首页 > 最新文献

Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
Do pet dogs reciprocate the receipt of food from familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics? 宠物狗从熟悉和不熟悉的同类那里接受食物时是否会有回报?
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-10 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13430
Jim McGetrick, Leona Fux, Johannes Schullern-Schrattenhofen, Jean-Loup Rault, Friederike Range

Reciprocity is one of the most prominent explanations for the evolution of stable cooperation. Although reciprocity has been studied for decades in numerous animal species and behavioural contexts, its underlying proximate mechanisms remain unclear. Domestic dogs provide a useful model species for the study of proximate mechanisms, though there are currently inconsistent findings regarding dogs' propensity to reciprocate. Here, we investigated whether, after minimal training, pet dogs would press a button, which remotely controlled a food dispenser, to deliver food to an enclosure occupied by a helpful conspecific that had provided them with food or an unhelpful conspecific that had not provided them with food. We included an asocial control condition in which the enclosure was unoccupied and a social facilitation control in which the food delivery mechanism was non-functional. Whether subjects were familiar with the helpful and unhelpful conspecifics was also varied. In addition, to investigate potential mechanisms underlying reciprocity, we measured subjects salivary oxytocin concentration before and after they experienced the helpful and unhelpful acts. There was no effect of the previous helpfulness or the familiarity of the partner on the number of times subjects pressed the button. However, there was also no effect of the presence of a partner or the operationality of the food delivery mechanism on the number of button presses, indicating that subjects were not pressing the button to provision the partner. Moreover, the experience of the helpful or unhelpful act did not influence subjects' salivary oxytocin concentration. Variation in findings of reciprocity across studies appears to correspond with differing training protocols. Subjects' understanding of the task in the current study may have been constrained by the limited training received. Additional tests to verify subjects' understanding of such tasks are warranted in future studies.

互惠是稳定合作进化的最重要解释之一。尽管数十年来人们已经在许多动物物种和行为环境中对互惠性进行了研究,但其潜在的近似机制仍不清楚。家犬为近似机制的研究提供了一个有用的模式物种,尽管目前关于家犬互惠倾向的研究结果并不一致。在这里,我们研究了宠物狗在经过最低限度的训练后,是否会按下一个遥控食物分配器的按钮,将食物送到为其提供食物的同类或未为其提供食物的同类所占据的围栏中。我们还加入了一种非社会控制条件,即围栏无人居住;以及一种社会促进控制条件,即食物分配机制不起作用。被试是否熟悉提供帮助和不提供帮助的同种动物也有不同。此外,为了研究互惠的潜在机制,我们测量了受试者在经历帮助和不帮助行为前后唾液催产素的浓度。之前的帮助行为或伴侣的熟悉程度对受试者按下按钮的次数没有影响。然而,同伴的存在或食物传送装置的操作性对按下按钮的次数也没有影响,这表明受试者按下按钮并不是为了给同伴提供食物。此外,帮助或不帮助行为的经历也不会影响受试者唾液催产素的浓度。不同研究中互惠结果的差异似乎与不同的训练方案有关。在目前的研究中,受试者对任务的理解可能受到了有限培训的限制。在今后的研究中,有必要进行更多的测试来验证受试者对此类任务的理解。
{"title":"Do pet dogs reciprocate the receipt of food from familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics?","authors":"Jim McGetrick,&nbsp;Leona Fux,&nbsp;Johannes Schullern-Schrattenhofen,&nbsp;Jean-Loup Rault,&nbsp;Friederike Range","doi":"10.1111/eth.13430","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13430","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Reciprocity is one of the most prominent explanations for the evolution of stable cooperation. Although reciprocity has been studied for decades in numerous animal species and behavioural contexts, its underlying proximate mechanisms remain unclear. Domestic dogs provide a useful model species for the study of proximate mechanisms, though there are currently inconsistent findings regarding dogs' propensity to reciprocate. Here, we investigated whether, after minimal training, pet dogs would press a button, which remotely controlled a food dispenser, to deliver food to an enclosure occupied by a helpful conspecific that had provided them with food or an unhelpful conspecific that had not provided them with food. We included an asocial control condition in which the enclosure was unoccupied and a social facilitation control in which the food delivery mechanism was non-functional. Whether subjects were familiar with the helpful and unhelpful conspecifics was also varied. In addition, to investigate potential mechanisms underlying reciprocity, we measured subjects salivary oxytocin concentration before and after they experienced the helpful and unhelpful acts. There was no effect of the previous helpfulness or the familiarity of the partner on the number of times subjects pressed the button. However, there was also no effect of the presence of a partner or the operationality of the food delivery mechanism on the number of button presses, indicating that subjects were not pressing the button to provision the partner. Moreover, the experience of the helpful or unhelpful act did not influence subjects' salivary oxytocin concentration. Variation in findings of reciprocity across studies appears to correspond with differing training protocols. Subjects' understanding of the task in the current study may have been constrained by the limited training received. Additional tests to verify subjects' understanding of such tasks are warranted in future studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13430","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139465061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dynamic visual noise has limited influence on the habitat selection and behavioural activity of crustaceans and cephalopods 动态视觉噪音对甲壳类和头足类的栖息地选择和行为活动影响有限
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-10 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13432
Christian Drerup, Martin J. How, James E. Herbert-Read

