首页 > 最新文献

Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
A systematic review of methodologies for studying behavioral imprinting 行为印记研究方法的系统回顾
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-17 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13416
Mac L. Chamberlain, Alex Kacelnik, Autumn Bush, Mark E. Hauber

Behavioral imprinting is a learning phenomenon by which animals acquire preferences for stimuli through perceptual exposure during critical periods, without substantial external reinforcement. Since being acknowledged in 1516 by Sir Thomas More in artificially incubated domestic chickens, imprinting has been reported in diverse species, across various sensory modalities, and during different life-history stages. Due to this diversity, imprinting research uses highly varied methodologies, with distinctive differences between the methods employed for different types of imprinting. We systematically review relevant literature, identifying and describing the range of methodologies used to study imprinting across taxa and modalities. After compiling a representative dataset of 192 behavioral imprinting-focused experiments, we categorize studies by imprinting sensory modality, focal species, ontogenetic stage addressed, and methods applied for both exposure and testing. The majority of studies in the sample focus on filial imprinting in precocial birds but nonfilial types, such as sexual or home range imprinting in altricial and non-avian species, are also present, albeit at far lower proportions. Filial imprinting is studied across different sensory modalities, mostly through applying artificial stimuli, but nonfilial imprinting studies mainly use live animals as stimuli, without isolating the relevant sensory modalities. Most studies of filial imprinting measure preference by spatial proximity, following response, or the suppression of a fear response, whereas most studies of sexual imprinting employ the attempt frequency of sexual behaviors. Finally, we analyze the relative frequencies of methodological approaches in each imprinting category, to highlight potential biases due to uneven research effort rather than intrinsic biology. The patterns and biases in imprinting methodologies that we uncover hamper attempts to establish whether different forms of imprinting share mechanistic foundations, including whether imprinting constitutes a biologically meaningful learning category.

行为印记是一种学习现象,动物在关键时期通过感知暴露获得对刺激的偏好,而不需要大量的外部强化。自1516年托马斯·莫尔爵士在人工饲养的家鸡中发现印记以来,已有报道称,在不同的物种中,在不同的感觉模式中,在不同的生活史阶段,都有印记。由于这种多样性,印迹研究使用高度多样化的方法,不同类型的印迹所采用的方法之间存在显著差异。我们系统地回顾了相关文献,确定和描述了用于研究跨分类群和模态印记的方法范围。在编译了192个以行为印记为重点的实验的代表性数据集后,我们根据印记的感觉方式、焦点物种、个体发生阶段以及暴露和测试的方法对研究进行了分类。样本中的大多数研究集中在早熟鸟类的孝子印记上,但非孝子类型,如晚育和非鸟类物种的性或家庭范围印记,也存在,尽管比例要低得多。“子代印记”的研究主要是通过人工刺激进行的,而“非子代印记”的研究主要是用活体动物作为刺激,没有隔离相关的感觉模态。大多数关于孝顺印记的研究通过空间接近、跟随反应或恐惧反应的抑制来衡量偏好,而大多数关于性印记的研究则采用性行为的尝试频率。最后,我们分析了每个印记类别中方法方法的相对频率,以突出由于研究努力不均匀而不是内在生物学而导致的潜在偏差。我们发现的印迹方法中的模式和偏见阻碍了建立不同形式的印迹是否共享机制基础的尝试,包括印迹是否构成生物学上有意义的学习类别。
{"title":"A systematic review of methodologies for studying behavioral imprinting","authors":"Mac L. Chamberlain,&nbsp;Alex Kacelnik,&nbsp;Autumn Bush,&nbsp;Mark E. Hauber","doi":"10.1111/eth.13416","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13416","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Behavioral imprinting is a learning phenomenon by which animals acquire preferences for stimuli through perceptual exposure during critical periods, without substantial external reinforcement. Since being acknowledged in 1516 by Sir Thomas More in artificially incubated domestic chickens, imprinting has been reported in diverse species, across various sensory modalities, and during different life-history stages. Due to this diversity, imprinting research uses highly varied methodologies, with distinctive differences between the methods employed for different types of imprinting. We systematically review relevant literature, identifying and describing the range of methodologies used to study imprinting across taxa and modalities. After compiling a representative dataset of 192 behavioral imprinting-focused experiments, we categorize studies by imprinting sensory modality, focal species, ontogenetic stage addressed, and methods applied for both exposure and testing. The majority of studies in the sample focus on filial imprinting in precocial birds but nonfilial types, such as sexual or home range imprinting in altricial and non-avian species, are also present, albeit at far lower proportions. Filial imprinting is studied across different sensory modalities, mostly through applying artificial stimuli, but nonfilial imprinting studies mainly use live animals as stimuli, without isolating the relevant sensory modalities. Most studies of filial imprinting measure preference by spatial proximity, following response, or the suppression of a fear response, whereas most studies of sexual imprinting employ the attempt frequency of sexual behaviors. Finally, we analyze the relative frequencies of methodological approaches in each imprinting category, to highlight potential biases due to uneven research effort rather than intrinsic biology. The patterns and biases in imprinting methodologies that we uncover hamper attempts to establish whether different forms of imprinting share mechanistic foundations, including whether imprinting constitutes a biologically meaningful learning category.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136032683","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Guiana dolphins use mangrove margins as a natural barrier to chase fish prey 圭亚那海豚利用红树林边缘作为天然屏障追逐鱼类猎物
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-14 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13411
Julia C. Pierry, Maria E. Morete, Emygdio L. A. Monteiro-Filho, Clarissa R. Teixeira

The behavioral plasticity among and within cetacean species can be driven by their prey sources, local adaptations to environmental features, and/or interactions with human activities. One of the tactics displayed by cetaceans is the barrier feeding, in which individuals or groups herd fish schools against natural and non-natural barriers to restrict movements of their prey. Coastal odontocetes, for example, are known for using shorelines and underwater slopes to trap their prey. Here, we documented two distinct populations of Guiana dolphin using the mangrove margins as a natural barrier to herd and capture fish. The observations took place in the Cananéia estuary, São Paulo State, southeastern Brazil, and in the Paranaguá estuary, Paraná State, southern Brazil. A total of 63 dolphins over eight encounters were observed performing six different events using mangroves as a natural barrier, summing both study areas. These findings not only expand our understanding of the behavior plasticity of the species but also call attention to the importance of the mangrove ecosystem directly to Guiana dolphins and indirectly to the components of their food web.

