新模型的再现以及灭绝线索无法削弱非连续性更新的情况
Pub Date : 2024-09-12 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00608-x
Brianna G. Sarno, Kathryn M. Kestner

Laboratory models of renewal are critical for understanding this form of behavioral relapse and informing clinical practice to reduce the occurrence of relapse and facilitate the maintenance of treatment gains. Nonsequential renewal is a modified procedure developed by (Sullivan et al. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 110(1), 74–86 2018) that provides an arrangement to study operant renewal in a manner more consistent with clients receiving clinical services, which involves exposing subjects to treatment and baseline conditions across two or more contexts. Experiment 1 replicated the procedure of (Craig et al. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 112(2), 210–223 2019) to compare ABA renewal of target responding in rats exposed to the nonsequential or sequential renewal procedure. Experiment 2 investigated the use of an olfactory extinction cue in mitigating renewal. In Experiment 1, the Nonsequential Group displayed a greater magnitude of renewal compared to the Sequential Group, consistent with the findings from (Craig et al. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 110(1), 74–86 2019) but inconsistent with those from (Sullivan et al. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 110(1), 74–86 2018) with human participants. The extinction cue did not mitigate renewal in Experiment 2. The use of a laboratory model of renewal that is more analogous to clinical settings may be valuable in a translational approach to investigating relapse-prevention techniques.

更新的实验室模型对于理解这种行为复发形式和指导临床实践以减少复发的发生并促进治疗成果的维持至关重要。非序列更新是(Sullivan 等人,《行为实验分析杂志》,110(1),74-86 2018 年)开发的一种改进程序,它提供了一种以更符合接受临床服务的客户的方式来研究操作性更新的安排,其中涉及在两个或更多情境中让受试者接触治疗和基线条件。实验 1 复制了(Craig 等人,Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 112(2), 210-223 2019)的程序,比较了 ABA 对暴露于非顺序或顺序更新程序的大鼠目标反应的更新。实验 2 调查了嗅觉消退线索在减轻目标反应更新中的作用。在实验 1 中,与顺序组相比,非顺序组的更新幅度更大,这与(Craig 等人,《行为实验分析杂志》,110(1),74-86 2019 年)的研究结果一致,但与(Sullivan 等人,《行为实验分析杂志》,110(1),74-86 2018 年)对人类参与者的研究结果不一致。在实验 2 中,灭绝线索并没有减轻更新。在研究复发预防技术的转化方法中,使用更类似于临床环境的实验室复发模型可能很有价值。
{"title":"A Replication of a Nonsequential Renewal Model and a Failure to Attenuate Nonsequential Renewal with Extinction Cues","authors":"Brianna G. Sarno, Kathryn M. Kestner","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00608-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00608-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Laboratory models of renewal are critical for understanding this form of behavioral relapse and informing clinical practice to reduce the occurrence of relapse and facilitate the maintenance of treatment gains. <i>Nonsequential</i> renewal is a modified procedure developed by (Sullivan et al. <i>Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</i>, <i>110</i>(1), 74–86 2018) that provides an arrangement to study operant renewal in a manner more consistent with clients receiving clinical services, which involves exposing subjects to treatment and baseline conditions across two or more contexts. Experiment 1 replicated the procedure of (Craig et al. <i>Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</i>, <i>112</i>(2), 210–223 2019) to compare ABA renewal of target responding in rats exposed to the nonsequential or sequential renewal procedure. Experiment 2 investigated the use of an olfactory extinction cue in mitigating renewal. In Experiment 1, the Nonsequential Group displayed a greater magnitude of renewal compared to the Sequential Group, consistent with the findings from (Craig et al. <i>Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</i>, <i>110</i>(1), 74–86 2019) but inconsistent with those from (Sullivan et al. <i>Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior</i>, <i>110</i>(1), 74–86 2018) with human participants. The extinction cue did not mitigate renewal in Experiment 2. The use of a laboratory model of renewal that is more analogous to clinical settings may be valuable in a translational approach to investigating relapse-prevention techniques.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142181908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Predicting and Interpreting Patterns of Responding on the IRAP in the Context of Facial Emotions and Depression 在面部情绪和抑郁的背景下预测和解释 IRAP 的反应模式
Pub Date : 2024-09-05 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00607-y
Renato Bortoloti, Ana Paula Rubert Alves de Azevedo, Colin Harte, Dermot Barnes-Holmes

