首页 > 最新文献

Applied Neuropsychology-Adult最新文献

英文 中文
A further study of the psychometric qualities of the Strange Stories-Revised across the three stages of aging. 进一步研究《奇异故事-修订版》在老龄化三个阶段的心理测量质量。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2022-08-08 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2022.2109419
Catherine Gourlay, Pascal Collin, Camille D'Auteuil, Marie Jacques, Peter B Scherzer

Introduction: Happé's Strange Stories task was developed in 1994 to assess theory of mind, the ability to infer mental states in oneself and others. Since then, it has undergone revisions, translations, and adaptations. A modified version of the task, the Strange Stories-Revised (SS-R), previously showed satisfactory qualities in a study aiming at identifying psychometrically acceptable social cognitive measures.

Objective: The current study expands upon the psychometric evaluation study by examining the qualities of a short version of the SS-R in a sample of healthy adult subjects.

Methods: One hundred and eighteen healthy adults completed the task along with neurocognitive measures. Mean scores of the long and short versions were compared. Associations between ToM as measured by performance on this abbreviated version of the SS-R, and potential confounders were explored. Internal consistency, dimensionality of the short version, and performance comparisons across three stages of aging (18-34; 35-59; 60-85 years old) were investigated, and standard measurement error was calculated to improve precision and data interpretation.

Results: Reliability coefficients were comparable in the short and long versions. Principal component analysis showed that a one-factor structure best fits the data. Significant differences were observed in ToM performance across the three age groups, indicating a decline with time that was also captured by the long version, starting during midlife and increasing in significance with age.

Conclusion: The short version of the SS-R is a promising measure that can be profitably used in time-limited settings to assess theory of mind.

简介Happé 的 "奇怪的故事 "任务开发于 1994 年,用于评估心智理论,即推断自己和他人心理状态的能力。从那时起,它经历了修订、翻译和改编。在一项旨在确定心理测量学上可接受的社会认知测量的研究中,该任务的修订版--"奇怪的故事-修订版"(SS-R)--显示出令人满意的质量:本研究在心理测量评估研究的基础上,对健康成人样本中的短版 SS-R 进行了质量检测:方法:118 名健康成年人在完成任务的同时进行了神经认知测量。比较了长版和短版的平均得分。研究人员还探讨了以该缩写版 SS-R 的表现来衡量的 ToM 与潜在混杂因素之间的关联。研究了短版的内部一致性、维度和三个老龄化阶段(18-34 岁;35-59 岁;60-85 岁)的成绩比较,并计算了标准测量误差,以提高精确度和数据解释能力:长、短问卷的信度系数相当。主成分分析表明,单因素结构最适合数据。在三个年龄组中观察到 ToM 表现的显著差异,表明随着时间的推移,ToM 表现有所下降,长版本也捕捉到了这种下降,从中年开始,随着年龄的增长,下降幅度越来越大:结论:SS-R 的短版是一种很有前途的测量方法,可以在有时间限制的情况下用于评估心智理论。
{"title":"A further study of the psychometric qualities of the Strange Stories-Revised across the three stages of aging.","authors":"Catherine Gourlay, Pascal Collin, Camille D'Auteuil, Marie Jacques, Peter B Scherzer","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2109419","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2109419","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Happé's Strange Stories task was developed in 1994 to assess theory of mind, the ability to infer mental states in oneself and others. Since then, it has undergone revisions, translations, and adaptations. A modified version of the task, the Strange Stories-Revised (SS-R), previously showed satisfactory qualities in a study aiming at identifying psychometrically acceptable social cognitive measures.</p><p><strong>Objective: </strong>The current study expands upon the psychometric evaluation study by examining the qualities of a short version of the SS-R in a sample of healthy adult subjects.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>One hundred and eighteen healthy adults completed the task along with neurocognitive measures. Mean scores of the long and short versions were compared. Associations between ToM as measured by performance on this abbreviated version of the SS-R, and potential confounders were explored. Internal consistency, dimensionality of the short version, and performance comparisons across three stages of aging (18-34; 35-59; 60-85 years old) were investigated, and standard measurement error was calculated to improve precision and data interpretation.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Reliability coefficients were comparable in the short and long versions. Principal component analysis showed that a one-factor structure best fits the data. Significant differences were observed in ToM performance across the three age groups, indicating a decline with time that was also captured by the long version, starting during midlife and increasing in significance with age.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The short version of the SS-R is a promising measure that can be profitably used in time-limited settings to assess theory of mind.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40610597","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age and neurocognition are associated with credibility evaluations of health websites. 年龄和神经认知与健康网站的可信度评价有关。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-24 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2022.2096453
Michelle A Babicz, Samina Rahman, Victoria M Kordovski, Savanna M Tierney, Steven Paul Woods