Environments contain various forms of noise that can interfere with the ability of animal sensory systems to perceive information. One ubiquitous type of visual noise in shallow aquatic habitats is caustic flicker (or caustics), consisting of dynamically moving light patterns caused by the refraction of light when passing through the water's rippling surface. While some teleost fish avoid environments with caustic noise (where their prey can be more difficult to detect), it remains untested whether caustics affect the habitat selection of invertebrates. In the present study, we ask whether three invertebrate species, the shore crab Carcinus maenas, the brown shrimp Crangon crangon, and the common cuttlefish Sepia officinalis, prefer or avoid associating with environments with caustic noise, and whether caustics affect their behavioural activity and habitat exploration. To do this, we exposed the three species in binary choice experiments to different simulated caustic noise levels varying in their temporal (speed) and spatial (definition) components. Neither of the three tested invertebrate species spent more or less time in environments with higher caustic noise levels. While we also found no evidence that caustics affected the behavioural activity and exploration of Ca. maenas and S. officinalis, the brown shrimp Cr. crangon reduced its activity with increasing spatial caustic noise. However, all obtained effect sizes in this study were small, suggesting that caustic noise only minimally affects invertebrate behaviour. Overall, our results show that, unlike in teleost fish, caustics have limited influence on the habitat selection, exploration, and activity of crustaceans and cephalopods.

环境中存在各种形式的噪音,会干扰动物感官系统感知信息的能力。在浅水栖息地,一种普遍存在的视觉噪声是苛性闪烁(或苛色),它是由光线通过水波纹表面时发生折射而产生的动态移动光型组成的。虽然一些长尾鱼类会避开有苛性噪声的环境(在这种环境中,它们的猎物更难被发现),但苛性噪声是否会影响无脊椎动物的栖息地选择,目前仍未得到证实。在本研究中,我们询问了三种无脊椎动物--岸蟹(Carcinus maenas)、褐虾(Crangon crangon)和普通墨鱼(Sepia officinalis)--是否喜欢或避免与有腐蚀性噪音的环境发生联系,以及腐蚀性是否会影响它们的行为活动和栖息地探索。为此,我们在二元选择实验中将这三种无脊椎动物暴露在不同的模拟苛性噪声水平下,这些噪声在时间(速度)和空间(定义)上各不相同。三个受测无脊椎动物物种都没有在苛性噪音水平较高的环境中花费更多或更少的时间。虽然我们也没有发现苛性碱影响 Ca. maenas 和 S. officinalis 的行为活动和探索的证据,但褐虾 Cr. crangon 的活动却随着空间苛性碱噪声的增加而减少。然而,本研究获得的所有效应大小都很小,表明腐蚀性噪声对无脊椎动物行为的影响很小。总之,我们的研究结果表明,与远程鱼类不同,苛性碱对甲壳类和头足类的栖息地选择、探索和活动影响有限。
{"title":"Dynamic visual noise has limited influence on the habitat selection and behavioural activity of crustaceans and cephalopods","authors":"Christian Drerup,&nbsp;Martin J. How,&nbsp;James E. Herbert-Read","doi":"10.1111/eth.13432","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13432","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Environments contain various forms of noise that can interfere with the ability of animal sensory systems to perceive information. One ubiquitous type of visual noise in shallow aquatic habitats is caustic flicker (or caustics), consisting of dynamically moving light patterns caused by the refraction of light when passing through the water's rippling surface. While some teleost fish avoid environments with caustic noise (where their prey can be more difficult to detect), it remains untested whether caustics affect the habitat selection of invertebrates. In the present study, we ask whether three invertebrate species, the shore crab <i>Carcinus maenas</i>, the brown shrimp <i>Crangon crangon</i>, and the common cuttlefish <i>Sepia officinalis</i>, prefer or avoid associating with environments with caustic noise, and whether caustics affect their behavioural activity and habitat exploration. To do this, we exposed the three species in binary choice experiments to different simulated caustic noise levels varying in their temporal (speed) and spatial (definition) components. Neither of the three tested invertebrate species spent more or less time in environments with higher caustic noise levels. While we also found no evidence that caustics affected the behavioural activity and exploration of <i>Ca. maenas</i> and <i>S. officinalis</i>, the brown shrimp <i>Cr. crangon</i> reduced its activity with increasing spatial caustic noise. However, all obtained effect sizes in this study were small, suggesting that caustic noise only minimally affects invertebrate behaviour. Overall, our results show that, unlike in teleost fish, caustics have limited influence on the habitat selection, exploration, and activity of crustaceans and cephalopods.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13432","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139410947","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of short- and long-term enrichment on brain and behavior in Trinidadian guppies 短期和长期强化对特立尼达河豚大脑和行为的影响
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-05 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13436
R. Quinn Iffert, Laura R. Stein

Environmental enrichment (EE) often increases positive behavioral and physiological effects on captive animals. Fish are commercially and scientifically important taxa that have been shown to benefit from EE. Here we examined the effects of both short- and long-term EE in Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). In the short-term exposure to EE, female guppies were raised in standard conditions, and after reaching adulthood were moved to either an enriched or a deprived environment for 2 weeks. Long-term exposure guppies were reared from birth for 12 weeks (until sexual maturity) in either an enriched or deprived environment. We then assessed growth, brain size, and neophobic and exploratory behaviors in standard assays. Guppies given EE were bolder, regardless of timescale, although females showed a more pronounced change in behaviors than males. We further found that guppies reared with EE were smaller yet had larger relative brain sizes than guppies reared under deprived conditions. Here we highlight that EE had influenced growth, brain size, neophobic, and exploratory behaviors in guppies, and behavioral changes were observed after only 2 weeks. Our results highlight the need for assessing the influences of EE in captivity, in particular for researchers studying cognition and behavior.