鲸类动物之间和物种内部的行为可塑性受其猎物来源、对环境特征的局部适应以及与人类活动的相互作用等因素的驱动。鲸类动物表现出的一种策略是屏障喂养,在这种策略中,个体或群体将鱼群驱赶到自然和非自然的屏障上,以限制猎物的活动。例如,海岸齿螈以利用海岸线和水下斜坡来捕获猎物而闻名。在这里,我们记录了两个截然不同的圭亚那海豚种群,它们利用红树林边缘作为放牧和捕获鱼类的天然屏障。观测在巴西东南部圣保罗州的canan河口和巴西南部帕拉纳州的帕拉纳瓜河口进行。共观察到63只海豚在8次相遇中以红树林作为自然屏障进行了6次不同的活动,将两个研究区域相加。这些发现不仅扩大了我们对物种行为可塑性的理解,而且还引起了人们对红树林生态系统对圭亚那海豚的重要性的关注,并间接地引起了对它们食物网组成部分的关注。
{"title":"Guiana dolphins use mangrove margins as a natural barrier to chase fish prey","authors":"Julia C. Pierry,&nbsp;Maria E. Morete,&nbsp;Emygdio L. A. Monteiro-Filho,&nbsp;Clarissa R. Teixeira","doi":"10.1111/eth.13411","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13411","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The behavioral plasticity among and within cetacean species can be driven by their prey sources, local adaptations to environmental features, and/or interactions with human activities. One of the tactics displayed by cetaceans is the barrier feeding, in which individuals or groups herd fish schools against natural and non-natural barriers to restrict movements of their prey. Coastal odontocetes, for example, are known for using shorelines and underwater slopes to trap their prey. Here, we documented two distinct populations of Guiana dolphin using the mangrove margins as a natural barrier to herd and capture fish. The observations took place in the Cananéia estuary, São Paulo State, southeastern Brazil, and in the Paranaguá estuary, Paraná State, southern Brazil. A total of 63 dolphins over eight encounters were observed performing six different events using mangroves as a natural barrier, summing both study areas. These findings not only expand our understanding of the behavior plasticity of the species but also call attention to the importance of the mangrove ecosystem directly to Guiana dolphins and indirectly to the components of their food web.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135803749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shaped by you: The effect of social partner on cortisol and behavior during adolescence in a female rodent 由你塑造社会伙伴对雌性啮齿动物青春期皮质醇和行为的影响
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-13 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13414
Taylor L. Rystrom, Yvonne Wesseler, S. Helene Richter, Norbert Sachser, Sylvia Kaiser

Adolescence is a sensitive phase when cues from the social environment can shape consistent individual differences in behavioral and hormonal profiles, but the effect of the social partner on these phenotypes is not well-understood, especially for females. We therefore aimed to investigate whether the age class and sex of the housing partner affect social behavior and cortisol concentrations in female guinea pigs during adolescence and estimate the repeatability of cortisol concentrations in this life stage. Female guinea pigs were housed upon weaning for 4 weeks with either an adult female, adolescent female, or adolescent male. Social behavior was observed in the home enclosure and a preference test and social interaction test were carried out at the end of the experiment. Cortisol concentrations (baseline and response to a challenge) were measured prior to the start of the experiment and 2 and 4 weeks after pair formation. We hypothesized that the social partner would affect behavior and cortisol concentrations. We found that females housed with an adult female were less aggressive and more consistently displaced by their housing partner than females housed with an adolescent female. Sex of the housing partner did not affect focal female behavior. Regarding hormones, baseline cortisol concentration was neither significantly repeatable nor significantly affected by the housing partner. However, cortisol responsiveness was highly repeatable both 1 h and 2 h after the onset of a challenge. The age class of the housing partner affected cortisol responsiveness, with higher cortisol concentrations measured at 1 h in females housed with an adult female. These results indicate that adolescent female guinea pigs exhibit consistent individual differences in cortisol responsiveness while retaining plasticity to adjust to cues from the social environment. Taken together, we show in a female rodent that the social environment can significantly shape behavioral and endocrine parameters during adolescence.

青春期是一个敏感的阶段,来自社会环境的线索可以塑造行为和激素特征上一致的个体差异,但社会伴侣对这些表型的影响尚不清楚,特别是对女性。因此,我们旨在调查同居伴侣的年龄阶层和性别是否会影响青春期雌性豚鼠的社会行为和皮质醇浓度,并估计皮质醇浓度在这一生命阶段的可重复性。雌性豚鼠在断奶后与成年雌性、青春期雌性或青春期雄性饲养4周。在家庭围栏内观察社会行为,并在实验结束时进行偏好测试和社会互动测试。皮质醇浓度(基线和对挑战的反应)在实验开始前和配对形成后2周和4周进行测量。我们假设社会伙伴会影响行为和皮质醇浓度。我们发现,与与青春期女性住在一起的女性相比,与成年女性住在一起的女性攻击性更低,而且更容易被伴侣赶走。同居伴侣的性别不影响焦点女性的行为。在激素方面,基线皮质醇浓度既不显着重复也不显着受住房伴侣的影响。然而,皮质醇反应在刺激开始后1小时和2小时都是高度可重复的。同居伴侣的年龄等级影响皮质醇反应,与成年女性同居的女性在1小时内测量到的皮质醇浓度较高。这些结果表明,青春期雌性豚鼠在皮质醇反应方面表现出一致的个体差异,同时保留了适应社会环境线索的可塑性。综上所述,我们在一只雌性啮齿动物身上发现,社会环境可以显著地塑造青春期的行为和内分泌参数。
{"title":"Shaped by you: The effect of social partner on cortisol and behavior during adolescence in a female rodent","authors":"Taylor L. Rystrom,&nbsp;Yvonne Wesseler,&nbsp;S. Helene Richter,&nbsp;Norbert Sachser,&nbsp;Sylvia Kaiser","doi":"10.1111/eth.13414","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13414","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Adolescence is a sensitive phase when cues from the social environment can shape consistent individual differences in behavioral and hormonal profiles, but the effect of the social partner on these phenotypes is not well-understood, especially for females. We therefore aimed to investigate whether the age class and sex of the housing partner affect social behavior and cortisol concentrations in female guinea pigs during adolescence and estimate the repeatability of cortisol concentrations in this life stage. Female guinea pigs were housed upon weaning for 4 weeks with either an adult female, adolescent female, or adolescent male. Social behavior was observed in the home enclosure and a preference test and social interaction test were carried out at the end of the experiment. Cortisol concentrations (baseline and response to a challenge) were measured prior to the start of the experiment and 2 and 4 weeks after pair formation. We hypothesized that the social partner would affect behavior and cortisol concentrations. We found that females housed with an adult female were less aggressive and more consistently displaced by their housing partner than females housed with an adolescent female. Sex of the housing partner did not affect focal female behavior. Regarding hormones, baseline cortisol concentration was neither significantly repeatable nor significantly affected by the housing partner. However, cortisol responsiveness was highly repeatable both 1 h and 2 h after the onset of a challenge. The age class of the housing partner affected cortisol responsiveness, with higher cortisol concentrations measured at 1 h in females housed with an adult female. These results indicate that adolescent female guinea pigs exhibit consistent individual differences in cortisol responsiveness while retaining plasticity to adjust to cues from the social environment. Taken together, we show in a female rodent that the social environment can significantly shape behavioral and endocrine parameters during adolescence.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13414","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135918930","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Phenotype predicts interspecific dominance hierarchies in a cloud-forest hummingbird guild 表型可预测云林蜂鸟联盟中的种间优势等级制度
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-13 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13410
Facundo Fernandez-Duque, Eliot T. Miller, Matias Fernandez-Duque, Jay Falk, Gabriela Venable, Sophie Rabinowicz, C. Dustin Becker, Mark E. Hauber