The internet has become a common means by which many older adults seek out health information. The prevalence of misinformation on the internet makes the search for accurate online health information a more complex and evaluative process. This study examined the role of age and neurocognition in credibility evaluations of credible and non-credible health websites. Forty-one older adults and fifty younger adults completed a structured credibility rating task in which they evaluated a series of webpages displaying health information about migraine treatments. Participants also completed measures of neurocognition, internet use, and health literacy. Results suggested that older adults rated non-credible health websites as more credible than younger adults, but the age groups did not differ in their ratings of credible sites. Within the full sample, neurocognition was associated with credibility ratings for non-credible health websites, whereas health literacy was related to the ratings of credible sites. Findings indicate that older adults may be more likely to trust non-credible health websites than younger adults, which may be related to differences in higher-order neurocognitive functions. Future work might examine whether cognitive-based supports for credibility training in older adults can be used to improve the accuracy with which they evaluate online health information.

互联网已成为许多老年人寻求健康信息的常用手段。互联网上错误信息的盛行使得寻找准确的在线健康信息成为一个更加复杂和需要评估的过程。本研究探讨了年龄和神经认知在可信和不可信健康网站可信度评价中的作用。41 名老年人和 50 名年轻人完成了一项结构化可信度评级任务,对一系列显示偏头痛治疗健康信息的网页进行了评估。参与者还完成了神经认知、互联网使用和健康素养的测量。结果表明,与年轻人相比,老年人对非可信健康网站的评分更高,但两个年龄组对可信网站的评分并无差异。在全部样本中,神经认知与非可信健康网站的可信度评级有关,而健康素养与可信网站的评级有关。研究结果表明,与年轻人相比,老年人可能更容易相信不可信的健康网站,这可能与高阶神经认知功能的差异有关。未来的工作可能会研究是否可以利用基于认知的老年人可信度培训支持来提高他们评估在线健康信息的准确性。
{"title":"Age and neurocognition are associated with credibility evaluations of health websites.","authors":"Michelle A Babicz, Samina Rahman, Victoria M Kordovski, Savanna M Tierney, Steven Paul Woods","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2096453","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2096453","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The internet has become a common means by which many older adults seek out health information. The prevalence of misinformation on the internet makes the search for accurate online health information a more complex and evaluative process. This study examined the role of age and neurocognition in credibility evaluations of credible and non-credible health websites. Forty-one older adults and fifty younger adults completed a structured credibility rating task in which they evaluated a series of webpages displaying health information about migraine treatments. Participants also completed measures of neurocognition, internet use, and health literacy. Results suggested that older adults rated non-credible health websites as more credible than younger adults, but the age groups did not differ in their ratings of credible sites. Within the full sample, neurocognition was associated with credibility ratings for non-credible health websites, whereas health literacy was related to the ratings of credible sites. Findings indicate that older adults may be more likely to trust non-credible health websites than younger adults, which may be related to differences in higher-order neurocognitive functions. Future work might examine whether cognitive-based supports for credibility training in older adults can be used to improve the accuracy with which they evaluate online health information.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40629536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of task novelty for age cohort and cognition level on memory for everyday virtual meal preparation tasks 年龄组和认知水平的任务新颖性对日常虚拟备餐任务记忆的影响
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-27 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2024.2377383
Michael Barnett, Michael Persin, Harrison Boynton
Nonsensical information increases task novelty, which makes it difficult to rely on previous learning and provides insight into the learning of new tasks. This study investigated procedural-based a...
无意义的信息会增加任务的新颖性,从而使人难以依赖以前的学习,并为新任务的学习提供启示。本研究调查了基于程序的...
{"title":"The effects of task novelty for age cohort and cognition level on memory for everyday virtual meal preparation tasks","authors":"Michael Barnett, Michael Persin, Harrison Boynton","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2024.2377383","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2024.2377383","url":null,"abstract":"Nonsensical information increases task novelty, which makes it difficult to rely on previous learning and provides insight into the learning of new tasks. This study investigated procedural-based a...","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141779136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The modified Ruche visuospatial learning test (RUCHE-M) for the assessment of visuospatial episodic memory in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: Preliminary evidence for the investigation of memory binding. 用于评估颞叶癫痫患者视觉空间外显记忆的改良鲁切视觉空间学习测试(RUCHE-M):调查记忆束缚的初步证据。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-02-08 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2022.2031200
Nicolle Zimmermann, Monique Pontes, Rosana da Silva Fontana, Isabella D'Andrea Meira, Rochele Fonseca, François Jean Delaere

Introduction: The Ruche test is a visuospatial form of the Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT), with initial evidence of utility in the diagnosis of temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE)-related memory disorders.

