首页 > 最新文献

Folia Primatologica最新文献

英文 中文
Variation in Ontogenetic Facial Suture Fusion Patterns in Catarrhines. 卡他鼻炎患者个体发生面部缝线融合模式的变化。
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 Epub Date: 2021-06-23 DOI: 10.1159/000517079
Madeline F Parker, M Kathleen Pitirri, Timothy D Smith, Anne M Burrows, James J Cray

Timing of craniofacial suture fusion is important for the determination of demographics and primate ontogeny. There has been much work concerning the timing of fusion of calvarial sutures over the last century, but little comprehensive work focusing on facial sutures. Here we assess the relationships of facial suture fusion across ontogeny among select catarrhines. Fusion timing patterns for 5 facial sutures were examined in 1,599 crania of Homo, Pan, Gorilla, Pongo, Hylobatidae, Papio, and Macaca. Calvarial volume (early ontogeny) and dental eruption (late ontogeny) were used as indicators of stage of development. General linear models, test for homogeneity of slopes, and ANOVA were used to determine differences in timing of fusion by taxon. For calvarial volume, taxonomic groups segregated by regression slopes, with models for Homo indicating sutural fusion throughout ontogeny, Pongo, Macaca, and Papio representing earlier and more complete suture fusion, and Pan, Gorilla, and Hylobatidae indicating very early facial suture fusion. Similar patterns are observed when dental eruption is used for developmental staging. Only Gorilla and Hylobatidae are observed to, generally, fuse all facial suture sites in adulthood. Finally, Homo appears to be unique in its delay and patency of sutures into late ontogeny. The taxonomic patterns of facial suture closure identified in this study likely reflect important evolutionary shifts in facial growth and development in catarrhines.

颅面缝合融合的时机对于确定人口统计学和灵长类动物的个体发育是重要的。在过去的一个世纪里,已经有很多关于颅缝融合时间的研究,但很少有关于面部缝合的综合研究。在这里,我们评估面部缝线融合在个体发育中的关系。研究了1599例猿人、猿人、大猩猩、猿人、猴猴科、猴猴科和猕猴颅骨的5条面部缝合融合时间模式。颅骨体积(早期个体发育)和牙齿萌出(晚期个体发育)作为发育阶段的指标。采用一般线性模型、坡度均匀性检验和方差分析来确定不同分类单元融合时间的差异。对于颅骨体积,分类类群以回归斜率划分,Homo模型表明整个个体发育过程中缝合融合,Pongo、Macaca和Papio模型表明更早和更完整的缝合融合,Pan、Gorilla和Hylobatidae模型表明非常早的面部缝合融合。类似的模式也可以用牙萌来判断发育分期。只有大猩猩和海蛛科在成年期通常会融合所有面部缝合部位。最后,人属在个体发育晚期的延迟和开放缝合线上似乎是独一无二的。在这项研究中发现的面部缝合闭合的分类模式可能反映了卡他鼻虫面部生长和发育的重要进化转变。
{"title":"Variation in Ontogenetic Facial Suture Fusion Patterns in Catarrhines.","authors":"Madeline F Parker,&nbsp;M Kathleen Pitirri,&nbsp;Timothy D Smith,&nbsp;Anne M Burrows,&nbsp;James J Cray","doi":"10.1159/000517079","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000517079","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Timing of craniofacial suture fusion is important for the determination of demographics and primate ontogeny. There has been much work concerning the timing of fusion of calvarial sutures over the last century, but little comprehensive work focusing on facial sutures. Here we assess the relationships of facial suture fusion across ontogeny among select catarrhines. Fusion timing patterns for 5 facial sutures were examined in 1,599 crania of Homo, Pan, Gorilla, Pongo, Hylobatidae, Papio, and Macaca. Calvarial volume (early ontogeny) and dental eruption (late ontogeny) were used as indicators of stage of development. General linear models, test for homogeneity of slopes, and ANOVA were used to determine differences in timing of fusion by taxon. For calvarial volume, taxonomic groups segregated by regression slopes, with models for Homo indicating sutural fusion throughout ontogeny, Pongo, Macaca, and Papio representing earlier and more complete suture fusion, and Pan, Gorilla, and Hylobatidae indicating very early facial suture fusion. Similar patterns are observed when dental eruption is used for developmental staging. Only Gorilla and Hylobatidae are observed to, generally, fuse all facial suture sites in adulthood. Finally, Homo appears to be unique in its delay and patency of sutures into late ontogeny. The taxonomic patterns of facial suture closure identified in this study likely reflect important evolutionary shifts in facial growth and development in catarrhines.</p>","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000517079","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39119184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Contents Vol. 91, 2020 目录2020年第91卷
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-11-01 DOI: 10.1159/000512886
240 8th European Federation for Primatology Meeting and 2019 Primate Society of Great Britain Winter Meeting (EFP-PSGB) Oxford, UK, September 8–11, 2019
240第八届欧洲灵长类联合会暨2019年英国灵长类学会冬季会议(EFP-PSGB),英国牛津,2019年9月8-11日