This study aimed to build on existing research on the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP), which has identified different patterns of IRAP effects associated with responses to happy and angry facial expressions. In particular, the study aimed to (1) replicate the previously observed happiness superiority effect, but with a far larger participant group than previous studies and (2) explore whether the IRAP effect would be influenced by self-reported levels of depression. One hundred twenty-two participants completed an IRAP that presented pairs of emotional human faces in a 2x2 crossover design yielding four trial-types: happy face–happy face, happy face–angry face, angry face–happy face and angry face–angry face. The results showed a clear happiness superiority effect, with the IRAP effect for trial-type 1 (happy face–happy face) larger than the IRAP effect for trial-type 4 (angry face–angry face). Self-reported depression appeared to moderate responding on the trial-type 1 alone, with low depressed individuals producing a larger D-IRAP score on the trial-type 1 relative to high depressed individuals. The findings support recent arguments that the stimulus function properties of all elements within an IRAP should be taken into account when predicting and interpreting behavioral patterns produced on the procedure. Considerations for use of the IRAP to predict behaviors in the natural environment are discussed.

本研究旨在以现有的内隐关系评估程序(IRAP)研究为基础,该程序发现了与快乐和愤怒面部表情反应相关的不同IRAP效应模式。特别是,本研究旨在:(1)复制之前观察到的快乐优势效应,但参与者群体要比之前的研究大得多;(2)探索 IRAP 效应是否会受到自我报告的抑郁水平的影响。122 名参与者完成了 IRAP,在 2x2 交叉设计中呈现了成对的情绪化人脸,产生了四种试验类型:开心脸-开心脸、开心脸-生气脸、生气脸-开心脸和生气脸-生气脸。结果显示了明显的快乐优势效应,试验类型 1(快乐脸-快乐脸)的 IRAP 效应大于试验类型 4(愤怒脸-愤怒脸)的 IRAP 效应。自我报告的抑郁情绪似乎仅对试验类型 1 的反应有调节作用,相对于抑郁情绪较高的人,抑郁情绪较低的人在试验类型 1 上的 D-IRAP 得分更高。这些研究结果支持最近的观点,即在预测和解释程序中产生的行为模式时,应考虑到 IRAP 中所有元素的刺激功能特性。研究还讨论了使用 IRAP 预测自然环境中行为的注意事项。
{"title":"Predicting and Interpreting Patterns of Responding on the IRAP in the Context of Facial Emotions and Depression","authors":"Renato Bortoloti, Ana Paula Rubert Alves de Azevedo, Colin Harte, Dermot Barnes-Holmes","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00607-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00607-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study aimed to build on existing research on the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP), which has identified different patterns of IRAP effects associated with responses to happy and angry facial expressions. In particular, the study aimed to (1) replicate the previously observed happiness superiority effect, but with a far larger participant group than previous studies and (2) explore whether the IRAP effect would be influenced by self-reported levels of depression. One hundred twenty-two participants completed an IRAP that presented pairs of emotional human faces in a 2x2 crossover design yielding four trial-types: happy face–happy face, happy face–angry face, angry face–happy face and angry face–angry face. The results showed a clear happiness superiority effect, with the IRAP effect for trial-type 1 (happy face–happy face) larger than the IRAP effect for trial-type 4 (angry face–angry face). Self-reported depression appeared to moderate responding on the trial-type 1 alone, with low depressed individuals producing a larger D-IRAP score on the trial-type 1 relative to high depressed individuals. The findings support recent arguments that the stimulus function properties of all elements within an IRAP should be taken into account when predicting and interpreting behavioral patterns produced on the procedure. Considerations for use of the IRAP to predict behaviors in the natural environment are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142181909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mathematical Prediction of Emergent Relations in the Merger of Equivalence Classes 等价类合并中新出现关系的数学预测
Pub Date : 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00605-0
Ramon Marin, Giovan W. Ribeiro

Two distinct equivalence classes can be merged by teaching one relation between one member of each class. This association between the classes has the potential to generate emergent relations among all members of each class. The present study proposes an equation that allows prediction of the total number of emergent relations among classes when they are merged. The equation was tested in two situations and reliably predicted the number of emergent relations when two classes were merged into one. Some functional implications of the findings for the merger of equivalence classes are discussed.