Competition over resources often leads to intra- and interspecific interactions, which can be detrimental to the individuals involved. Thus, natural selection should favor communication systems that reliably convey information regarding the relative competitive abilities of an individual, reducing the need for physically damaging confrontation. Body size, sex, age, relatedness, and ornamentation are important factors determining dominance across diverse taxa in intraspecific interactions. These traits, when perceptible, may serve as signals across species in guilds that have frequent interspecific interactions. Hummingbirds provide a tractable system to study such community dynamics due to their high frequency of interactions, variable ornamentation, diverse body sizes, fast metabolism, and large overlap in resource utilization. Even in this system, potential interactions between morphology and coloration are rarely accounted for together when analyzing dominance between species. We take a novel approach to understanding interspecific dominance by assessing behavior, morphology, and coloration across different types of behavioral interactions. Across 11 tropical montane hummingbird species, we find that dominance is predicted by wing size and some metrics of plumage coloration. However, the biological significance of these factors varies between the different dominance behaviors performed. These results inform our understanding of interspecific signaling and its role in the evolution of intraguild communication and resource competition.

对资源的竞争常常导致种内和种间的相互作用,这对相关个体是有害的。因此,自然选择应该倾向于可靠地传达有关个体相对竞争能力的信息的通信系统,从而减少对身体有害的对抗的需要。体型、性别、年龄、亲缘关系和纹饰是决定不同分类群在种内相互作用中优势的重要因素。这些特征,当被察觉时,可能作为物种之间的信号,在有频繁的种间互动的行会中。蜂鸟因其频繁的相互作用、多变的纹饰、多样的体型、快速的新陈代谢和资源利用上的大重叠,为研究这种群落动态提供了一个易于处理的系统。即使在这个系统中,当分析物种之间的优势时,形态和颜色之间的潜在相互作用也很少被考虑在一起。我们采取了一种新的方法,通过评估不同类型的行为相互作用的行为,形态和颜色来理解种间优势。在11种热带山地蜂鸟中,我们发现优势是通过翅膀大小和羽毛颜色的一些指标来预测的。然而,这些因素的生物学意义因不同的优势行为而异。这些结果为我们了解种间信号及其在种群内通信和资源竞争进化中的作用提供了依据。
{"title":"Phenotype predicts interspecific dominance hierarchies in a cloud-forest hummingbird guild","authors":"Facundo Fernandez-Duque,&nbsp;Eliot T. Miller,&nbsp;Matias Fernandez-Duque,&nbsp;Jay Falk,&nbsp;Gabriela Venable,&nbsp;Sophie Rabinowicz,&nbsp;C. Dustin Becker,&nbsp;Mark E. Hauber","doi":"10.1111/eth.13410","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13410","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Competition over resources often leads to intra- and interspecific interactions, which can be detrimental to the individuals involved. Thus, natural selection should favor communication systems that reliably convey information regarding the relative competitive abilities of an individual, reducing the need for physically damaging confrontation. Body size, sex, age, relatedness, and ornamentation are important factors determining dominance across diverse taxa in intraspecific interactions. These traits, when perceptible, may serve as signals across species in guilds that have frequent interspecific interactions. Hummingbirds provide a tractable system to study such community dynamics due to their high frequency of interactions, variable ornamentation, diverse body sizes, fast metabolism, and large overlap in resource utilization. Even in this system, potential interactions between morphology and coloration are rarely accounted for together when analyzing dominance between species. We take a novel approach to understanding interspecific dominance by assessing behavior, morphology, and coloration across different types of behavioral interactions. Across 11 tropical montane hummingbird species, we find that dominance is predicted by wing size and some metrics of plumage coloration. However, the biological significance of these factors varies between the different dominance behaviors performed. These results inform our understanding of interspecific signaling and its role in the evolution of intraguild communication and resource competition.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13410","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135917856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Conditional reciprocity in invertebrates is robust to disturbances 无脊椎动物的条件互惠性不受干扰影响
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-12 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13415
Maria Cristina Lorenzi, Picchi Laura, Ricci-Bonot Claire, Jeanne Elise

Reciprocity, one of the evolutionary explanations for cooperation, evolves between individuals when helping is conditional upon receiving help in return (“A helps B because B helped A before”). Often tested in controlled laboratory conditions, its robustness to external disturbances has rarely been evaluated. Here, we investigated whether reciprocity is robust when disturbances occur in between the donation by one individual and its return by a partner. As model system, we used a simultaneously hermaphroditic polychaete worm (Ophryotrocha diadema), where partners in isolated dyads engage in iterated exchanges of eggs for sperm (eggs are a costly donation relatively to sperm). If a partner donates eggs and the other fertilizes them, the latter is likely to donate eggs a couple of days later. (Eggs and larvae are then cared by the parents which sit at the nest and only leave it for foraging.) We removed the eggs from the nests daily or weekly and found that reciprocity decisions were not substantially altered; focal worms were more likely to lay if the partner laid before. We also found that, irrespective of treatment, worms exchanged comparable number of eggs, and were stimulated to lay significantly sooner if the partners laid a second clutch before they reciprocated. These results show that reciprocity is robust to disturbances which may happen in the 2 days’ time gap between egg donation and egg reciprocation.