Aims: To present the translation to Brazilian Portuguese and modification of the Ruche test (RUCHE-M) and compare the RUCHE-M and RAVLT performance between patients with right and left TLE.

Methods: Twenty-five neuropsychologists participated in instrument adaptation. Thirty-seven patients with right (n = 19) and left (n = 18) TLE participated. Data were compared with the Mann-Whitney U test.

Results: All specialists considered the final RUCHE-M to be adequate. The RUCHE-M forgetting speed index (FSI) score and several RAVLT scores differed significantly between patients with right and left TLE.

Conclusion: The RUCHE-M showed limited utility for the assessment of visuospatial episodic memory in patients with TLE. The manipulation of memory binding as demonstrated by FSI score seems to be a promising paradigm for the assessment of right hippocampal function.

简介Ruche测试是雷伊听觉言语学习测试(RAVLT)的一种视觉空间形式,有初步证据表明该测试可用于诊断颞叶癫痫(TLE)相关记忆障碍。目的:介绍Ruche测试(RUCHE-M)的巴西葡萄牙语翻译和修改情况,并比较右侧和左侧TLE患者的RUCHE-M和RAVLT表现:25名神经心理学家参与了测试工具的改编工作。37名右侧(n = 19)和左侧(n = 18)TLE患者参加了此次活动。数据比较采用曼-惠特尼U检验:结果:所有专家都认为最终的RUCHE-M是合适的。左右侧TLE患者的RUCHE-M遗忘速度指数(FSI)得分和几项RAVLT得分存在显著差异:结论:RUCHE-M在评估TLE患者的视觉空间外显记忆方面作用有限。结论:RUCHE-M在评估TLE患者的视觉空间外显记忆方面作用有限,而通过FSI评分对记忆束缚的操作似乎是评估右侧海马功能的一个很有前途的范例。
{"title":"The modified Ruche visuospatial learning test (RUCHE-M) for the assessment of visuospatial episodic memory in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: Preliminary evidence for the investigation of memory binding.","authors":"Nicolle Zimmermann, Monique Pontes, Rosana da Silva Fontana, Isabella D'Andrea Meira, Rochele Fonseca, François Jean Delaere","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2031200","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2031200","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>The Ruche test is a visuospatial form of the Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT), with initial evidence of utility in the diagnosis of temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE)-related memory disorders.</p><p><strong>Aims: </strong>To present the translation to Brazilian Portuguese and modification of the Ruche test (RUCHE-M) and compare the RUCHE-M and RAVLT performance between patients with right and left TLE.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Twenty-five neuropsychologists participated in instrument adaptation. Thirty-seven patients with right (<i>n</i> = 19) and left (<i>n</i> = 18) TLE participated. Data were compared with the Mann-Whitney <i>U</i> test.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>All specialists considered the final RUCHE-M to be adequate. The RUCHE-M forgetting speed index (FSI) score and several RAVLT scores differed significantly between patients with right and left TLE.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The RUCHE-M showed limited utility for the assessment of visuospatial episodic memory in patients with TLE. The manipulation of memory binding as demonstrated by FSI score seems to be a promising paradigm for the assessment of right hippocampal function.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39602957","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of visual memory impairment on Victoria Symptom Validity Test performance: A known-groups analysis. 视觉记忆障碍对维多利亚症状有效性测试成绩的影响:已知组分析
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-01-05 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2021.2021911
Jane K Stocks, Allison N Shields, Adam B DeBoer, Brian M Cerny, Caitlin M Ogram Buckley, Gabriel P Ovsiew, Kyle J Jennette, Zachary J Resch, Karen S Basurto, Woojin Song, Neil H Pliskin, Jason R Soble

Objective: We assessed the effect of visual learning and recall impairment on Victoria Symptom Validity Test (VSVT) accuracy and response latency for Easy, Difficult, and Total Items.

Method: A sample of 163 adult patients administered the VSVT and Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised were classified as valid (114/163) or invalid (49/163) groups via independent criterion performance validity tests (PVTs). Classification accuracies for all VSVT indices were examined for the overall sample, and separately for subgroups based on visual memory functioning.