两个不同的等价类可以通过在每个类的一个成员之间建立一种关系来合并。类之间的这种关联有可能在每个类的所有成员之间产生新兴关系。本研究提出了一个等式,可以预测等价类合并时产生的关系总数。该方程在两种情况下进行了测试,并可靠地预测了两个类合并为一个类时出现的关系数量。本文讨论了研究结果对等价类合并的一些功能影响。
{"title":"Mathematical Prediction of Emergent Relations in the Merger of Equivalence Classes","authors":"Ramon Marin, Giovan W. Ribeiro","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00605-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00605-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Two distinct equivalence classes can be merged by teaching one relation between one member of each class. This association between the classes has the potential to generate emergent relations among all members of each class. The present study proposes an equation that allows prediction of the total number of emergent relations among classes when they are merged. The equation was tested in two situations and reliably predicted the number of emergent relations when two classes were merged into one. Some functional implications of the findings for the merger of equivalence classes are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142227605","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential Trial-Type Effects in an Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: Extending the DAARRE Model 内隐关系评估程序中的差异试验类型效应:扩展 DAARRE 模型
Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00604-1
Rodrigo Vianna de Almeida, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Julian C. Leslie

The differential arbitrarily applicable relational responding effects (DAARRE) model explains two effects commonly observed with the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP): the single trial-type dominance effect (STTDE) and the dissonant-target trial-type effect (DTTTE). We sought to explore variables that modulate these effects. Forty-two participants who completed an IRAP met the practice criteria (median latency ≤ 2,000 ms and accuracy ≥ 80% correct) and thus proceeded to six test blocks of trials. The IRAP had four trial-types: trial-type 1: happy-face—happy-word; trial-type 2: happy-face—fear-word; trial-type 3: fear-face—happy-word; trial-type 4: fear-face—fear-word. Participants were randomly assigned to start with a consistent (respond True, False, False, True, respectively, to the four trial-types) or inconsistent (opposite responding) block. Difference (DIRAP) scores were calculated (inconsistent minus consistent latencies). A significantly larger DIRAP score was observed in trial-type 2 relative to trial-type 3 (a DTTTE), but only for participants who started with the inconsistent block. A “happiness superiority” STTDE (i.e., larger DIRAP score on trial-type 1 relative to trial-type 4) was observed, but only for participants who failed to maintain the criteria at the trial-type level (in any of the trial-types). Overall, the findings highlight the importance of distinguishing between the functional versus relational properties of the stimuli presented within an IRAP. The modulation of the DTTTE is interpreted in terms of functional coherence between the target and response option in inconsistent blocks. A novel data-analytic algorithm is presented to identify performance criteria violations at the trial-type level. The findings suggest that a greater focus is required in relational frame theory on the impact of the functional properties of stimuli on relational framing.