互惠是合作的进化解释之一,当帮助以获得回报为条件时("A 帮助 B 是因为 B 曾经帮助过 A"),个体之间就会产生互惠。互惠通常在可控的实验室条件下进行测试,但其对外部干扰的稳健性却很少进行评估。在这里,我们研究了当一个人的捐赠和另一个人的回报之间发生干扰时,互惠是否具有稳健性。作为模型系统,我们使用了一种同时雌雄同体的多毛类蠕虫(Ophryotrocha diadema),在这种蠕虫中,孤立的两对中的伙伴会反复进行卵子与精子的交换(相对于精子而言,卵子的捐赠成本较高)。如果一方捐献卵子,另一方使其受精,那么后者很可能在几天后捐献卵子。(卵和幼虫随后由坐在巢中的亲鸟照料,亲鸟只有在觅食时才会离开巢穴)。我们每天或每周从巢中取出虫卵,发现互惠决定并没有发生重大改变;如果伴侣在之前产卵,则焦点虫更有可能产卵。我们还发现,无论处理方式如何,蠕虫交换的卵数量相当,而且如果伴侣在互惠之前产下第二窝卵,蠕虫产卵时间会明显提前。这些结果表明,互惠对捐卵和互惠之间的两天时间间隔内可能发生的干扰具有稳健性。
{"title":"Conditional reciprocity in invertebrates is robust to disturbances","authors":"Maria Cristina Lorenzi,&nbsp;Picchi Laura,&nbsp;Ricci-Bonot Claire,&nbsp;Jeanne Elise","doi":"10.1111/eth.13415","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13415","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Reciprocity, one of the evolutionary explanations for cooperation, evolves between individuals when helping is conditional upon receiving help in return (“A helps B because B helped A before”). Often tested in controlled laboratory conditions, its robustness to external disturbances has rarely been evaluated. Here, we investigated whether reciprocity is robust when disturbances occur in between the donation by one individual and its return by a partner. As model system, we used a simultaneously hermaphroditic polychaete worm (<i>Ophryotrocha diadema</i>), where partners in isolated dyads engage in iterated exchanges of eggs for sperm (eggs are a costly donation relatively to sperm). If a partner donates eggs and the other fertilizes them, the latter is likely to donate eggs a couple of days later. (Eggs and larvae are then cared by the parents which sit at the nest and only leave it for foraging.) We removed the eggs from the nests daily or weekly and found that reciprocity decisions were not substantially altered; focal worms were more likely to lay if the partner laid before. We also found that, irrespective of treatment, worms exchanged comparable number of eggs, and were stimulated to lay significantly sooner if the partners laid a second clutch before they reciprocated. These results show that reciprocity is robust to disturbances which may happen in the 2 days’ time gap between egg donation and egg reciprocation.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135967874","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Seasonally variable and sex-specific motivation for cooperative resource defence in a duetting songbird—The Chubb's cisticola 一种对唱鸣禽--楚布氏狸花鸟--合作保护资源的季节性变化和性别特异性动机
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-07 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13409
Michał Budka, Patryk Kokociński, Tomasz Stanisław Osiejuk

Duets are one of the most complex forms of acoustic displays, involving two individuals vocalising in a coordinated way to defend resources cooperatively. However, motivation to defend resources may be sex specific and variable in time and space, which should be reflected in the changes in duet singing behaviour. We experimentally examined whether the motivation to defend resources is sex specific and seasonally variable in an Afrotropical, sedentary songbird living in pairs or small groups—the Chubb's cisticola. We broadcasted songs of males, females and duets in a random order, both at the beginning and the end of the breeding season. We quantified the birds' responses from the perspective of the entire group as well as its individual members. We found cooperative responses to each type of intruder, with the birds flying and singing. Groups consisting of two to four individuals responded significantly more strongly towards intrusions by females than pairs, and at the beginning than at the end of breeding season. Solo syllables comprised less than 4% of all recorded syllables, and in most cases were sung within a song also containing duet and chorus syllables (only 12 from 1773 songs analysed were solo songs). Males sang solo syllables more often than females (83% vs. 17% of all solo syllables) and did it significantly more often towards intruding females than males or pairs. The number of solo syllables sung by females was independent of treatment and season. Together, our results suggests that Chubb's cisticola uses duets to cooperatively defend resources located within its territory, but the intensity of reaction is sex specific and seasonally variable. We suggest that defended resources could be mates, group stability or reproductive effort. Looking at the function of duets from the perspective of the group and its particular members, various stages of the season and different resources defended by birds allow for a better understanding of cooperative singing.