Results: In the overall sample, all indices produced acceptable classification accuracy (areas under the curve [AUCs] ≥ 0.79). When stratified by visual learning/recall impairment, accuracy indices yielded acceptable classification for both the unimpaired (AUCs ≥0.79) and impaired subsamples (AUCs ≥0.75). Latency indices had acceptable classification accuracy for the unimpaired subsample (AUCs ≥0.74), but accuracy and sensitivity dropped for the impaired sample (AUCs ≥0.67).

Conclusions: VSVT accuracy and response latency yielded acceptable classification accuracies in the overall sample, and this effect was maintained in those with and without visual learning/recall impairment for the accuracy indices. Findings indicate that the VSVT is a psychometrically robust PVT with largely invariant cut-scores, even in the presence of bona fide visual learning/recall impairment.

目的:我们评估了视觉学习和回忆障碍对维多利亚症状效度测验(VSVT)中简单、困难和总分项目的准确性和反应延迟的影响:我们评估了视觉学习和记忆障碍对维多利亚症状有效性测验(VSVT)的准确性以及对简单、困难和总项目的反应潜伏期的影响:对163名成年患者进行维多利亚症状有效性测验(VSVT)和简明视觉空间记忆测验(Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised),通过独立的标准表现有效性测验(PVT)将其分为有效组(114/163)和无效组(49/163)。对总体样本和基于视觉记忆功能的亚组分别检测了所有 VSVT 指数的分类准确性:在总体样本中,所有指数的分类准确度均可接受(曲线下面积 [AUC] ≥ 0.79)。如果按视觉学习/记忆障碍进行分层,未受损子样本(AUC ≥0.79)和受损子样本(AUC ≥0.75)的准确度指数都能产生可接受的分类。未受损子样本的潜伏期指数具有可接受的分类准确性(AUC ≥0.74),但受损样本的准确性和灵敏度下降(AUC ≥0.67):结论:VSVT 的准确度和反应潜伏期在总体样本中产生了可接受的分类准确度,而且在有视觉学习/记忆障碍和无视觉学习/记忆障碍的样本中,准确度指数都保持了这种效果。研究结果表明,即使存在真正的视觉学习/记忆障碍,VSVT 也是一种心理计量学上稳健的 PVT,其切分分数基本不变。
{"title":"The impact of visual memory impairment on Victoria Symptom Validity Test performance: A known-groups analysis.","authors":"Jane K Stocks, Allison N Shields, Adam B DeBoer, Brian M Cerny, Caitlin M Ogram Buckley, Gabriel P Ovsiew, Kyle J Jennette, Zachary J Resch, Karen S Basurto, Woojin Song, Neil H Pliskin, Jason R Soble","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2021.2021911","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2021.2021911","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>We assessed the effect of visual learning and recall impairment on Victoria Symptom Validity Test (VSVT) accuracy and response latency for Easy, Difficult, and Total Items.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A sample of 163 adult patients administered the VSVT and Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised were classified as valid (114/163) or invalid (49/163) groups via independent criterion performance validity tests (PVTs). Classification accuracies for all VSVT indices were examined for the overall sample, and separately for subgroups based on visual memory functioning.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In the overall sample, all indices produced acceptable classification accuracy (areas under the curve [AUCs] ≥ 0.79). When stratified by visual learning/recall impairment, accuracy indices yielded acceptable classification for both the unimpaired (AUCs ≥0.79) and impaired subsamples (AUCs ≥0.75). Latency indices had acceptable classification accuracy for the unimpaired subsample (AUCs ≥0.74), but accuracy and sensitivity dropped for the impaired sample (AUCs ≥0.67).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>VSVT accuracy and response latency yielded acceptable classification accuracies in the overall sample, and this effect was maintained in those with and without visual learning/recall impairment for the accuracy indices. Findings indicate that the VSVT is a psychometrically robust PVT with largely invariant cut-scores, even in the presence of bona fide visual learning/recall impairment.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39786109","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Computerized social cognitive training in the subacute phase after traumatic brain injury: A quasi-randomized controlled trial. 脑外伤后亚急性期的计算机化社会认知训练:准随机对照试验。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-02-23 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2022.2042693
P Rodríguez-Rajo, A García-Rudolph, R Sánchez-Carrión, C Aparicio-López, A Enseñat-Cantallops, A García-Molina

Objectives: To assess the usefulness of a computerized tasks module designed for the rehabilitation of social cognition (SC) in acquired brain injury.

Methods: Quasi-randomized controlled trial (ClinicalTrials.gov:NCT03479970) involving 45 patients with moderate-severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) in a subacute inpatient rehabilitation hospital. The experimental group (n = 28) received treatment with a computerized SC module in combination with a non-SC module. The control group (n = 26) only received a treatment with non-SC module.