差异任意适用关系反应效应(DAARRE)模型解释了内隐关系评估程序(IRAP)中常见的两种效应:单一试验类型优势效应(STTDE)和不和谐目标试验类型效应(DTTTE)。我们试图探索调节这些效应的变量。完成 IRAP 的 42 名参与者均符合练习标准(中位潜伏期≤ 2,000 毫秒,正确率≥ 80%),并因此进入了六个试验块。IRAP 有四种试验类型:试验类型 1:快乐-表情-快乐词;试验类型 2:快乐-表情-恐惧词;试验类型 3:恐惧-表情-快乐词;试验类型 4:恐惧-表情-恐惧词。参与者被随机分配到一致(分别对四种试验类型做出 "真"、"假"、"假"、"真 "的反应)或不一致(做出相反的反应)区块。计算差异(DIRAP)得分(不一致潜伏期减去一致潜伏期)。与试验类型 3(DTTTE)相比,试验类型 2 的 DIRAP 分数明显更高,但只有从不一致区块开始的参与者才会出现这种情况。观察到了 "幸福优势 "STTDE(即相对于试验类型 4,试验类型 1 的 DIRAP 得分更高),但仅适用于未能在试验类型级别(任何试验类型)保持标准的参与者。总之,研究结果强调了区分 IRAP 中呈现的刺激物的功能属性和关系属性的重要性。DTTTE 的调节可以从不连贯区块中目标和反应选项之间的功能连贯性来解释。研究还提出了一种新颖的数据分析算法,用于识别试验类型水平上违反成绩标准的情况。研究结果表明,关系框架理论需要更加关注刺激物的功能特性对关系框架的影响。
{"title":"Differential Trial-Type Effects in an Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: Extending the DAARRE Model","authors":"Rodrigo Vianna de Almeida, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Julian C. Leslie","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00604-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00604-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The differential arbitrarily applicable relational responding effects (DAARRE) model explains two effects commonly observed with the implicit relational assessment procedure (IRAP): the single trial-type dominance effect (STTDE) and the dissonant-target trial-type effect (DTTTE). We sought to explore variables that modulate these effects. Forty-two participants who completed an IRAP met the practice criteria (median latency ≤ 2,000 ms and accuracy ≥ 80% correct) and thus proceeded to six test blocks of trials. The IRAP had four trial-types: trial-type 1: happy-face—happy-word; trial-type 2: happy-face—fear-word; trial-type 3: fear-face—happy-word; trial-type 4: fear-face—fear-word. Participants were randomly assigned to start with a consistent (respond True, False, False, True, respectively, to the four trial-types) or inconsistent (opposite responding) block. Difference (<i>D</i><sub>IRAP</sub>) scores were calculated (inconsistent minus consistent latencies). A significantly larger <i>D</i><sub>IRAP</sub> score was observed in trial-type 2 relative to trial-type 3 (a DTTTE), but only for participants who started with the inconsistent block. A “happiness superiority” STTDE (i.e., larger <i>D</i><sub>IRAP</sub> score on trial-type 1 relative to trial-type 4) was observed, but only for participants who failed to maintain the criteria at the trial-type level (in any of the trial-types). Overall, the findings highlight the importance of distinguishing between the functional versus relational properties of the stimuli presented within an IRAP. The modulation of the DTTTE is interpreted in terms of functional coherence between the target and response option in inconsistent blocks. A novel data-analytic algorithm is presented to identify performance criteria violations at the trial-type level. The findings suggest that a greater focus is required in relational frame theory on the impact of the functional properties of stimuli on relational framing.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142181910","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Comprehensive Behavioral Model of Emotion Rooted in Relational Frame Theory and Contemporary Extensions 植根于关系框架理论和当代扩展的情绪综合行为模型
Pub Date : 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00603-2
Jordan Belisle, Dana Paliliunas, Rocco Catrone, Elana Sickman, Arvind Ramakrishnan

There exists a vast literature on affect and emotion in psychological disciplines, yet contemporary conceptualizations and technologies to predict and influence emotion have been slower to emerge in behavior analysis. The current article is an attempt to conceptualize emotional experiencing through a radical behavioral lens using relational frame theory (RFT) and contemporary extensions. RFT provides a behavioral approach to cognitive appraisal within existing models of human emotion by emphasizing derived relational responding, transformation of stimulus function, and generalized reinforcement learning. Relational density theory (RDT) and the hyperdimensional multilevel (HDML) framework both expand upon RFT and may allow for a more complete account of emotional experiencing within complex networks. Synthesizing these two approaches yields multiple testable predictions that are consistent with RDT across levels of the HDML. Moreover, the ROE-M (relating, orienting, and evoking functions within a motivational context) is a dynamical unit that may be readily evident within emotional experiencing as it is generally described within the psychological literature, and compatible with the synthesized model. Taken together, these approaches and emerging research on affective dynamics may provide a starting point to develop a robust and comprehensive analysis of human emotion that can strengthen behavior analysis and therapies