二重唱是最复杂的声音展示形式之一,涉及两个个体以协调的方式发声,以合作保护资源。然而,保护资源的动机可能是性别特异性的,在时间和空间上是可变的,这应该反映在二重唱行为的变化中。我们通过实验研究了一种生活在一对或一小群的非洲热带久坐鸣鸟(Chubb's cisticola)保护资源的动机是否具有性别特异性和季节性。我们在繁殖季节的开始和结束时,以随机的顺序播放雄性、雌性和二重唱的歌曲。我们从整个群体和个体成员的角度量化了鸟类的反应。我们发现它们对每一种入侵者都有合作的反应,鸟儿在飞翔和歌唱。由2 - 4只个体组成的群体对雌性入侵的反应明显强于成对,在繁殖季节开始时比在繁殖季节结束时更强烈。独唱音节在所有录制的音节中所占的比例不到4%,而且在大多数情况下,是在一首歌中演唱的,同时也包含了二重唱和合唱音节(在分析的1773首歌曲中,只有12首是独唱歌曲)。男性唱独唱音节的频率比女性高(83%比17%),而且对闯入的女性唱独唱音节的频率明显高于男性或成对唱独唱音节的频率。雌性独唱音节的数量与治疗和季节无关。总之,我们的研究结果表明,丘伯的cisticola使用二重唱来合作保护其领土内的资源,但反应的强度是特定性别和季节变化的。我们认为,保护资源可能是配偶、群体稳定性或繁殖努力。从群体及其特定成员的角度来看二重唱的功能,鸟类在季节的不同阶段和保护的不同资源可以更好地理解合作歌唱。
{"title":"Seasonally variable and sex-specific motivation for cooperative resource defence in a duetting songbird—The Chubb's cisticola","authors":"Michał Budka,&nbsp;Patryk Kokociński,&nbsp;Tomasz Stanisław Osiejuk","doi":"10.1111/eth.13409","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13409","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Duets are one of the most complex forms of acoustic displays, involving two individuals vocalising in a coordinated way to defend resources cooperatively. However, motivation to defend resources may be sex specific and variable in time and space, which should be reflected in the changes in duet singing behaviour. We experimentally examined whether the motivation to defend resources is sex specific and seasonally variable in an Afrotropical, sedentary songbird living in pairs or small groups—the Chubb's cisticola. We broadcasted songs of males, females and duets in a random order, both at the beginning and the end of the breeding season. We quantified the birds' responses from the perspective of the entire group as well as its individual members. We found cooperative responses to each type of intruder, with the birds flying and singing. Groups consisting of two to four individuals responded significantly more strongly towards intrusions by females than pairs, and at the beginning than at the end of breeding season. Solo syllables comprised less than 4% of all recorded syllables, and in most cases were sung within a song also containing duet and chorus syllables (only 12 from 1773 songs analysed were solo songs). Males sang solo syllables more often than females (83% vs. 17% of all solo syllables) and did it significantly more often towards intruding females than males or pairs. The number of solo syllables sung by females was independent of treatment and season. Together, our results suggests that Chubb's cisticola uses duets to cooperatively defend resources located within its territory, but the intensity of reaction is sex specific and seasonally variable. We suggest that defended resources could be mates, group stability or reproductive effort. Looking at the function of duets from the perspective of the group and its particular members, various stages of the season and different resources defended by birds allow for a better understanding of cooperative singing.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135252977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Grooming reciprocation in Himalayan tahr and the cognitive constraint hypothesis 喜马拉雅大羚羊的梳理互惠和认知约束假说
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-07 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13408
Gabriele Schino, Carola Manzoni, Massimiliano Di Giovanni

The cognitive constraint hypothesis maintains reciprocity is rare among animals because it requires cognitive capabilities that are rare, if not absent, in animals. In particular, it suggests that temporal discounting, limited memory, and limited capability of complex calculations make long-term reciprocation essentially impossible for animals. The cognitive constraint hypothesis therefore predicts that, if reciprocity ever occurs in animals, it should always be immediate. In this study, we tested for long-term reciprocation in Himalayan tahr (Hemitragus jemlahicus), a species that is not notable for its advanced cognitive capabilities. We showed that tahr are able to reciprocate grooming over extended time periods, that is, that they groom preferentially those individuals from which they receive more grooming even when all cases of immediate reciprocation are excluded from analysis. These results do not support the cognitive constraint hypothesis. We note the cognitive constraint hypothesis makes two silent assumptions: that all reciprocation is necessarily calculated and that it is based on partner control processes. We suggest these assumptions are unwarranted, and that most group living animals base their ability to reciprocate on emotionally based partner choice.

认知限制假说认为,互惠在动物中是罕见的,因为它需要的认知能力在动物中即使不是不存在,也是罕见的。特别是,该假说认为,时间折扣、有限的记忆和有限的复杂计算能力使得动物基本上不可能长期互惠。因此,认知约束假说预测,如果互惠发生在动物身上,它应该总是即时的。在这项研究中,我们测试了喜马拉雅泰勒(Hemitragus jemlahicus)的长期互惠行为,喜马拉雅泰勒是一种以高级认知能力著称的物种。我们的结果表明,喜马拉雅驯鹿能够在较长的时间段内互惠地梳理毛发,也就是说,即使在分析中排除了所有即时互惠的情况,喜马拉雅驯鹿也会优先梳理那些它们从其身上获得更多毛发的个体。这些结果并不支持认知约束假说。我们注意到,认知约束假说提出了两个无声的假设:所有的互惠都必然是经过计算的,而且是基于伴侣控制过程的。我们认为这些假设都是没有根据的,大多数群居动物的互惠能力都是基于对伴侣的情感选择。
{"title":"Grooming reciprocation in Himalayan tahr and the cognitive constraint hypothesis","authors":"Gabriele Schino,&nbsp;Carola Manzoni,&nbsp;Massimiliano Di Giovanni","doi":"10.1111/eth.13408","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13408","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The cognitive constraint hypothesis maintains reciprocity is rare among animals because it requires cognitive capabilities that are rare, if not absent, in animals. In particular, it suggests that temporal discounting, limited memory, and limited capability of complex calculations make long-term reciprocation essentially impossible for animals. The cognitive constraint hypothesis therefore predicts that, if reciprocity ever occurs in animals, it should always be immediate. In this study, we tested for long-term reciprocation in Himalayan tahr (<i>Hemitragus jemlahicus</i>), a species that is not notable for its advanced cognitive capabilities. We showed that tahr are able to reciprocate grooming over extended time periods, that is, that they groom preferentially those individuals from which they receive more grooming even when all cases of immediate reciprocation are excluded from analysis. These results do not support the cognitive constraint hypothesis. We note the cognitive constraint hypothesis makes two silent assumptions: that all reciprocation is necessarily calculated and that it is based on partner control processes. We suggest these assumptions are unwarranted, and that most group living animals base their ability to reciprocate on emotionally based partner choice.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13408","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135254633","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Male Barbary macaques choose loyal coalition partners which may increase their coalition network betweenness 雄性巴巴利猕猴选择忠诚的联盟伙伴,这可能会增加它们的联盟网络间度
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13413
Oliver Schülke, Eva-Maria Rathke, Andreas Berghänel, Julia Ostner

Reciprocity in the form of contingent exchanges of goods and services is widespread across animals. While there is ample evidence for helping to be contingent upon the help received from a partner, less attention has been paid to partner avoidance based on harm inflicted by a partner. Here, we investigated whether partner choice for agonistic support against powerful targets is guided by loyalty received, i.e., the tendency to refrain from attacking the subject in a coalition with any third partner. We further assessed whether loyalty received by all cooperation partners may generate increased levels of betweenness in the coalition network of a group, a measure of indirect connectedness that has previously been associated with fitness benefits. Based on observational data from male coalitions against male group mates in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus), loyalty received was found to predict the frequency of cooperation in coalitions and the loyalty given to a partner. We propose that loyalty-guided reciprocity will be favored in species with rank-changing coalitions where defection is particularly risky. The more loyal a male's cooperation partners were, the more central he was in the coalition network in terms of higher in betweenness, suggesting a cognitively simple strategy underlying complex network positioning. Analyses of simulated data suggest strong correlations of loyalty and betweenness to be more prevalent in the relatively small groups characteristic of many primate species.