Results: Intragroup comparisons showed that the experimental group had better results for all SC measures, except for International Affective Picture System (IAPS). The control group improved for Facial Expressions of cEmotion-Stimuli and Tests (FEEST) and Moving Shapes Paradigm (MSP), showing no changes with respect to pretreatment in IAPS, MSP and Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET). Intergroup comparisons did not present differences between the two groups for pretreatment measures. Post-treatment comparison showed that the experimental group obtained better results for RMET than the control group.

Conclusion: The computerized SC module was useful for the rehabilitation of SC in patients with moderate-severe TBI in the subacute phase. The group that received combined rehabilitative treatment (SC + non-SC) obtained better results for SC than the group that received treatment intended only for non-SC.

目的评估为后天性脑损伤患者的社会认知(SC)康复设计的计算机化任务模块的实用性:准随机对照试验(ClinicalTrials.gov:NCT03479970)涉及一家亚急性住院康复医院的 45 名中重度创伤性脑损伤(TBI)患者。实验组(28 人)接受计算机化 SC 模块与非 SC 模块相结合的治疗。对照组(26 人)只接受了非 SC 模块的治疗:组内比较显示,除国际情感图像系统(IAPS)外,实验组在所有 SC 测量中都取得了更好的结果。对照组在 "面部表情运动刺激和测试"(FEEST)和 "移动形状范式"(MSP)方面有所改善,而在 "国际情感图片系统"(IAPS)、"移动形状范式"(MSP)和 "读心测试"(RMET)方面与治疗前相比没有变化。组间比较显示,两组在治疗前的测量结果没有差异。治疗后的比较显示,实验组的 RMET 成绩优于对照组:结论:计算机化的SC模块对中重度创伤性脑损伤亚急性期患者的SC康复很有帮助。接受联合康复治疗(SC + 非 SC)的实验组比只接受非 SC 治疗的实验组在 SC 方面取得了更好的效果。
{"title":"Computerized social cognitive training in the subacute phase after traumatic brain injury: A quasi-randomized controlled trial.","authors":"P Rodríguez-Rajo, A García-Rudolph, R Sánchez-Carrión, C Aparicio-López, A Enseñat-Cantallops, A García-Molina","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2042693","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2042693","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objectives: </strong>To assess the usefulness of a computerized tasks module designed for the rehabilitation of social cognition (SC) in acquired brain injury.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Quasi-randomized controlled trial (ClinicalTrials.gov:NCT03479970) involving 45 patients with moderate-severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) in a subacute inpatient rehabilitation hospital. The experimental group (<i>n</i> = 28) received treatment with a computerized SC module in combination with a non-SC module. The control group (<i>n</i> = 26) only received a treatment with non-SC module.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Intragroup comparisons showed that the experimental group had better results for all SC measures, except for International Affective Picture System (IAPS). The control group improved for Facial Expressions of cEmotion-Stimuli and Tests (FEEST) and Moving Shapes Paradigm (MSP), showing no changes with respect to pretreatment in IAPS, MSP and Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET). Intergroup comparisons did not present differences between the two groups for pretreatment measures. Post-treatment comparison showed that the experimental group obtained better results for RMET than the control group.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The computerized SC module was useful for the rehabilitation of SC in patients with moderate-severe TBI in the subacute phase. The group that received combined rehabilitative treatment (SC + non-SC) obtained better results for SC than the group that received treatment intended only for non-SC.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39657384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does the d2 Test of Attention only assess sustained attention? Evidence of working memory processes involved. d2 注意力测试只评估持续注意吗?涉及工作记忆过程的证据。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-01-08 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2021.2023152
Leandro da Silva-Sauer, Ricardo Basso Garcia, Alan Ehrich de Moura, Bernardino Fernández-Calvo

The d2 Test of Attention (d2) is widely used for assessing sustained attention and we aimed at verifying whether working memory may be a secondary construct measured by d2. 70 university students were assessed using d2 conventional paper-and-pencil and computational version. The experimental group and control group performed the task with or without target key, respectively. Continuous Performance Test (CPT) and N-back (1 and 2-back) tasks were used to measure sustained attention and working memory, respectively. Computational d2 performance was predicted by CPT (p < .05; R2 = .15) in the experimental group, and it was predicted by 2-back (p < .05; R2 = .28) in the control group. Conventional d2 performance was predicted by 2-back for both control group (p = .01; R2 = .20) and experimental group (p = .02, R2 = .17). Results suggest the involvement of working memory in d2, possibly a secondary construct assessed by this instrument.