环境强化(EE)通常会对圈养动物的行为和生理产生积极影响。鱼类是重要的商业和科学类群,已被证明能从 EE 中获益。在这里,我们研究了短期和长期 EE 对特立尼达河豚(Poecilia reticulata)的影响。在短期暴露于 EE 的情况下,雌性河鲈在标准条件下长大,成年后被转移到富集或匮乏的环境中饲养 2 周。长期暴露于 EE 的雌性河湟鱼从出生起就在富氧或贫氧环境中饲养 12 周(直至性成熟)。然后,我们通过标准实验对其生长、脑容量、恐新行为和探索行为进行了评估。无论时间长短,给予 EE 的河豚都更加大胆,但雌性河豚的行为变化比雄性河豚更明显。我们还发现,与在剥夺条件下饲养的虹鳟相比,使用 EE 饲养的虹鳟体型更小,但相对脑容量却更大。在此,我们强调,EE 影响了河豚鱼的生长、脑容量、恐新和探索行为,而且仅在 2 周后就观察到了行为变化。我们的研究结果强调了评估圈养环境影响的必要性,尤其是对研究认知和行为的研究人员而言。
{"title":"Effects of short- and long-term enrichment on brain and behavior in Trinidadian guppies","authors":"R. Quinn Iffert,&nbsp;Laura R. Stein","doi":"10.1111/eth.13436","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13436","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Environmental enrichment (EE) often increases positive behavioral and physiological effects on captive animals. Fish are commercially and scientifically important taxa that have been shown to benefit from EE. Here we examined the effects of both short- and long-term EE in Trinidadian guppies (<i>Poecilia reticulata</i>). In the short-term exposure to EE, female guppies were raised in standard conditions, and after reaching adulthood were moved to either an enriched or a deprived environment for 2 weeks. Long-term exposure guppies were reared from birth for 12 weeks (until sexual maturity) in either an enriched or deprived environment. We then assessed growth, brain size, and neophobic and exploratory behaviors in standard assays. Guppies given EE were bolder, regardless of timescale, although females showed a more pronounced change in behaviors than males. We further found that guppies reared with EE were smaller yet had larger relative brain sizes than guppies reared under deprived conditions. Here we highlight that EE had influenced growth, brain size, neophobic, and exploratory behaviors in guppies, and behavioral changes were observed after only 2 weeks. Our results highlight the need for assessing the influences of EE in captivity, in particular for researchers studying cognition and behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13436","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139376402","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stuck on you: Wind-dispersed seeds attach to the external surfaces of a tree frog 粘在你身上随风飘散的种子附着在树蛙的外表上
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-03 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13435
John Gould, Jose W. Valdez

Many plants exploit the ability of animals to move in order to disperse their pollen and seeds. Despite the growing understanding of the role animals play in the reproductive lives of plants, there are few examples of amphibians contributing to these processes. We report on an Australian tree frog, Litoria fallax, interacting with and likely moving seeds of the broad-leaved cumbungi, Typha orientalis, over short distances via external transport. Field observations revealed both juvenile and adult L. fallax individuals carrying typha seeds, which were adhered to the skin on the feet, legs, belly and dorsum. Approximately 30% of observed frogs were found to be carrying 1–14 seeds at a time. The small size and specialised fibre tufts of typha seeds, which enable them to be primarily dispersed by wind, also make them susceptible to being unintentionally picked up and transported by L. fallax as they move through aquatic environments. The moist surfaces of frogs, coupled with their presence in freshwater systems that can be dominated by typha, create favourable conditions for seed attachment. These findings highlight a previously unknown interaction between an amphibian and a plant that enables seed dispersal, supporting the need for further exploration into unexpected plant vectors.

许多植物利用动物的移动能力来传播花粉和种子。尽管人们对动物在植物繁殖过程中所扮演的角色有了越来越多的了解,但很少有两栖动物参与这些过程的例子。我们报告了一种澳大利亚树蛙 Litoria fallax 与阔叶香蒲(Typha orientalis)种子的相互作用,并可能通过外部运输将种子进行短距离迁移。实地观察发现,幼蛙和成年蛙个体都携带香蒲种子,这些种子附着在脚、腿、腹部和背部的皮肤上。观察发现,约有 30% 的青蛙一次携带 1-14 粒种子。香蒲种子体积小、纤维束特殊,主要靠风力传播,这也使得香蒲种子在水生环境中移动时,容易被落叶蛙无意中拾取和携带。蛙类潮湿的体表,再加上它们在淡水系统中的存在,这些都为种子附着创造了有利条件。这些发现突显了两栖动物与植物之间以前未知的相互作用,这种相互作用使种子得以传播,支持了进一步探索意想不到的植物媒介的必要性。
{"title":"Stuck on you: Wind-dispersed seeds attach to the external surfaces of a tree frog","authors":"John Gould,&nbsp;Jose W. Valdez","doi":"10.1111/eth.13435","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13435","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many plants exploit the ability of animals to move in order to disperse their pollen and seeds. Despite the growing understanding of the role animals play in the reproductive lives of plants, there are few examples of amphibians contributing to these processes. We report on an Australian tree frog, <i>Litoria fallax</i>, interacting with and likely moving seeds of the broad-leaved cumbungi, <i>Typha orientalis</i>, over short distances via external transport. Field observations revealed both juvenile and adult <i>L. fallax</i> individuals carrying typha seeds, which were adhered to the skin on the feet, legs, belly and dorsum. Approximately 30% of observed frogs were found to be carrying 1–14 seeds at a time. The small size and specialised fibre tufts of typha seeds, which enable them to be primarily dispersed by wind, also make them susceptible to being unintentionally picked up and transported by <i>L. fallax</i> as they move through aquatic environments. The moist surfaces of frogs, coupled with their presence in freshwater systems that can be dominated by typha, create favourable conditions for seed attachment. These findings highlight a previously unknown interaction between an amphibian and a plant that enables seed dispersal, supporting the need for further exploration into unexpected plant vectors.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13435","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139092139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reciprocity versus pseudo-reciprocity: A false dichotomy 互惠与假互惠:错误的二分法
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13431
Gerald G. Carter