心理学学科中存在大量关于情感和情绪的文献,但行为分析中预测和影响情绪的当代概念和技术却出现较慢。本文试图利用关系框架理论(RFT)和当代扩展理论,通过激进的行为视角对情绪体验进行概念化。关系框架理论通过强调派生关系反应、刺激功能转换和广义强化学习,在现有的人类情绪模型中为认知评估提供了一种行为方法。关系密度理论(RDT)和超维多层次(HDML)框架都是对 RFT 的扩展,可以更全面地解释复杂网络中的情绪体验。综合这两种方法可以得出多种可检验的预测,这些预测与 RDT 在 HDML 各层次上的预测是一致的。此外,ROE-M(动机背景下的关联、定向和唤起功能)是一个动态单元,在心理学文献中通常描述的情绪体验中很容易体现出来,并且与综合模型相兼容。综上所述,这些方法和关于情感动力学的新兴研究可能会提供一个起点,对人类情感进行稳健而全面的分析,从而加强行为分析和疗法。
{"title":"A Comprehensive Behavioral Model of Emotion Rooted in Relational Frame Theory and Contemporary Extensions","authors":"Jordan Belisle, Dana Paliliunas, Rocco Catrone, Elana Sickman, Arvind Ramakrishnan","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00603-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00603-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p>There exists a vast literature on affect and emotion in psychological disciplines, yet contemporary conceptualizations and technologies to predict and influence emotion have been slower to emerge in behavior analysis. The current article is an attempt to conceptualize emotional experiencing through a radical behavioral lens using relational frame theory (RFT) and contemporary extensions. RFT provides a behavioral approach to cognitive appraisal within existing models of human emotion by emphasizing derived relational responding, transformation of stimulus function, and generalized reinforcement learning. Relational density theory (RDT) and the hyperdimensional multilevel (HDML) framework both expand upon RFT and may allow for a more complete account of emotional experiencing within complex networks. Synthesizing these two approaches yields multiple testable predictions that are consistent with RDT across levels of the HDML. Moreover, the ROE-M (relating, orienting, and evoking functions within a motivational context) is a dynamical unit that may be readily evident within emotional experiencing as it is generally described within the psychological literature, and compatible with the synthesized model. Taken together, these approaches and emerging research on affective dynamics may provide a starting point to develop a robust and comprehensive analysis of human emotion that can strengthen behavior analysis and therapies</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141608643","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Increasing Toy Engagement via Response Disequilibrium Theory: A Systematic Replication 通过反应失衡理论提高玩具参与度:系统复制
Pub Date : 2024-06-25 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00601-4
Megan Ryan, Hunter King, Emily Chesbrough, John Michael Falligant

Response disequilibrium theory suggests that a response deficit in a contingent activity (e.g., iPad time) can increase engagement in an instrumental activity (e.g., work completion) to access the contingent activity. The purpose of the current study was to conduct a systematic replication of Falligant and Rooker The Psychological Record, 71, 307–311, (2021) to further demonstrate the generality and applicability of this approach in clinical contexts. Results of the current study align with prior research demonstrating the ability of the disequilibrium approach to quantify the magnitude and direction of predicted change in instrumental activities based on measures of free operant baseline responding. We discuss study findings from a practical standpoint and offer recommendations for future research on the use of response disequilibrium theory for increasing instrumental activities in clinical practice and research.

反应失衡理论认为,在或有活动(如 iPad 使用时间)中的反应不足会增加工具性活动(如完成工作)的参与度,从而获得或有活动。本研究的目的是对 Falligant 和 Rooker 的研究进行系统性的复制,以进一步证明这种方法在临床环境中的普遍性和适用性。目前的研究结果与之前的研究结果一致,都证明了不平衡方法能够根据自由操作基线反应的测量结果,量化工具活动中预测变化的幅度和方向。我们从实用的角度讨论了研究结果,并为今后在临床实践和研究中使用反应失衡理论增加工具性活动的研究提出了建议。
{"title":"Increasing Toy Engagement via Response Disequilibrium Theory: A Systematic Replication","authors":"Megan Ryan, Hunter King, Emily Chesbrough, John Michael Falligant","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00601-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00601-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Response disequilibrium theory suggests that a response deficit in a contingent activity (e.g., iPad time) can increase engagement in an instrumental activity (e.g., work completion) to access the contingent activity. The purpose of the current study was to conduct a systematic replication of Falligant and Rooker <i>The Psychological Record</i>, <i>71</i>, 307–311, (2021) to further demonstrate the generality and applicability of this approach in clinical contexts. Results of the current study align with prior research demonstrating the ability of the disequilibrium approach to quantify the magnitude and direction of predicted change in instrumental activities based on measures of free operant baseline responding. We discuss study findings from a practical standpoint and offer recommendations for future research on the use of response disequilibrium theory for increasing instrumental activities in clinical practice and research.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141508379","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relational Cues are Affectively Differentiated 关系线索具有情感差异
Pub Date : 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00600-5
Micah Amd
{"title":"Relational Cues are Affectively Differentiated","authors":"Micah Amd","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00600-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00600-5","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141383656","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Operant Wheel Running on a Fixed Interval Schedule: Does Contingent Sucrose Reinforcement Increase Running through a Schedule-Induction or Reinforcement Effect? 操作轮在固定间隔时间表上运行:条件蔗糖强化是通过计划诱导还是强化效应来提高奔跑速度?
Pub Date : 2024-05-28 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00599-9
Terry W. Belke, Craig H. McClelland