以有条件的商品和服务交换为形式的互惠在动物中非常普遍。虽然有大量证据表明,帮助是以从伙伴那里得到的帮助为条件的,但人们对基于伙伴造成的伤害而回避伙伴的情况关注较少。在这里,我们研究了在针对强大目标的争斗支持中,伙伴的选择是否受所获忠诚度的指导,即在与任何第三伙伴的联盟中避免攻击主体的倾向。我们还进一步评估了所有合作伙伴的忠诚度是否会提高群体联盟网络中的 "间性"(betweenness)水平。根据巴巴利猕猴(Macaca sylvanus)中雄性联盟对雄性群体伙伴的观察数据,我们发现所获得的忠诚度可以预测联盟中的合作频率以及给予伙伴的忠诚度。我们认为,在具有等级变化联盟的物种中,忠诚引导的互惠将受到青睐,因为在这种联盟中,叛逃的风险特别大。雄性的合作伙伴越忠诚,它在联盟网络中的中心位置就越高,这表明复杂的网络定位背后隐藏着一种简单的认知策略。对模拟数据的分析表明,在许多灵长类动物所特有的相对较小的群体中,忠诚度和间度的强相关性更为普遍。
{"title":"Male Barbary macaques choose loyal coalition partners which may increase their coalition network betweenness","authors":"Oliver Schülke,&nbsp;Eva-Maria Rathke,&nbsp;Andreas Berghänel,&nbsp;Julia Ostner","doi":"10.1111/eth.13413","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13413","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Reciprocity in the form of contingent exchanges of goods and services is widespread across animals. While there is ample evidence for helping to be contingent upon the help received from a partner, less attention has been paid to partner avoidance based on harm inflicted by a partner. Here, we investigated whether partner choice for agonistic support against powerful targets is guided by loyalty received, i.e., the tendency to refrain from attacking the subject in a coalition with any third partner. We further assessed whether loyalty received by all cooperation partners may generate increased levels of betweenness in the coalition network of a group, a measure of indirect connectedness that has previously been associated with fitness benefits. Based on observational data from male coalitions against male group mates in Barbary macaques (<i>Macaca sylvanus</i>), loyalty received was found to predict the frequency of cooperation in coalitions and the loyalty given to a partner. We propose that loyalty-guided reciprocity will be favored in species with rank-changing coalitions where defection is particularly risky. The more loyal a male's cooperation partners were, the more central he was in the coalition network in terms of higher in betweenness, suggesting a cognitively simple strategy underlying complex network positioning. Analyses of simulated data suggest strong correlations of loyalty and betweenness to be more prevalent in the relatively small groups characteristic of many primate species.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13413","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135590469","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does a niche construction strategy adaptation really exist in brown-headed cowbirds? 棕头牛鸝真的存在利基构建战略适应吗?
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-03 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13412
Manuel Soler
<p>The subject of brood parasitism has become the focus of widespread attention for the numerous finely tuned adaptations that have been discovered in coevolving specialist brood parasite–host systems (Davies, <span>2000</span>; Krüger, <span>2007</span>; Soler, <span>2017a</span>). A recent study by Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) claims to have found one of these fascinating adaptations, posing the exciting conclusion that brown-headed cowbird (<i>Molothrus ater</i>) chicks use a niche construction strategy (alteration of its own environment for its own fitness benefit) in prothonotary warbler (<i>Protonotaria citrea</i>) host nests. In this experimental study, the authors have found that the selective brood reduction strategy driven by cowbird nestlings is reducing, but not eliminating host broods. According to their interpretation, this appears to represent an adaptive niche construction strategy given the brood reduction results and that survival of cowbird nestlings is higher in broods of two warbler nestlings than when alone or in broods of four warbler nestlings. This is an intriguing possibility but does a niche construction strategy allowing the survival of two nestmates really exist in brown-headed cowbirds?</p><p>Niche construction is a mechanism whereby individuals actively manipulate their environment to obtain more appropriate conditions in which their possibilities of survival and reproductive success increase (Aaby & Ramsey, <span>2022</span>; Odling-Smee et al., <span>2013</span>; Trappes et al., <span>2022</span>). In birds, nest building is a clear example of niche construction (Trappes et al., <span>2022</span>). The strategy of the common cuckoo (<i>Cuculus canorus</i>) nestling, which soon after hatching evicts all host nestmates from the nest, allowing it to monopolize the feeding effort of its foster parents, can be considered another clear case of niche construction. Other brood parasites (non-evictors) share the nest with host nestlings, but usually, the parasitic nestling(s) outcompetes host nestlings. This also implies an active manipulation of their environment (the nest), and therefore, this strategy could also be considered niche construction. However, Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) suggest that in the brown-headed cowbird—protonotary warbler system, the brood parasite selectively manipulates brood reduction favouring the presence of two warbler nestlings in the nest. This suggestion is based on comparisons of data on nestling warbler mortality in four experimental groups, with two parasitized and two unparasitized treatments. However, the crucial prediction of the niche construction hypothesis—that is, that the cowbird nestling causes selective host brood reduction, allowing the survival of just two host nestlings—was not demonstrated.</p><p>In addition, this study selectively cites publications and raises several other key questions. Antonson et al. (<span>2022</span>) base their study on the ‘beggin