Reciprocity and pseudo-reciprocity are two important models for the evolution of cooperation and often considered alternative hypotheses. Reciprocity is typically defined as a scenario where help given causes help received: cooperation is stabilized because each actor's cooperative investments are conditional on the cooperative returns from the receiver. Pseudo-reciprocity is a scenario where help enables byproduct returns: cooperation is inherently stable because the actor's cooperative investments yield byproduct returns from the receiver's self-serving behavior. These models are strict alternatives only if reciprocity is defined by the restrictive assumption of zero fitness interdependence, meaning that the helper has no “stake” in the receiver's fitness. Reciprocity and interdependence are, however, not mutually exclusive when helping can increase both reciprocal help and byproduct returns. For instance, helping partners survive can simultaneously increase their willingness to reciprocate, their ability to reciprocate, and byproduct benefits of their existence. Interdependence can “pave the road” to reciprocal helping, and partners who reciprocate help can also become interdependent. However, larger cooperative investments can increase the need for responsiveness to partner returns. Therefore, most long-term cooperative relationships involve both responsiveness and interdependence. Categorizing these relationships as “reciprocity” can be viewed as ignoring interdependence, but calling them ‘pseudo-reciprocity’ is confusing because stability also comes from the cooperative investments being conditional on returns. Rather than conceptualizing cooperation into discrete categories, it is more insightful to imagine a coordinate system with responsiveness and interdependence as continuous dimensions. One can ask: To what degree is helping behavior responsive to the partner's behavior? And to what degree does the helper inherently benefit from the receiver's survival or reproduction? The amounts of responsiveness and interdependence will often be hard to estimate, but both are unlikely to be zero. Identifying their relative importance, and how that changes over time, would greatly clarify the nature of cooperative relationships.

互惠和伪互惠是合作演化的两个重要模型,通常被视为替代性假设。互惠通常被定义为给予的帮助会导致接受的帮助:合作之所以稳定,是因为每个行为者的合作投资都以接受者的合作回报为条件。伪互惠是指帮助能够带来副产品回报:合作本质上是稳定的,因为行为者的合作投资从接受者的自我服务行为中获得了副产品回报。只有当互惠性是由 "相互依赖程度为零 "这一限制性假设来定义时,即帮助者与接受者的相互依赖程度没有 "利害关系 "时,这些模型才是严格的替代模型。然而,当帮助可以增加互惠帮助和副产品回报时,互惠和相互依赖并不相互排斥。例如,帮助伙伴生存可以同时提高他们的互惠意愿、互惠能力以及他们生存的副产品收益。相互依赖可以为互惠帮助 "铺平道路",而互惠帮助的伙伴也会变得相互依赖。然而,较大的合作投资会增加对合作伙伴回报的响应需求。因此,大多数长期合作关系都同时涉及响应性和相互依存性。将这些关系归类为 "互惠 "可能会被视为忽视了相互依赖,但将其称为 "伪互惠 "则会造成混淆,因为稳定性也来自于以回报为条件的合作投资。与其将合作概念化为离散的类别,不如设想一个以响应性和相互依赖性为连续维度的坐标系,这样更有洞察力。我们可以问:帮助行为在多大程度上是对伙伴行为的回应?帮助者在多大程度上从受助者的生存或繁衍中获得了固有利益?反应性和相互依赖性的程度往往难以估计,但两者都不可能为零。确定它们的相对重要性,以及它们如何随着时间的推移而变化,将极大地澄清合作关系的本质。
{"title":"Reciprocity versus pseudo-reciprocity: A false dichotomy","authors":"Gerald G. Carter","doi":"10.1111/eth.13431","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13431","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Reciprocity and pseudo-reciprocity are two important models for the evolution of cooperation and often considered alternative hypotheses. Reciprocity is typically defined as a scenario where help given <i>causes</i> help received: cooperation is stabilized because each actor's cooperative investments are conditional on the cooperative returns from the receiver. Pseudo-reciprocity is a scenario where help <i>enables</i> byproduct returns: cooperation is inherently stable because the actor's cooperative investments yield byproduct returns from the receiver's self-serving behavior. These models are strict alternatives only if reciprocity is defined by the restrictive assumption of zero <i>fitness interdependence</i>, meaning that the helper has no “stake” in the receiver's fitness. Reciprocity and interdependence are, however, not mutually exclusive when helping can increase both reciprocal help and byproduct returns. For instance, helping partners survive can simultaneously increase their willingness to reciprocate, their ability to reciprocate, and byproduct benefits of their existence. Interdependence can “pave the road” to reciprocal helping, and partners who reciprocate help can also become interdependent. However, larger cooperative investments can increase the need for responsiveness to partner returns. Therefore, most long-term cooperative relationships involve both responsiveness and interdependence. Categorizing these relationships as “reciprocity” can be viewed as ignoring interdependence, but calling them ‘pseudo-reciprocity’ is confusing because stability also comes from the cooperative investments being conditional on returns. Rather than conceptualizing cooperation into <i>discrete categories</i>, it is more insightful to imagine a coordinate system with responsiveness and interdependence as <i>continuous dimensions</i>. One can ask: To what degree is helping behavior responsive to the partner's behavior? And to what degree does the helper inherently benefit from the receiver's survival or reproduction? The amounts of responsiveness and interdependence will often be hard to estimate, but both are unlikely to be zero. Identifying their relative importance, and how that changes over time, would greatly clarify the nature of cooperative relationships.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13431","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138949967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On closer inspection: Reviewing the debate on whether fish cooperate to inspect predators 仔细观察:回顾关于鱼类是否合作检查捕食者的争论
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-12-20 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13427
A. Li Veiros, Manon K. Schweinfurth, Michael M. Webster