d2 注意力测验(d2)被广泛用于评估持续注意,我们的目的是验证工作记忆是否可能是 d2 测定的二级结构。我们使用传统纸笔版和计算版 d2 对 70 名大学生进行了评估。实验组和对照组分别在有目标键或无目标键的情况下完成任务。持续表现测试(CPT)和N-back(1和2-back)任务分别用于测量持续注意力和工作记忆。在实验组中,计算 d2 成绩是由 CPT 预测的(p < .05;R2 = .15),而在对照组中,计算 d2 成绩是由 2-back 预测的(p < .05;R2 = .28)。对照组(p = .01; R2 = .20)和实验组(p = .02, R2 = .17)的常规 d2 成绩均可通过 2-back 预测。结果表明,工作记忆参与了 d2,这可能是该工具评估的次要结构。
{"title":"Does the d2 Test of Attention only assess sustained attention? Evidence of working memory processes involved.","authors":"Leandro da Silva-Sauer, Ricardo Basso Garcia, Alan Ehrich de Moura, Bernardino Fernández-Calvo","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2021.2023152","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2021.2023152","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The d2 Test of Attention (d2) is widely used for assessing sustained attention and we aimed at verifying whether working memory may be a secondary construct measured by d2. 70 university students were assessed using d2 conventional paper-and-pencil and computational version. The experimental group and control group performed the task with or without target key, respectively. Continuous Performance Test (CPT) and N-back (1 and 2-back) tasks were used to measure sustained attention and working memory, respectively. Computational d2 performance was predicted by CPT (<i>p</i> < .05; <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = .15) in the experimental group, and it was predicted by 2-back (<i>p</i> < .05; <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = .28) in the control group. Conventional d2 performance was predicted by 2-back for both control group (<i>p</i> = .01; <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = .20) and experimental group (<i>p</i> = .02, <i>R</i><sup>2</sup> = .17). Results suggest the involvement of working memory in d2, possibly a secondary construct assessed by this instrument.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39799043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptual lateralization on the Rod-and-Frame Test in young and older adults. 年轻人和老年人在棒框测验中的知觉侧化。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-02-09 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2022.2030741
Rima Abdul Razzak, Jeff Bagust

Introduction: There is an overall left visual field/right hemisphere advantage in young adults for masked, tachistoscopically presented images on the Rod-and-Frame Test (RFT). This study explored potential age-related lateralization differences in processing of visual context on the RFT.

Methods: The 35 young and 33 older adults aligned a rod surrounded either by no frame, a vertical, or leftward/rightward tilted frame to their perceived vertical. Algebraic errors of rod alignment were used to derive the rod-and-frame effect (RFE) and asymmetry index.

Results: Young adults had frequent indirect effects, mostly to the right-tilted frame, while older adults hardly produced any. Compared with nontilted frames, young adults displayed larger alignment errors with left-tilted frames; however, older adults exhibited this same effect for both frame tilt conditions. Young adults had smaller RFE values than older adults for the right-tilted frame, with no age-related difference in RFE for the left-tilted frame or asymmetry index. The negative asymmetry index was statistically different from the true vertical only in young adults.

Conclusion: There is an age-related reduction in the right hemisphere processing of left-sided visual contexts on the RFT. Such findings can assist clinicians to improve interpretation of RFT findings in clinical patients.