Sucrose reinforcement increases operant wheel running, but it is not certain that the increase is due to a reinforcement rather than a schedule-induction effect. To address this issue, eight female Long Evans rats were exposed to a fixed-interval (FI) 60-s schedule with wheel running as the operant. Across three conditions, water, sucrose, and water were delivered as outcomes. In addition, rats also ran freely for 20 min prior to, and subsequent to, exposure to the FI 60-s schedule. Results showed that operant wheel running on the FI 60-s schedule increased when sucrose was delivered. At the level of revolutions, over the reinforcement interval more rats showed increased revolutions in the sucrose condition in the latter half of the interval, consistent with a reinforcement effect, than an inverted U-shaped pattern in the initial half of the interval. Running during the pre- and postoperant running period was not substantially affected by the removal of sucrose reinforcement on the FI 60-s schedule. Sucrose reinforcement, but not water, increased running relative to the pre/post running periods. Results suggest that although both schedule-induction and reinforcement effects occur, the effect that dominates appears to be a function of the duration of the reinforcement interval with a reinforcement effect more prevalent with shorter intervals.

蔗糖强化会增加操作性车轮跑,但还不能确定这种增加是由于强化而不是时间表诱导效应。为了解决这个问题,我们对八只雌性长伊文大鼠进行了60秒固定间隔(FI)训练,以车轮跑为操作行为。在三种条件下,结果分别是水、蔗糖和水。此外,大鼠在接触固定间隔 60 秒时间表之前和之后还自由奔跑了 20 分钟。结果表明,当蔗糖被输送到大鼠体内时,大鼠在FI 60秒计划中的操作性轮跑会增加。在转数水平上,在强化间隔中,更多的大鼠在蔗糖条件下的后半段转数增加,这与强化效应一致,而在强化间隔的前半段则呈倒 U 型。在 FI 60 秒计划中取消蔗糖强化后,大鼠在术前和术后奔跑期的奔跑没有受到实质性影响。蔗糖强化(而不是水)增加了跑步前/后的跑步次数。结果表明,虽然会出现时间表诱导效应和强化效应,但占主导地位的效应似乎是强化间隔时间长短的函数,强化间隔时间越短,强化效应越明显。
{"title":"Operant Wheel Running on a Fixed Interval Schedule: Does Contingent Sucrose Reinforcement Increase Running through a Schedule-Induction or Reinforcement Effect?","authors":"Terry W. Belke, Craig H. McClelland","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00599-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00599-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Sucrose reinforcement increases operant wheel running, but it is not certain that the increase is due to a reinforcement rather than a schedule-induction effect. To address this issue, eight female Long Evans rats were exposed to a fixed-interval (FI) 60-s schedule with wheel running as the operant. Across three conditions, water, sucrose, and water were delivered as outcomes. In addition, rats also ran freely for 20 min prior to, and subsequent to, exposure to the FI 60-s schedule. Results showed that operant wheel running on the FI 60-s schedule increased when sucrose was delivered. At the level of revolutions, over the reinforcement interval more rats showed increased revolutions in the sucrose condition in the latter half of the interval, consistent with a reinforcement effect, than an inverted U-shaped pattern in the initial half of the interval. Running during the pre- and postoperant running period was not substantially affected by the removal of sucrose reinforcement on the FI 60-s schedule. Sucrose reinforcement, but not water, increased running relative to the pre/post running periods. Results suggest that although both schedule-induction and reinforcement effects occur, the effect that dominates appears to be a function of the duration of the reinforcement interval with a reinforcement effect more prevalent with shorter intervals.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141166354","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do–Say Correspondence in Adults: Audience Control in a Virtual Game 成人的 "做-说 "对应关系:虚拟游戏中的观众控制
Pub Date : 2024-05-28 DOI: 10.1007/s40732-024-00598-w
Cristiane Alves, Josiane Maria Donadeli, Julio C. de Rose