{"title":"Contents Vol. 91, 2020","authors":"","doi":"10.1159/000512886","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000512886","url":null,"abstract":"240 8th European Federation for Primatology Meeting and 2019 Primate Society of Great Britain Winter Meeting (EFP-PSGB) Oxford, UK, September 8–11, 2019","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000512886","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42971690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Front & Back Matter 正面和背面
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-11-01 DOI: 10.1159/000513387
{"title":"Front & Back Matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1159/000513387","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000513387","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45350727","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Front & Back Matter 正面和背面
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-09-01 DOI: 10.1159/000511297
{"title":"Front & Back Matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1159/000511297","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000511297","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41263196","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Front & Back Matter 正面和背面事项
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-05-01 DOI: 10.1159/000508775
240 8th European Federation for Primatology Meeting and 2019 Primate Society of Great Britain Winter Meeting (EFP-PSGB) Oxford, UK, September 8–11, 2019
240第八届欧洲灵长类学联合会会议和2019年英国灵长类学会冬季会议(EFP-PSGB),英国牛津,2019年9月8日至11日
{"title":"Front & Back Matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1159/000508775","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000508775","url":null,"abstract":"240 8th European Federation for Primatology Meeting and 2019 Primate Society of Great Britain Winter Meeting (EFP-PSGB) Oxford, UK, September 8–11, 2019","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41427687","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Abstracts of the 7th Iberian Congress of Primatology. 第七届伊比利亚灵长类动物学大会摘要。
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-04-15 DOI: 10.1159/000506768
Caterina Casanova
{"title":"Abstracts of the 7th Iberian Congress of Primatology.","authors":"Caterina Casanova","doi":"10.1159/000506768","DOIUrl":"10.1159/000506768","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37837726","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Home Range Size and Seasonal Variation in Habitat Use of Aye-Ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis) in Torotorofotsy, Madagascar. 在马达加斯加Torotorofotsy地区,斑叶蝉(Daubentonia Madagascar)的栖息地范围大小和季节变化。
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 Epub Date: 2020-07-14 DOI: 10.1159/000508620
Timothy M Sefczek, Ryan A Hagenson, Doménico Roger Randimbiharinirina, Jeannin Nicolas Rakotondrazandry, Edward E Louis

Madagascar's dramatic climatic fluctuations mean most lemurs adjust behaviors seasonally as resource availability fluctuates. Many lemurs will adopt one of two strategies, a resource maximizer or an area minimizer, when adjusting to seasonal shifts in resource availability. However, it is unknown if and how aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis) ranging behavior is influenced by seasonality. We explored whether habitat use changed seasonally. We followed two aye-ayes, an adult male and an adult female, in the undisturbed forest of Torotorofotsy, Madagascar, from April 2012 to December 2017. We used instantaneous focal-animal sampling to collect behavioral data every 5 min and GPS locations every 20 min. We used the minimum convex polygon (MCP) to determine overall home range, and the Brownian bridge movement model (BBMM) to estimate overall and seasonal home range of the female aye-aye from November 2014 to October 2017. We used Wilcoxon signed-rank tests to determine whether there were significant differences in home range sizes between seasons across years and to examine whether there were seasonal differences in height of invertebrate foraging, generalized linear models to assess seasonal differences in travel rates and nesting locations, and χ2 tests to determine whether there were differences in forest strata use when foraging on invertebrates. The male's MCP home range was 2,586 ha, and the female's MCP home range was 765 ha. The seasonal BBMM for the female varied between 443.6 and 1,010.0 ha, though infant rearing appears to have influenced these values. There were no significant differences in seasonal home range, travel rates, nesting locations, or height of invertebrate feeding. However, canopy level invertebrate foraging occurred more often than understory or ground levels. It appears aye-ayes in this undisturbed forest were not influenced by seasonal shifts and had larger home ranges than any previously reported. These findings may indicate that aye-ayes in an undisturbed forest are resource maximizers, closely linked to invertebrate assemblages.