简介在棒框测验(RFT)中,年轻成人对遮蔽的、由测速镜呈现的图像具有总体左侧视野/右侧大脑半球优势。本研究探讨了在 RFT 中处理视觉背景时可能存在的与年龄相关的侧化差异:方法:35 名年轻人和 33 名老年人将一根周围没有框架、垂直或向左/向右倾斜的框架的杆对准他们感知到的垂直方向。利用杆对齐的代数误差得出杆与框架效应(RFE)和不对称指数:结果:青壮年经常出现间接效应,主要是对右倾斜框架的间接效应,而老年人几乎不产生间接效应。与非倾斜框架相比,青壮年对左倾斜框架的对齐误差更大;然而,老年人在两种框架倾斜条件下都表现出同样的效应。在右倾斜框架下,青壮年的对齐误差值小于老年人,而在左倾斜框架或不对称指数下,青壮年的对齐误差值与年龄无关。只有年轻人的负不对称指数与真实垂直指数存在统计学差异:结论:在 RFT 中,右半球对左侧视觉情境的处理会随着年龄的增长而减少。这些发现可以帮助临床医生更好地解释临床患者的 RFT 发现。
{"title":"Perceptual lateralization on the Rod-and-Frame Test in young and older adults.","authors":"Rima Abdul Razzak, Jeff Bagust","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2030741","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2030741","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>There is an overall left visual field/right hemisphere advantage in young adults for masked, tachistoscopically presented images on the Rod-and-Frame Test (RFT). This study explored potential age-related lateralization differences in processing of visual context on the RFT.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>The 35 young and 33 older adults aligned a rod surrounded either by no frame, a vertical, or leftward/rightward tilted frame to their perceived vertical. Algebraic errors of rod alignment were used to derive the rod-and-frame effect (RFE) and asymmetry index.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Young adults had frequent indirect effects, mostly to the right-tilted frame, while older adults hardly produced any. Compared with nontilted frames, young adults displayed larger alignment errors with left-tilted frames; however, older adults exhibited this same effect for both frame tilt conditions. Young adults had smaller RFE values than older adults for the right-tilted frame, with no age-related difference in RFE for the left-tilted frame or asymmetry index. The negative asymmetry index was statistically different from the true vertical only in young adults.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>There is an age-related reduction in the right hemisphere processing of left-sided visual contexts on the RFT. Such findings can assist clinicians to improve interpretation of RFT findings in clinical patients.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39903224","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Time setting errors in the Clock Drawing Test are associated with both semantic and executive deficits. 时钟绘制测试中的时间设定错误与语义和执行障碍有关。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-01-07 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2021.2023154
Matan Soffer, Ashley Melichercik, Nathan Herrmann, Christopher R Bowie, Corinne E Fischer, Alastair J Flint, Sanjeev Kumar, Krista L Lanctôt, Linda Mah, Benoit H Mulsant, Shima Ovaysikia, Bruce G Pollock, Tarek K Rajji, Meryl A Butters

The common requirement to set the time to "10 past 11" on the Clock Drawing Test is intended to elicit a stimulus bound response (SBR), in which the responder is "pulled" to the salient stimulus "10," resulting in hands set at "10 before 11." SBRs are considered markers of executive dysfunction, although this assumption has not yet been validated. We compared SBR and other time-setting errors on inhibitory control tests, hypothesizing that they represent related constructs. The role of semantic dysfunction in the formation of those errors was also investigated. We examined baseline test performance of participants with Mild Cognitive Impairment or a history of depression, and control participants, enrolled in a dementia prevention study. Among 258 participants, we identified clocks with SBRs (n = 16), other time errors (n = 22), or no errors at all (n = 42). Performance between the groups with SBRs and other time-setting errors did not differ on any of the executive tests, and both error groups performed significantly worse than the No Error group on the semantic tests. Control for covariates further supported semantic and executive components in time-setting errors. Both semantic and inhibitory control deficits may underlie time representation errors in general.

在时钟画图测验中,将时间设定为 "11 点过 10 分 "的常见要求旨在诱发刺激约束反应(SBR),即反应者被 "牵引 "到突出刺激 "10 "处,导致双手设定在 "11 点前 10 分"。SBR 被认为是执行功能障碍的标志,尽管这一假设尚未得到验证。我们比较了抑制控制测试中的SBR和其他时间设定错误,假设它们代表了相关的结构。我们还研究了语义功能障碍在这些错误形成中的作用。我们对参加痴呆症预防研究的轻度认知障碍或有抑郁症病史的参与者和对照组参与者的基线测试表现进行了研究。在 258 名参与者中,我们发现了有 SBRs(16 人)、其他时间错误(22 人)或完全没有错误(42 人)的时钟。在任何一项执行测试中,时间设定错误组和其他时间设定错误组的表现均无差异,而在语义测试中,两个错误组的表现均明显差于无错误组。对协变量的控制进一步支持了时间设定错误中的语义和执行成分。语义和抑制控制缺陷可能是时间表征错误的普遍原因。
{"title":"Time setting errors in the Clock Drawing Test are associated with both semantic and executive deficits.","authors":"Matan Soffer, Ashley Melichercik, Nathan Herrmann, Christopher R Bowie, Corinne E Fischer, Alastair J Flint, Sanjeev Kumar, Krista L Lanctôt, Linda Mah, Benoit H Mulsant, Shima Ovaysikia, Bruce G Pollock, Tarek K Rajji, Meryl A Butters","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2021.2023154","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2021.2023154","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The common requirement to set the time to \"10 past 11\" on the Clock Drawing Test is intended to elicit a stimulus bound response (SBR), in which the responder is \"pulled\" to the salient stimulus \"10,\" resulting in hands set at \"10 before 11.\" SBRs are considered markers of executive dysfunction, although this assumption has not yet been validated. We compared SBR and other time-setting errors on inhibitory control tests, hypothesizing that they represent related constructs. The role of semantic dysfunction in the formation of those errors was also investigated. We examined baseline test performance of participants with Mild Cognitive Impairment or a history of depression, and control participants, enrolled in a dementia prevention study. Among 258 participants, we identified clocks with SBRs (<i>n</i> = 16), other time errors (<i>n</i> = 22), or no errors at all (<i>n</i> = 42). Performance between the groups with SBRs and other time-setting errors did not differ on any of the executive tests, and both error groups performed significantly worse than the No Error group on the semantic tests. Control for covariates further supported semantic and executive components in time-setting errors. Both semantic and inhibitory control deficits may underlie time representation errors in general.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39792938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decision rules for an abbreviated administration of the Test of Memory Malingering. 简略记忆错觉测试的决策规则。
IF 1.7 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2022-01-24 DOI: 10.1080/23279095.2022.2026948
Lee Ashendorf, Susanne Withrow, Sarah H Ward, Sara K Sullivan, Michael A Sugarman