Cooperative behaviours, which benefit a recipient, are widespread in the animal kingdom; yet their evolution is not straightforward. Reciprocity, i.e., cooperating with previously experienced cooperative partners, has been suggested to underly cooperation, but has been contested throughout the years. Once a textbook example of reciprocity was cooperative predator inspection, where one or several individuals leave their group to approach a potential threat. Each can at any point stop or retreat, increasing the risk for its partner. It was suggested that inspecting individuals follow a specific reciprocal strategy called tit-for-tat, i.e., cooperating on the first move and then copying the partner's last move. Numerous studies provide evidence to support the claim that fish cooperate to inspect predators, including three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus), guppies (Poecilia reticulata) and minnows (Phoxinus phoxinus). However, over the past few decades some scholars have expressed scepticism whether predator inspection is indeed a cooperative behaviour or rather a case of by-product mutualism, which describes behaviours that benefit a partner as a corollary of an otherwise selfish behaviour. For instance, it has been shown that pairs of fish moving in unfamiliar environments appear to coordinate movements even in the absence of predators. Many studies have also used coarse measures of overall approach rates towards predators rather than the fine-grained analyses necessary to infer tit-for-tat in cooperative inspections. Now is the time to return to the question of cooperative predator inspection with new tools and approaches to resolve a decades-old debate.

合作行为使接受者受益,这种行为在动物界非常普遍,但其进化过程却并不简单。互惠性,即与先前有过合作经验的伙伴合作,被认为是合作的基础,但多年来一直存在争议。互惠性的一个典型例子是捕食者合作巡视,即一个或几个个体离开它们的群体去接近潜在的威胁。每个个体都可以在任何时候停止或撤退,从而增加同伴的风险。有研究认为,巡视个体遵循一种特定的互惠策略,即 "针锋相对"(tit-for-tat),即先合作,然后模仿伙伴的最后一招。许多研究都提供了证据来支持鱼类合作检查捕食者的说法,包括三刺粘背鱼(Gasterosteus aculeatus)、河鲈(Poecilia reticulata)和鲦鱼(Phoxinus phoxinus)。然而,在过去几十年中,一些学者对捕食者巡视是否真的是一种合作行为或副产品互利行为表示怀疑。例如,有研究表明,即使在没有捕食者的情况下,在陌生环境中活动的成对鱼类似乎也会协调行动。许多研究还使用了对捕食者总体接近率的粗略测量,而不是在合作检查中推断针锋相对所需的精细分析。现在是时候用新的工具和方法回到捕食者合作巡视的问题上来了,以解决几十年来的争论。
{"title":"On closer inspection: Reviewing the debate on whether fish cooperate to inspect predators","authors":"A. Li Veiros,&nbsp;Manon K. Schweinfurth,&nbsp;Michael M. Webster","doi":"10.1111/eth.13427","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13427","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Cooperative behaviours, which benefit a recipient, are widespread in the animal kingdom; yet their evolution is not straightforward. Reciprocity, i.e., cooperating with previously experienced cooperative partners, has been suggested to underly cooperation, but has been contested throughout the years. Once a textbook example of reciprocity was cooperative predator inspection, where one or several individuals leave their group to approach a potential threat. Each can at any point stop or retreat, increasing the risk for its partner. It was suggested that inspecting individuals follow a specific reciprocal strategy called tit-for-tat, i.e., cooperating on the first move and then copying the partner's last move. Numerous studies provide evidence to support the claim that fish cooperate to inspect predators, including three-spined sticklebacks (<i>Gasterosteus aculeatus</i>), guppies (<i>Poecilia reticulata</i>) and minnows (<i>Phoxinus phoxinus</i>). However, over the past few decades some scholars have expressed scepticism whether predator inspection is indeed a cooperative behaviour or rather a case of by-product mutualism, which describes behaviours that benefit a partner as a corollary of an otherwise selfish behaviour. For instance, it has been shown that pairs of fish moving in unfamiliar environments appear to coordinate movements even in the absence of predators. Many studies have also used coarse measures of overall approach rates towards predators rather than the fine-grained analyses necessary to infer tit-for-tat in cooperative inspections. Now is the time to return to the question of cooperative predator inspection with new tools and approaches to resolve a decades-old debate.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13427","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138823778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Instrumental helping and short-term reciprocity in chimpanzees and human children 黑猩猩和人类儿童的工具性帮助和短期互惠
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13426
Hagen Knofe, Jan M. Engelmann, Sebastian Grueneisen, Esther Herrmann