马达加斯加剧烈的气候波动意味着大多数狐猴会随着资源可用性的波动而季节性地调整行为。在适应资源可用性的季节性变化时,许多狐猴会采取两种策略中的一种,即资源最大化或面积最小化。然而,目前尚不清楚是否以及如何影响aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascar)的范围行为受季节影响。我们探讨了生境利用是否随季节变化。从2012年4月到2017年12月,我们在马达加斯加Torotorofotsy的未受干扰的森林中跟踪了两只眼睛,一只成年雄性和一只成年雌性。在2014年11月至2017年10月期间,我们利用最小凸多边形(MCP)和布朗桥运动模型(BBMM)来估算雌性野叶猴的总体和季节活动范围,并利用瞬时焦点动物采样每5分钟收集一次行为数据,每20分钟收集一次GPS定位。我们使用Wilcoxon符号秩检验来确定不同季节之间的栖息地大小是否存在显著差异,并检验无脊椎动物觅食高度是否存在季节性差异,使用广义线性模型来评估旅行率和筑巢地点的季节性差异,并使用χ2检验来确定无脊椎动物觅食时使用的森林地层是否存在差异。雄性MCP的家庭范围为2586公顷,雌性MCP的家庭范围为765公顷。雌性的季节性BBMM在443.6和1010.0公顷之间变化,尽管婴儿养育似乎影响了这些值。在季节范围、旅行率、筑巢地点或无脊椎动物取食的高度上没有显著差异。然而,树冠层的无脊椎动物觅食比林下或地面更频繁。在这片未受干扰的森林里,似乎没有受到季节变化的影响,而且它们的活动范围比以前报道的都要大。这些发现可能表明,在未受干扰的森林中,“眼-眼”是资源最大化者,与无脊椎动物群落密切相关。
{"title":"Home Range Size and Seasonal Variation in Habitat Use of Aye-Ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis) in Torotorofotsy, Madagascar.","authors":"Timothy M Sefczek,&nbsp;Ryan A Hagenson,&nbsp;Doménico Roger Randimbiharinirina,&nbsp;Jeannin Nicolas Rakotondrazandry,&nbsp;Edward E Louis","doi":"10.1159/000508620","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000508620","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Madagascar's dramatic climatic fluctuations mean most lemurs adjust behaviors seasonally as resource availability fluctuates. Many lemurs will adopt one of two strategies, a resource maximizer or an area minimizer, when adjusting to seasonal shifts in resource availability. However, it is unknown if and how aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis) ranging behavior is influenced by seasonality. We explored whether habitat use changed seasonally. We followed two aye-ayes, an adult male and an adult female, in the undisturbed forest of Torotorofotsy, Madagascar, from April 2012 to December 2017. We used instantaneous focal-animal sampling to collect behavioral data every 5 min and GPS locations every 20 min. We used the minimum convex polygon (MCP) to determine overall home range, and the Brownian bridge movement model (BBMM) to estimate overall and seasonal home range of the female aye-aye from November 2014 to October 2017. We used Wilcoxon signed-rank tests to determine whether there were significant differences in home range sizes between seasons across years and to examine whether there were seasonal differences in height of invertebrate foraging, generalized linear models to assess seasonal differences in travel rates and nesting locations, and χ2 tests to determine whether there were differences in forest strata use when foraging on invertebrates. The male's MCP home range was 2,586 ha, and the female's MCP home range was 765 ha. The seasonal BBMM for the female varied between 443.6 and 1,010.0 ha, though infant rearing appears to have influenced these values. There were no significant differences in seasonal home range, travel rates, nesting locations, or height of invertebrate feeding. However, canopy level invertebrate foraging occurred more often than understory or ground levels. It appears aye-ayes in this undisturbed forest were not influenced by seasonal shifts and had larger home ranges than any previously reported. These findings may indicate that aye-ayes in an undisturbed forest are resource maximizers, closely linked to invertebrate assemblages.</p>","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000508620","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38158210","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Daily Distance Traveled Is Associated with Greater Brain Size in Primates. 在灵长类动物中,每天行走的距离与更大的大脑大小有关。
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 Epub Date: 2020-10-27 DOI: 10.1159/000510782
Marco Vidal-Cordasco, Lucía Rodríguez-González, Olalla Prado-Nóvoa, Guillermo Zorrilla-Revilla, Mario Modesto-Mata