由于在共同进化的特殊寄主-寄主系统中发现了许多精细调节的适应,育雏寄生的主题已成为广泛关注的焦点(Davies, 2000;克鲁格,2007;太阳系,2017)。Antonson等人(2022)最近的一项研究声称发现了其中一种令人着迷的适应性,并提出了令人兴奋的结论,即褐头牛鹂(Molothrus ater)雏鸟在原冠莺(Protonotaria citrea)宿主巢穴中使用生态位构建策略(改变自身环境以获得自身健康利益)。在本实验研究中,作者发现,由牛椋鸟雏鸟驱动的选择性减育策略是减少而不是消除宿主窝。根据他们的解释,考虑到雏鸟数量减少的结果,这似乎代表了一种适应性生态位构建策略,而且两只莺巢的雏鸟存活率高于单独或四只莺巢的雏鸟存活率。这是一种有趣的可能性,但是在棕头牛鹂中真的存在一种允许两个同伴生存的生态位构建策略吗?生态位构建是一种机制,通过这种机制,个体主动操纵其环境,以获得更合适的条件,从而增加其生存和繁殖成功的可能性(aby & Ramsey, 2022;Odling-Smee et al., 2013;Trappes et al., 2022)。在鸟类中,筑巢是生态位建设的一个明显例子(Trappes et al., 2022)。普通杜鹃(Cuculus canorus)的筑巢策略,在孵化后不久就把所有的寄主同伴赶出巢,从而垄断了养父母的喂养努力,可以被认为是生态位构建的另一个明显案例。其他幼虫寄生虫(非驱逐者)与寄主雏鸟共享巢穴,但通常情况下,寄主雏鸟比寄主雏鸟竞争更激烈。这也意味着它们对环境(巢穴)的主动操纵,因此,这种策略也可以被认为是生态位构建。然而,Antonson等人(2022)认为,在褐头牛头鸟-原尾莺系统中,幼虫寄生虫选择性地操纵幼虫减少,有利于巢中有两只莺的雏鸟。这一建议是基于四个实验组的雏鸟死亡率数据的比较,两个寄生和两个未寄生处理。然而,生态位构建假说的关键预测——即,牛鹂的雏鸟导致选择性的寄主窝减少,只允许两个寄主窝存活——并没有得到证明。此外,本研究选择性地引用了出版物,并提出了其他几个关键问题。Antonson等人(2022)的研究基于“乞讨协助假说”(Kilner等人,2004),该假说认为,与寄主雏鸟共用巢穴的棕头牛鹂雏鸟比单独饲养的牛鹂雏鸟生长得更快,因为寄生雏鸟利用寄主雏鸟发出的乞讨叫声来获取资源。也就是说,寄主雏鸟的乞讨表现迫使它们的父母增加食物传递的速度,由于其更高的竞争能力,这些额外的食物将被雏鸟消耗掉(Kilner et al., 2004)。为了支持寄生雏鸟需要宿主雏鸟的帮助才能获得足够数量的食物这一假设,作者引用了所有支持这一假设的出版物,包括在该假设提出之前(Lichtenstein & Sealy, 1998)和之后(Gloag et al., 2012;Hoover & Reetz, 2006;Kilner et al., 2004;Li & Hauber, 2021)。其中一篇论文Li和Hauber(2021)明确指出,这一假设仅适用于中型宿主物种。此外,Gloag等人(2012)报告称,乞讨援助假说仅在一个小宿主物种中得到支持,而在一个较大的宿主物种中没有得到支持。不幸的是,Antonson et al.(2022)没有提到任何不支持驱逐幼虫寄生虫乞讨援助假说预测的实验研究(Grim et al., 2009;Hauber & Moskát, 2008;Martín-Gálvez et al., 2005)。Antonson et al.(2022)似乎也没有意识到其他研究(例如Bolopo et al., 2015;Rivers et al., 2010;Soler & de Neve, 2013)和一篇综述(Soler, 2017b)得出的结论是,在几个共用巢穴的寄生虫物种中,较大的雏鸟成功地竞争额外的食物,无论它们是宿主还是寄生虫。Antonson et al.(2022)在他们的论文中没有讨论这一关键因素(即寄生雏鸟相对于宿主雏鸟的大小)是如何影响哪只雏鸟获得额外食物的,因为寄生雏鸟和宿主雏鸟共享的巢穴会产生强烈的乞讨行为。 由于在共同进化的特殊寄主-寄主系统中发现了许多精细调节的适应,育雏寄生的主题已成为广泛关注的焦点(Davies, 2000;克鲁格,2007;太阳系,2017)。Antonson等人(2022)最近的一项研究声称发现了其中一种令人着迷的适应性,并提出了令人兴奋的结论,即褐头牛鹂(Molothrus ater)雏鸟在原冠莺(Protonotaria citrea)宿主巢穴中使用生态位构建策略(改变自身环境以获得自身健康利益)。在本实验研究中,作者发现,由牛椋鸟雏鸟驱动的选择性减育策略是减少而不是消除宿主窝。根据他们的解释,考虑到雏鸟数量减少的结果,这似乎代表了一种适应性生态位构建策略,而且两只莺巢的雏鸟存活率高于单独或四只莺巢的雏鸟存活率。这是一种有趣的可能性,但是在棕头牛鹂中真的存在一种允许两个同伴生存的生态位构建策略吗?生态位构建是个体主动操纵环境以获得更合适的生存条件和繁殖成功率增加的一种机制(aby &拉姆齐,2022;Odling-Smee et al., 2013;Trappes et al., 2022)。在鸟类中,筑巢是生态位建设的一个明显例子(Trappes et al., 2022)。普通杜鹃(Cuculus canorus)的筑巢策略,在孵化后不久就把所有的寄主同伴赶出巢,从而垄断了养父母的喂养努力,可以被认为是生态位构建的另一个明显案例。其他幼虫寄生虫(非驱逐者)与寄主雏鸟共享巢穴,但通常情况下,寄主雏鸟比寄主雏鸟竞争更激烈。这也意味着它们对环境(巢穴)的主动操纵,因此,这种策略也可以被认为是生态位构建。然而,Antonson等人(2022)认为,在褐头牛头鸟-原尾莺系统中,幼虫寄生虫选择性地操纵幼虫减少,有利于巢中有两只莺的雏鸟。这一建议是基于四个实验组的雏鸟死亡率数据的比较,两个寄生和两个未寄生处理。然而,生态位构建假说的关键预测——即,牛鹂的雏鸟导致选择性的寄主窝减少,只允许两个寄主窝存活——并没有得到证明。此外,本研究选择性地引用了出版物,并提出了其他几个关键问题。Antonson等人(2022)的研究基于“乞讨协助假说”(Kilner等人,2004),该假说认为,与寄主雏鸟共用巢穴的棕头牛鹂雏鸟比单独饲养的牛鹂雏鸟生长得更快,因为寄生雏鸟利用寄主雏鸟发出的乞讨叫声来获取资源。也就是说,寄主雏鸟的乞讨表现迫使它们的父母增加食物传递的速度,由于其更高的竞争能力,这些额外的食物将被雏鸟消耗掉(Kilner et al., 2004)。为了支持寄生雏鸟需要宿主雏鸟的帮助来获得足够数量的食物这一假设,作者引用了之前所有支持这一假设的出版物(Lichtenstein &Sealy, 1998)和假说提出后(Gloag et al., 2012;胡佛,Reetz, 2006;Kilner et al., 2004;李,霍伯,2021)。其中一篇论文Li和Hauber(2021)明确指出,这一假设仅适用于中型
{"title":"Does a niche construction strategy adaptation really exist in brown-headed cowbirds?","authors":"Manuel Soler","doi":"10.1111/eth.13412","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13412","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;The subject of brood parasitism has become the focus of widespread attention for the numerous finely tuned adaptations that have been discovered in coevolving specialist brood parasite–host systems (Davies, &lt;span&gt;2000&lt;/span&gt;; Krüger, &lt;span&gt;2007&lt;/span&gt;; Soler, &lt;span&gt;2017a&lt;/span&gt;). A recent study by Antonson et al. (&lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;) claims to have found one of these fascinating adaptations, posing the exciting conclusion that brown-headed cowbird (&lt;i&gt;Molothrus ater&lt;/i&gt;) chicks use a niche construction strategy (alteration of its own environment for its own fitness benefit) in prothonotary warbler (&lt;i&gt;Protonotaria citrea&lt;/i&gt;) host nests. In this experimental study, the authors have found that the selective brood reduction strategy driven by cowbird nestlings is reducing, but not eliminating host broods. According to their interpretation, this appears to represent an adaptive niche construction strategy given the brood reduction results and that survival of cowbird nestlings is higher in broods of two warbler nestlings than when alone or in broods of four warbler nestlings. This is an intriguing possibility but does a niche construction strategy allowing the survival of two nestmates really exist in brown-headed cowbirds?&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Niche construction is a mechanism whereby individuals actively manipulate their environment to obtain more appropriate conditions in which their possibilities of survival and reproductive success increase (Aaby &amp; Ramsey, &lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;; Odling-Smee et al., &lt;span&gt;2013&lt;/span&gt;; Trappes et al., &lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;). In birds, nest building is a clear example of niche construction (Trappes et al., &lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;). The strategy of the common cuckoo (&lt;i&gt;Cuculus canorus&lt;/i&gt;) nestling, which soon after hatching evicts all host nestmates from the nest, allowing it to monopolize the feeding effort of its foster parents, can be considered another clear case of niche construction. Other brood parasites (non-evictors) share the nest with host nestlings, but usually, the parasitic nestling(s) outcompetes host nestlings. This also implies an active manipulation of their environment (the nest), and therefore, this strategy could also be considered niche construction. However, Antonson et al. (&lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;) suggest that in the brown-headed cowbird—protonotary warbler system, the brood parasite selectively manipulates brood reduction favouring the presence of two warbler nestlings in the nest. This suggestion is based on comparisons of data on nestling warbler mortality in four experimental groups, with two parasitized and two unparasitized treatments. However, the crucial prediction of the niche construction hypothesis—that is, that the cowbird nestling causes selective host brood reduction, allowing the survival of just two host nestlings—was not demonstrated.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;In addition, this study selectively cites publications and raises several other key questions. Antonson et al. (&lt;span&gt;2022&lt;/span&gt;) base their study on the ‘beggin","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13412","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135695465","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direct reciprocity among humans 人类之间的直接互惠
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q4 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-30 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13407
Charlotte S. L. Rossetti, Christian Hilbe