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans cooperate in reciprocal patterns, but it is unclear whether these interactions are based on the same psychological foundations. While there is evidence suggesting that both species engage in long-term forms of reciprocity, there is very little work exploring their short-term behavioural contingencies with suitable methods. Here, we present a direct comparative study on short-term reciprocity in chimpanzees and human children using a novel, low-cost instrumental helping task. We investigated whether participants help a conspecific partner to obtain a tool for accessing a reward, and whether the level of helping depends on the partner's previous helpful or unhelpful behaviour. In line with prior research, both chimpanzees and children demonstrated helping behaviour towards their partner. However, the extent to which the two species showed short-term reciprocity differed considerably. After receiving help, tested children always helped in return. They helped substantially less when interacting with an unhelpful partner. Chimpanzees showed a higher tendency to help when interacting with a helpful compared to an unhelpful partner only in the first half the experiment. With increasing trial number, chimpanzees stopped discriminating between helpful and unhelpful partners. This study provides evidence for short-term reciprocity in human children and, to a lesser extent, in our closest living relatives. Our findings demonstrate that helping paradigms provide a useful context to investigate reciprocal motives in humans and chimpanzees alike.

黑猩猩(类人猿)和人类以互惠的模式合作,但尚不清楚这些互动是否基于相同的心理基础。虽然有证据表明,这两个物种都有长期的互惠形式,但用合适的方法探索它们的短期行为偶然性的工作却很少。在这里,我们使用一种新颖的、低成本的工具帮助任务,对黑猩猩和人类儿童的短期互惠进行了直接比较研究。我们调查了参与者是否会帮助同伴获得获得奖励的工具,以及帮助的程度是否取决于同伴之前的帮助或不帮助行为。与先前的研究一致,黑猩猩和儿童都表现出对伴侣的帮助行为。然而,这两个物种表现出短期互惠的程度有很大不同。接受帮助后,被测试的孩子总是以帮助作为回报。当他们与不乐于助人的伙伴互动时,他们的帮助会大大减少。只有在实验的前半部分,黑猩猩在与乐于助人的人交往时,比与不乐于助人的人交往时,表现出更高的助人倾向。随着试验次数的增加,黑猩猩不再区分乐于助人和不乐于助人的伴侣。这项研究为人类儿童的短期互惠提供了证据,在较小程度上,也为我们最亲近的亲戚提供了证据。我们的发现表明,帮助范式为研究人类和黑猩猩的互惠动机提供了一个有用的背景。
{"title":"Instrumental helping and short-term reciprocity in chimpanzees and human children","authors":"Hagen Knofe,&nbsp;Jan M. Engelmann,&nbsp;Sebastian Grueneisen,&nbsp;Esther Herrmann","doi":"10.1111/eth.13426","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13426","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Chimpanzees (<i>Pan troglodytes</i>) and humans cooperate in reciprocal patterns, but it is unclear whether these interactions are based on the same psychological foundations. While there is evidence suggesting that both species engage in long-term forms of reciprocity, there is very little work exploring their short-term behavioural contingencies with suitable methods. Here, we present a direct comparative study on short-term reciprocity in chimpanzees and human children using a novel, low-cost instrumental helping task. We investigated whether participants help a conspecific partner to obtain a tool for accessing a reward, and whether the level of helping depends on the partner's previous <i>helpful</i> or <i>unhelpful</i> behaviour. In line with prior research, both chimpanzees and children demonstrated helping behaviour towards their partner. However, the extent to which the two species showed short-term reciprocity differed considerably. After receiving help, tested children always helped in return. They helped substantially less when interacting with an unhelpful partner. Chimpanzees showed a higher tendency to help when interacting with a helpful compared to an unhelpful partner only in the first half the experiment. With increasing trial number, chimpanzees stopped discriminating between helpful and unhelpful partners. This study provides evidence for short-term reciprocity in human children and, to a lesser extent, in our closest living relatives. Our findings demonstrate that helping paradigms provide a useful context to investigate reciprocal motives in humans and chimpanzees alike.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138510157","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chronic acetamiprid exposure moderately affects the foraging behaviour of buff-tailed bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) 长期暴露于啶虫脒中会适度影响黄尾大黄蜂的觅食行为
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-27 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13425
Zoltán Tóth, Zsófia Kovács

Bumblebees are important pollinators in both natural and agricultural ecosystems, but their survival and pollination service are threatened by extensive pesticide use. Due to regulation changes, acetamiprid has become the only neonicotinoid substance that can be used without restrictions and in open-field cultivations in the European Union. Yet, we know little about how this active ingredient affects bumblebees' foraging behaviour and if such effects are similarly detrimental to those of other neonicotinoids. Here we investigated how a 14-day-long (chronic) exposure to low (5 ppb) and high (2500 ppb) concentrations of acetamiprid in syrup affected different aspects of foraging behaviour in buff-tailed bumblebees (Bombus terrestris, Linnaeus). We recorded individual foraging at artificial food patches during one-hour-long trials and then compared several foraging-related measures between differently dosed individuals. We found that 75.12% of the total syrup consumption occurred at the first-exploited patch, but individuals did not exhibit any bias toward pesticide-treated food patches. Chronic exposure to acetamiprid had little effect on individual foraging decisions at the first-exploited food patch but significantly affected the time to the first feeding and the number of feedings at the second-exploited patch in interaction with body mass. The duration of the first feeding was affected only by foragers' body mass. Our finding indicates that chronic exposure to a high but field-realistic concentration of acetamiprid may alter some aspects of bumblebees' foraging behaviour. If such behavioural changes accumulate during consecutive foraging bouts, chronic exposure to this pesticide might lead to a reduction of daily resource collection, ultimately jeopardising colony fitness or plant (crop) pollination.