Explanations for the brain size increments through primate and, particularly, human evolution are numerous. Commonly, these hypotheses rely on the influence that behavioral and ecological variables have on brain size in extant primates, such as diet quality, social group size, or home range (HR) area. However, HR area does not reflect the time spent moving. As such, it has not been properly addressed whether the effort involved in movement could have affected brain size evolution in primates. This study aimed to test the influence of daily movement on primates' brain sizes, controlling for these other behavioral and ecological factors. We used a large comparative dataset of extant primate species and phylogenetic comparative methods. Our results show a significant correlation between daily movement and brain mass, which is not explained by the influence of diet, social group size, HR, or body mass. Hence, from an evolutionary timescale, a longer daily movement distance is not a constraining factor for the energetic investment in a larger brain. On the contrary, increased mobility could have contributed to brain mass incrementations through evolution.

对于灵长类动物,尤其是人类进化过程中大脑大小增加的解释有很多。通常,这些假设依赖于现存灵长类动物的行为和生态变量对大脑大小的影响,如饮食质量、社会群体规模或家庭范围(HR)区域。然而,人力资源面积并不能反映移动所花费的时间。因此,在灵长类动物中,运动所涉及的努力是否会影响大脑大小的进化还没有得到适当的解决。这项研究旨在测试日常运动对灵长类动物大脑大小的影响,控制这些其他行为和生态因素。我们使用了现存灵长类物种的大型比较数据集和系统发育比较方法。我们的研究结果显示,日常运动与脑质量之间存在显著相关性,这不能用饮食、社会群体规模、HR或体重的影响来解释。因此,从进化的时间尺度来看,较长的日常运动距离并不是对更大大脑的能量投入的限制因素。相反,通过进化,活动能力的增加可能有助于大脑质量的增加。
{"title":"Daily Distance Traveled Is Associated with Greater Brain Size in Primates.","authors":"Marco Vidal-Cordasco,&nbsp;Lucía Rodríguez-González,&nbsp;Olalla Prado-Nóvoa,&nbsp;Guillermo Zorrilla-Revilla,&nbsp;Mario Modesto-Mata","doi":"10.1159/000510782","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000510782","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Explanations for the brain size increments through primate and, particularly, human evolution are numerous. Commonly, these hypotheses rely on the influence that behavioral and ecological variables have on brain size in extant primates, such as diet quality, social group size, or home range (HR) area. However, HR area does not reflect the time spent moving. As such, it has not been properly addressed whether the effort involved in movement could have affected brain size evolution in primates. This study aimed to test the influence of daily movement on primates' brain sizes, controlling for these other behavioral and ecological factors. We used a large comparative dataset of extant primate species and phylogenetic comparative methods. Our results show a significant correlation between daily movement and brain mass, which is not explained by the influence of diet, social group size, HR, or body mass. Hence, from an evolutionary timescale, a longer daily movement distance is not a constraining factor for the energetic investment in a larger brain. On the contrary, increased mobility could have contributed to brain mass incrementations through evolution.</p>","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000510782","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38636139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Brown Mouse Lemurs (Microcebus rufus) May Lack Opportunities to Learn about Predator Calls. 褐鼠狐猴(Microcebus rufus)可能缺乏学习捕食者叫声的机会。
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 Epub Date: 2020-03-06 DOI: 10.1159/000505953
Anja Martha Deppe