Direct reciprocity is the tendency to repay others' cooperation. This tendency can be crucial to maintain cooperation in evolving populations. Once direct reciprocity evolves, individuals have a long-run interest to cooperate, even if it is costly in the short run. The major theoretical framework to describe reciprocal behavior is the repeated prisoner's dilemma. Over the past decades, this game has been the major workhorse to predict when reciprocal cooperation ought to evolve, and which strategies individuals are supposed to adopt. Herein, we compare these predictions with the empirical evidence from experiments with human subjects. From a theory-driven perspective, humans represent an ideal test case, because they give researchers the most flexibility to tailor the experimental design to the assumptions of a model. Overall, we find that theoretical models describe well in which situations people cooperate. However, in the important case of “indefinitely repeated games,” they have difficulties to predict which strategies people use.

直接互惠是一种回报他人合作的倾向。这种倾向对于在不断进化的种群中维持合作至关重要。一旦演化出直接互惠性,个体就会有长期合作的兴趣,即使短期内合作的成本很高。描述互惠行为的主要理论框架是重复囚徒困境。在过去的几十年里,这个博弈一直是预测互惠合作何时应该发展以及个体应该采取何种策略的主要工具。在这里,我们将这些预测与人类实验的经验证据进行比较。从理论驱动的角度来看,人类是一个理想的测试案例,因为他们给了研究人员最大的灵活性,使实验设计符合模型的假设。总体而言,我们发现理论模型很好地描述了人们在哪些情况下会合作。然而,在 "无限重复博弈 "这一重要情况下,理论模型很难预测人们会使用哪些策略。
{"title":"Direct reciprocity among humans","authors":"Charlotte S. L. Rossetti,&nbsp;Christian Hilbe","doi":"10.1111/eth.13407","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13407","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Direct reciprocity is the tendency to repay others' cooperation. This tendency can be crucial to maintain cooperation in evolving populations. Once direct reciprocity evolves, individuals have a long-run interest to cooperate, even if it is costly in the short run. The major theoretical framework to describe reciprocal behavior is the repeated prisoner's dilemma. Over the past decades, this game has been the major workhorse to predict when reciprocal cooperation ought to evolve, and which strategies individuals are supposed to adopt. Herein, we compare these predictions with the empirical evidence from experiments with human subjects. From a theory-driven perspective, humans represent an ideal test case, because they give researchers the most flexibility to tailor the experimental design to the assumptions of a model. Overall, we find that theoretical models describe well in which situations people cooperate. However, in the important case of “indefinitely repeated games,” they have difficulties to predict which strategies people use.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":"130 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-09-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13407","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136341607","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1