大黄蜂在自然和农业生态系统中都是重要的传粉媒介,但它们的生存和传粉服务受到农药广泛使用的威胁。由于法规的变化,在欧盟,醋氨脒已成为唯一一种可以不受限制地在露天种植中使用的新烟碱类物质。然而,对于这种活性成分如何影响大黄蜂的觅食行为,以及这种影响是否对其他新烟碱类杀虫剂同样有害,我们知之甚少。在这里,我们研究了14天(慢性)暴露于低(5 ppb)和高(2500 ppb)浓度的糖浆中醋氨虫如何影响黄尾大黄蜂(Bombus terrestris, Linnaeus)觅食行为的不同方面。我们在一小时的试验中记录了个体在人工食物斑块上的觅食行为,然后比较了不同剂量个体之间的几种与觅食相关的措施。我们发现,75.12%的糖浆总消耗量发生在第一次开发的斑块,但个体对农药处理的食物斑块没有表现出任何偏见。长期暴露于啶虫脒对个体在第一个食物斑块的觅食决策影响不大,但对第一个食物斑块的第一次进食时间和第二次进食次数有显著影响,并与体重相互作用。第一次进食的持续时间只受觅食者体重的影响。我们的发现表明,长期暴露于高但实际浓度的醋氨虫可能会改变大黄蜂觅食行为的某些方面。如果这种行为变化在连续的觅食过程中积累,长期暴露于这种农药可能导致每日资源收集减少,最终危及群体适应性或植物(作物)授粉。
{"title":"Chronic acetamiprid exposure moderately affects the foraging behaviour of buff-tailed bumblebees (Bombus terrestris)","authors":"Zoltán Tóth,&nbsp;Zsófia Kovács","doi":"10.1111/eth.13425","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13425","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Bumblebees are important pollinators in both natural and agricultural ecosystems, but their survival and pollination service are threatened by extensive pesticide use. Due to regulation changes, acetamiprid has become the only neonicotinoid substance that can be used without restrictions and in open-field cultivations in the European Union. Yet, we know little about how this active ingredient affects bumblebees' foraging behaviour and if such effects are similarly detrimental to those of other neonicotinoids. Here we investigated how a 14-day-long (chronic) exposure to low (5 ppb) and high (2500 ppb) concentrations of acetamiprid in syrup affected different aspects of foraging behaviour in buff-tailed bumblebees (<i>Bombus terrestris</i>, Linnaeus). We recorded individual foraging at artificial food patches during one-hour-long trials and then compared several foraging-related measures between differently dosed individuals. We found that 75.12% of the total syrup consumption occurred at the first-exploited patch, but individuals did not exhibit any bias toward pesticide-treated food patches. Chronic exposure to acetamiprid had little effect on individual foraging decisions at the first-exploited food patch but significantly affected the time to the first feeding and the number of feedings at the second-exploited patch in interaction with body mass. The duration of the first feeding was affected only by foragers' body mass. Our finding indicates that chronic exposure to a high but field-realistic concentration of acetamiprid may alter some aspects of bumblebees' foraging behaviour. If such behavioural changes accumulate during consecutive foraging bouts, chronic exposure to this pesticide might lead to a reduction of daily resource collection, ultimately jeopardising colony fitness or plant (crop) pollination.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138510156","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A little on the nose: A mosquito targets the nostrils of tree frogs for a blood meal 鼻子上的一点:一只蚊子瞄准树蛙的鼻孔吸血
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-21 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13424
John Gould, Jose W. Valdez

As the females of most mosquitoes require a blood meal to provision their eggs, they can be a vector of parasites and pathogens that have profound impacts on both animal and human populations. Understanding the interactions between mosquitoes and their hosts, including animal and feeding site preferences, could thus provide valuable insights into disease transmission. In this study, we present a highly specific feeding strategy observed in a mosquito from Australia (Mimomyia elegans). Over 3 years of fieldwork, we found this mosquito feeding exclusively on the nostrils when using several amphibian species as hosts. Mosquitoes were observed initially landing on the backs of hosts before walking towards the nostrils, potentially minimizing detection and mitigating the risk of predation or defensive responses. This remarkable level of feeding site specificity suggests strong selection pressures that are causing the nostril to be the optimal location for blood extraction on amphibians. Possible explanations include the presence of thinner, more accessible skin tissue or enhanced vascular properties in the nostril region. Understanding mosquito host preferences, points of contact and feeding site specificity could provide valuable insights into disease transmission among amphibians, as mosquitoes have been identified as potential vectors of pathogens like the amphibian chytrid fungus.