Diurnal primates commonly modify their behaviour in response to predator vocalizations, but little is known about how nocturnal primates perceive predator calls. Mouse lemurs do not innately perceive predator calls as dangerous but may learn to do so in the wild. I hypothesized that wild brown mouse lemurs (Microcebus rufus) in Ranomafana National Park, Madagascar, differentiate between predator and non-predator vocalizations and exposed 29 adult subjects to calls of 3 sympatric predators (goshawk, fossa and owl), 3 sympatric non-predators and 4 controls. Brown mouse lemurs did not differentiate among stimuli, and there was no evidence that any recording was associated with danger, as neither activity nor spatial preferences changed in response to a stimulus, nor did subjects exhibit obvious behaviours indicative of danger such as flight or position on the branches. The failure to perceive predator calls as dangerous is common among small nocturnal solitary prey species and could be explained by a lack of learning opportunities. In social species acquired behaviours can spread quickly by social learning. Solitary species like mouse lemurs have to individually learn to associate predator calls with danger which is dependent on encounter rates. Unlike diurnal prey mouse lemurs lack the opportunity to observe diurnal raptors and associate their calls with the bird, and owls are proficient predators, which are difficult to observe in a rain forest where they hunt cryptically from perches. While sometimes nocturnal, the fossa is solitary and largely silent, and is also particularly rare at my study site. Cryptic, rare or non-vocal predators provide prey with few opportunities to associate their calls with danger. Mouse lemurs are subject to high predation rates by a wide range of predators and need to balance the costs and benefits of antipredation behaviours. Their cryptic lifestyle and preference for dense vegetation lowers the likelihood of being detected by predators, and they utilize visual and olfactory predator cues to make decisions about risk. Furthermore, their high reproductive output compensates for the high predation rates.

白天活动的灵长类动物通常会根据捕食者的叫声改变它们的行为,但人们对夜间活动的灵长类动物如何感知捕食者的叫声知之甚少。鼠狐猴并不天生认为捕食者的叫声是危险的,但可能在野外学会了这样做。我假设马达加斯加Ranomafana国家公园的野生褐鼠狐猴(Microcebus rufus)能够区分捕食者和非捕食者的声音,并将29名成年受试者暴露在3种同域捕食者(鹰、fossa和猫头鹰)、3种同域非捕食者和4种对照的叫声中。棕鼠狐猴不能区分不同的刺激,也没有证据表明任何记录与危险有关,因为活动和空间偏好都不会因刺激而改变,受试者也没有表现出明显的危险行为,比如逃跑或在树枝上的位置。在夜间独居的小型猎物中,无法感知捕食者的危险叫声是很常见的,这可以解释为缺乏学习机会。在群居物种中,后天行为可以通过社会学习迅速传播。像鼠狐猴这样的独居物种必须单独学习将捕食者的叫声与危险联系起来,这取决于相遇率。与白天捕食的老鼠不同,狐猴没有机会观察白天活动的猛禽,并将它们的叫声与鸟类联系起来,猫头鹰是熟练的捕食者,在雨林中很难观察到它们,因为它们在栖息之处神秘地捕猎。虽然有时是夜间活动,但窝窝是孤独的,而且基本上是沉默的,在我的研究地点也特别罕见。隐蔽的、罕见的或不发声的捕食者给猎物提供了很少的机会将它们的叫声与危险联系起来。鼠狐猴受到各种捕食者的高捕食率,需要平衡反捕食行为的成本和收益。它们隐秘的生活方式和对茂密植被的偏好降低了被捕食者发现的可能性,它们利用捕食者的视觉和嗅觉线索来做出风险决定。此外,它们的高繁殖产量弥补了高捕食率。
{"title":"Brown Mouse Lemurs (Microcebus rufus) May Lack Opportunities to Learn about Predator Calls.","authors":"Anja Martha Deppe","doi":"10.1159/000505953","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000505953","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Diurnal primates commonly modify their behaviour in response to predator vocalizations, but little is known about how nocturnal primates perceive predator calls. Mouse lemurs do not innately perceive predator calls as dangerous but may learn to do so in the wild. I hypothesized that wild brown mouse lemurs (Microcebus rufus) in Ranomafana National Park, Madagascar, differentiate between predator and non-predator vocalizations and exposed 29 adult subjects to calls of 3 sympatric predators (goshawk, fossa and owl), 3 sympatric non-predators and 4 controls. Brown mouse lemurs did not differentiate among stimuli, and there was no evidence that any recording was associated with danger, as neither activity nor spatial preferences changed in response to a stimulus, nor did subjects exhibit obvious behaviours indicative of danger such as flight or position on the branches. The failure to