This exploratory and descriptive study used a digital game to investigate the effects of audiences on the accuracy of self-reports of 10 undergraduate students. The participants assumed the role of a prince who, being accused of murdering the king, had to navigate a labyrinthine castle to find proof of his innocence. To wander through the castle players needed to collect objects that maintained the prince’s energy, keeping him alive. Some objects provided a small amount of energy and others provided 20 times more energy but their collection was prohibited by the kingdom’s laws. At specific moments the player was questioned by different characters (audiences) about whether they had transgressed the law. There were four audiences (mother, brother, soldier, and computer) that varied in how they indicated the probability of either punishment or support. All participants completed the game and eight transgressed, collecting forbidden objects. A larger number of distorted reports, denying transgressions, were made to the audiences who signaled a higher probability of punishment, but a significant proportion of distorted reports were made even to the audiences who presented themselves as supportive and nonpunitive. Because no actual punishment for transgressions occurred in the game, reporting behavior was likely influenced by participants’ histories with audiences functionally similar to those simulated in the game.

这项探索性和描述性研究利用一款数字游戏来调查受众对 10 名本科生自我报告准确性的影响。参与者扮演一位王子,他被指控谋杀了国王,必须在迷宫般的城堡中寻找证明自己清白的证据。要在城堡中穿行,玩家需要收集能维持王子能量的物品,使他保持活力。有些物品能提供少量能量,有些则能提供 20 倍的能量,但王国法律禁止收集这些物品。在特定的时刻,不同的角色(观众)会询问玩家是否触犯了法律。受众有四个(母亲、兄弟、士兵和电脑),他们表示惩罚或支持的概率各不相同。所有参与者都完成了游戏,其中八人越轨,收集了违禁物品。对表示惩罚可能性较高的受试者进行的否认越轨行为的歪曲报告数量较多,但即使是对表示支持和非惩罚性的受试者进行的歪曲报告也占了相当大的比例。由于游戏中没有出现对越轨行为的实际惩罚,参与者的报告行为很可能受到与游戏中模拟的受众功能相似的受众历史的影响。
{"title":"Do–Say Correspondence in Adults: Audience Control in a Virtual Game","authors":"Cristiane Alves, Josiane Maria Donadeli, Julio C. de Rose","doi":"10.1007/s40732-024-00598-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-024-00598-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This exploratory and descriptive study used a digital game to investigate the effects of audiences on the accuracy of self-reports of 10 undergraduate students. The participants assumed the role of a prince who, being accused of murdering the king, had to navigate a labyrinthine castle to find proof of his innocence. To wander through the castle players needed to collect objects that maintained the prince’s energy, keeping him alive. Some objects provided a small amount of energy and others provided 20 times more energy but their collection was prohibited by the kingdom’s laws. At specific moments the player was questioned by different characters (audiences) about whether they had transgressed the law. There were four audiences (mother, brother, soldier, and computer) that varied in how they indicated the probability of either punishment or support. All participants completed the game and eight transgressed, collecting forbidden objects. A larger number of distorted reports, denying transgressions, were made to the audiences who signaled a higher probability of punishment, but a significant proportion of distorted reports were made even to the audiences who presented themselves as supportive and nonpunitive. Because no actual punishment for transgressions occurred in the game, reporting behavior was likely influenced by participants’ histories with audiences functionally similar to those simulated in the game.</p>","PeriodicalId":501490,"journal":{"name":"The Psychological Record","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141166349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
The Psychological Record
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1