由于大多数雌性蚊子需要吸血来产卵,它们可能是寄生虫和病原体的载体,对动物和人类种群都有深远的影响。因此,了解蚊子与其宿主之间的相互作用,包括动物和觅食地点的偏好,可以为疾病传播提供有价值的见解。在这项研究中,我们提出了一种在澳大利亚的一种蚊子(Mimomyia elegans)中观察到的高度特异性的摄食策略。在3年的田野调查中,我们发现这种蚊子在以几种两栖动物为宿主时,只以鼻孔为食。观察到蚊子最初落在宿主的背上,然后走向鼻孔,这可能最大限度地减少发现并减轻捕食或防御反应的风险。这种显著的取血部位特异性表明,强大的选择压力使鼻孔成为两栖动物取血的最佳位置。可能的解释包括存在更薄、更容易接触的皮肤组织或鼻孔区域血管特性增强。了解蚊子的宿主偏好、接触点和取食部位特异性可以为两栖动物之间的疾病传播提供有价值的见解,因为蚊子已被确定为两栖动物壶菌等病原体的潜在载体。
{"title":"A little on the nose: A mosquito targets the nostrils of tree frogs for a blood meal","authors":"John Gould,&nbsp;Jose W. Valdez","doi":"10.1111/eth.13424","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13424","url":null,"abstract":"<p>As the females of most mosquitoes require a blood meal to provision their eggs, they can be a vector of parasites and pathogens that have profound impacts on both animal and human populations. Understanding the interactions between mosquitoes and their hosts, including animal and feeding site preferences, could thus provide valuable insights into disease transmission. In this study, we present a highly specific feeding strategy observed in a mosquito from Australia (<i>Mimomyia elegans</i>). Over 3 years of fieldwork, we found this mosquito feeding exclusively on the nostrils when using several amphibian species as hosts. Mosquitoes were observed initially landing on the backs of hosts before walking towards the nostrils, potentially minimizing detection and mitigating the risk of predation or defensive responses. This remarkable level of feeding site specificity suggests strong selection pressures that are causing the nostril to be the optimal location for blood extraction on amphibians. Possible explanations include the presence of thinner, more accessible skin tissue or enhanced vascular properties in the nostril region. Understanding mosquito host preferences, points of contact and feeding site specificity could provide valuable insights into disease transmission among amphibians, as mosquitoes have been identified as potential vectors of pathogens like the amphibian chytrid fungus.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13424","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138510192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive and sensory capacity each contribute to the canine spatial bias 认知和感觉能力都对犬的空间偏好有影响
IF 1.7 4区 生物学 Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2023-11-19 DOI: 10.1111/eth.13423
Ivaylo Borislavov Iotchev, Zsófia Bognár, Soufiane Bel Rhali, Enikő Kubinyi

Dogs interpret cues as being about location, which human infants would relate to objects. This spatial bias could shed light on the evolution of object-centered thought, however, research needs to rule out that this is not a by-product of dogs' weaker (compared to humans) visual capacities. In this study, we used a data set in which dogs were tested in two types of learning tasks (discrimination and reversal learning) with two types of rewarded cues (location and object features). In both tasks, dogs displayed spatial bias, that is, faster learning when the rewarded cue was a location. We investigated how sensory and cognitive capacity each contributes to this spatial bias. To this end, an estimate for general cognitive ability (g) was obtained from a battery of tests for some of the dogs. Cephalic index, a feature targeted in breeding and linked to differences in visual capacity, correlated negatively with the expression of spatial bias only in the easier discrimination learning task, while a negative correlation between g factor and spatial bias scores emerged in the more difficult reversal learning task. We conclude that dogs' spatial bias cannot be reduced to a sensory limitation and is easier to overcome with greater cognitive capacity.

狗将线索解释为位置,而人类婴儿则将其与物体联系起来。这种空间偏差可以揭示以物体为中心的思维的进化,然而,研究需要排除这不是狗(与人类相比)视觉能力较弱的副产品。在这项研究中,我们使用了一个数据集,其中狗在两种类型的学习任务(辨别和反转学习)中接受两种类型的奖励线索(位置和物体特征)的测试。在这两项任务中,狗都表现出空间偏见,即当奖励线索是一个位置时,它们的学习速度更快。我们研究了感觉和认知能力是如何导致这种空间偏差的。为此,对一些狗进行了一系列测试,得出了一般认知能力(g)的估计值。头侧指数(Cephalic index)是一种以繁殖为目标的特征,与视觉能力差异有关,仅在较容易的辨别学习任务中与空间偏差的表达呈负相关,而在较困难的反转学习任务中,g因子与空间偏差得分呈负相关。我们得出的结论是,狗的空间偏见不能归结为一种感官限制,而更容易通过更强的认知能力来克服。
{"title":"Cognitive and sensory capacity each contribute to the canine spatial bias","authors":"Ivaylo Borislavov Iotchev,&nbsp;Zsófia Bognár,&nbsp;Soufiane Bel Rhali,&nbsp;Enikő Kubinyi","doi":"10.1111/eth.13423","DOIUrl":"10.1111/eth.13423","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Dogs interpret cues as being about location, which human infants would relate to objects. This spatial bias could shed light on the evolution of object-centered thought, however, research needs to rule out that this is not a by-product of dogs' weaker (compared to humans) visual capacities. In this study, we used a data set in which dogs were tested in two types of learning tasks (discrimination and reversal learning) with two types of rewarded cues (location and object features). In both tasks, dogs displayed spatial bias, that is, faster learning when the rewarded cue was a location. We investigated how sensory and cognitive capacity each contributes to this spatial bias. To this end, an estimate for general cognitive ability (g) was obtained from a battery of tests for some of the dogs. Cephalic index, a feature targeted in breeding and linked to differences in visual capacity, correlated negatively with the expression of spatial bias only in the easier discrimination learning task, while a negative correlation between g factor and spatial bias scores emerged in the more difficult reversal learning task. We conclude that dogs' spatial bias cannot be reduced to a sensory limitation and is easier to overcome with greater cognitive capacity.</p>","PeriodicalId":50494,"journal":{"name":"Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2023-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/eth.13423","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138510191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"生物学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1