perceive predator calls as dangerous is common among small nocturnal solitary prey species and could be explained by a lack of learning opportunities. In social species acquired behaviours can spread quickly by social learning. Solitary species like mouse lemurs have to individually learn to associate predator calls with danger which is dependent on encounter rates. Unlike diurnal prey mouse lemurs lack the opportunity to observe diurnal raptors and associate their calls with the bird, and owls are proficient predators, which are difficult to observe in a rain forest where they hunt cryptically from perches. While sometimes nocturnal, the fossa is solitary and largely silent, and is also particularly rare at my study site. Cryptic, rare or non-vocal predators provide prey with few opportunities to associate their calls with danger. Mouse lemurs are subject to high predation rates by a wide range of predators and need to balance the costs and benefits of antipredation behaviours. Their cryptic lifestyle and preference for dense vegetation lowers the likelihood of being detected by predators, and they utilize visual and olfactory predator cues to make decisions about risk. Furthermore, their high reproductive output compensates for the high predation rates.</p>","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000505953","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37716291","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Prolonged Torpor in Goodman's Mouse Lemur (Microcebus lehilahytsara) from the High-Altitude Forest of Tsinjoarivo, Central-Eastern Madagascar. 来自马达加斯加中东部Tsinjoarivo高海拔森林的古德曼鼠狐猴(Microcebus lehilahytsara)的长时间冬眠。
IF 1.9 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 Epub Date: 2020-07-30 DOI: 10.1159/000509102
Jean-Basile Andriambeloson, Lydia K Greene, Marina B Blanco

The nocturnal mouse and dwarf lemurs from Madagascar are known to express heterothermy. Whereas dwarf lemurs (Cheirogaleus) are obligate hibernators, mouse lemurs (Microcebus) can express a great range of heterothermic responses, including daily torpor, prolonged torpor or hibernation, depending on the species, population or individual. Although there is indirect evidence of heterothermy in a handful of mouse lemur species, direct physiological confirmation is currently limited to four: Microcebus berthae(dry forest), M. ravelobensis(dry forest), M. griseorufus(spiny forest), M. murinus(dry forest/littoral forest). We studied Goodman's mouse lemurs (M. lehilahytsara) at the high-altitude rain forest of Tsinjoarivo, central-eastern Madagascar. We captured a total of 45 mouse lemurs during pre- and post-torpor seasons. We recorded body mass and measured tail base circumference, which was used as proxy for fattening. Moreover, using telemetry, we obtained sporadic skin temperature data from a mouse lemur female between March and June. From June 9 to 16, we performed more intensive data sampling and thereby confirmed the expression of prolonged torpor in this female, when skin temperatures consistently displayed values below 25°C for more than 24 h. We documented 3 torpor bouts, 2 of which were interrupted by naturally occurring arousals. Torpor bout duration ranged from approximately 47 to maximally about 69 h. We found similarities between the Goodman's mouse lemur torpor profile and those known from other species, including the fact that only a portion of any mouse lemur population deposits fat stores and undergoes torpor in a given year. The variable expression of heterothermy in Goodman's mouse lemurs contrasts with obligatory hibernation displayed by two sympatric species of dwarf lemurs, suggesting cheirogaleids use a range of metabolic strategies to cope with seasonality and cold environments.

来自马达加斯加的夜行鼠和矮狐猴被认为表达异源性。矮狐猴(Cheirogaleus)是专性冬眠动物,而鼠狐猴(Microcebus)可以表现出很大范围的异温反应,包括日常冬眠、长时间冬眠或冬眠,这取决于物种、种群或个体。虽然在少数鼠狐猴物种中有异温性的间接证据,但直接的生理证实目前仅限于四种:Microcebus berthae(干林),M. ravelobensis(干林),M. griseorufus(刺林),M. murinus(干林/沿海林)。我们在马达加斯加中东部Tsinjoarivo的高海拔雨林中研究了古德曼鼠狐猴(M. lehilahytsara)。我们在冬眠前和冬眠后共捕获了45只鼠狐猴。我们记录了体重,并测量了尾底围,这是肥胖的代表。此外,利用遥测技术,我们获得了3月至6月间雌性鼠狐猴的零星皮肤温度数据。从6月9日至16日,我们进行了更密集的数据采集,从而证实了这只雌性动物的长时间麻木表现,当皮肤温度持续低于25°C超过24小时时。我们记录了3次麻木发作,其中2次被自然发生的唤醒打断。冬眠的持续时间从大约47小时到最长约69小时不等。我们发现古德曼鼠狐猴的冬眠情况与其他物种的相似之处,包括在给定的一年中,任何鼠狐猴种群中只有一部分储存脂肪并经历冬眠。古德曼鼠狐猴异温性的可变表达与两种同域侏儒狐猴的强制性冬眠形成对比,表明狐猴使用一系列代谢策略来应对季节性和寒冷环境。
{"title":"Prolonged Torpor in Goodman's Mouse Lemur (Microcebus lehilahytsara) from the High-Altitude Forest of Tsinjoarivo, Central-Eastern Madagascar.","authors":"Jean-Basile Andriambeloson,&nbsp;Lydia K Greene,&nbsp;Marina B Blanco","doi":"10.1159/000509102","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1159/000509102","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The nocturnal mouse and dwarf lemurs from Madagascar are known to express heterothermy. Whereas dwarf lemurs (Cheirogaleus) are obligate hibernators, mouse lemurs (Microcebus) can express a great range of heterothermic responses, including daily torpor, prolonged torpor or hibernation, depending on the species, population or individual. Although there is indirect evidence of heterothermy in a handful of mouse lemur species, direct physiological confirmation is currently limited to four: Microcebus berthae(dry forest), M. ravelobensis(dry forest), M. griseorufus(spiny forest), M. murinus(dry forest/littoral forest). We studied Goodman's mouse lemurs (M. lehilahytsara) at the high-altitude rain forest of Tsinjoarivo, central-eastern Madagascar. We captured a total of 45 mouse lemurs during pre- and post-torpor seasons. We recorded body mass and measured tail base circumference, which was used as proxy for fattening. Moreover, using telemetry, we obtained sporadic skin temperature data from a mouse lemur female between March and June. From June 9 to 16, we performed more intensive data sampling and thereby confirmed the expression of prolonged torpor in this female, when skin temperatures consistently displayed values below 25°C for more than 24 h. We documented 3 torpor bouts, 2 of which were interrupted by naturally occurring arousals. Torpor bout duration ranged from approximately 47 to maximally about 69 h. We found similarities between the Goodman's mouse lemur torpor profile and those known from other species, including the fact that only a portion of any mouse lemur population deposits fat stores and undergoes torpor in a given year. The variable expression of heterothermy in Goodman's mouse lemurs contrasts with obligatory hibernation displayed by two sympatric species of dwarf lemurs, suggesting cheirogaleids use a range of metabolic strategies to cope with seasonality and cold environments.</p>","PeriodicalId":50437,"journal":{"name":"Folia Primatologica","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1159/000509102","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"38217992","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
期刊
Folia Primatologica
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1