The present study investigated abbreviation methods for the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM) in relation to traditional manual-based test cutoffs and independently derived more stringent cutoffs suggested by recent research (≤48 on Trial 2 or 3). Consecutively referred outpatient U.S. military veterans (n = 260) were seen for neuropsychological evaluation for mild traumatic brain injury or possible attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Performance on TOMM Trial 1 was evaluated, including the total score and errors on the first 10 items (TOMMe10), to determine correspondence and redundancy with Trials 2 and 3. Using the traditional cutoff, valid performance on Trials 2 and 3 was predicted by zero errors on TOMMe10 and by Trial 1 scores greater than 41. Invalid performance was predicted by commission of more than three errors on TOMMe10 and by Trial 1 scores less than 34. For revised TOMM cutoffs, a Trial 1 score above 46 was predictive of a valid score, and a TOMMe10 score of three or more errors or a Trial 1 score below 36 was associated with invalid TOMM performance. Conditional abbreviation of the TOMM is feasible in a vast majority of cases without sacrificing information regarding performance validity. Decision trees are provided to facilitate administration of the three trials.

本研究调查了记忆错觉测试(TOMM)的缩写方法与传统的人工测试临界值的关系,并独立得出了近期研究建议的更严格的临界值(试验2或试验3≤48)。连续转诊的门诊美国退伍军人(n = 260)因轻度脑外伤或可能的注意力缺陷/多动障碍接受了神经心理学评估。对 TOMM 测试 1 的表现进行了评估,包括总分和前 10 个项目(TOMMe10)的错误,以确定与测试 2 和测试 3 的对应性和冗余性。采用传统的分界线,TOMMe10 的错误为零和试验 1 的得分大于 41 分,即可预测试验 2 和 3 的有效表现。在 TOMMe10 上出现 3 次以上错误以及试验 1 分数低于 34 分,则可预测试验 2 和 3 的无效表现。对于修订后的 TOMM 临界值,试验 1 分数高于 46 分可预测有效分数,而 TOMMe10 分数为三次或三次以上错误或试验 1 分数低于 36 分则与无效 TOMM 成绩有关。在绝大多数情况下,对TOMM进行有条件缩写是可行的,而不会牺牲有关成绩有效性的信息。我们提供了决策树,以便于对三项试验进行管理。
{"title":"Decision rules for an abbreviated administration of the Test of Memory Malingering.","authors":"Lee Ashendorf, Susanne Withrow, Sarah H Ward, Sara K Sullivan, Michael A Sugarman","doi":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2026948","DOIUrl":"10.1080/23279095.2022.2026948","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study investigated abbreviation methods for the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM) in relation to traditional manual-based test cutoffs and independently derived more stringent cutoffs suggested by recent research (≤48 on Trial 2 or 3). Consecutively referred outpatient U.S. military veterans (<i>n</i> = 260) were seen for neuropsychological evaluation for mild traumatic brain injury or possible attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Performance on TOMM Trial 1 was evaluated, including the total score and errors on the first 10 items (TOMMe10), to determine correspondence and redundancy with Trials 2 and 3. Using the traditional cutoff, valid performance on Trials 2 and 3 was predicted by zero errors on TOMMe10 and by Trial 1 scores greater than 41. Invalid performance was predicted by commission of more than three errors on TOMMe10 and by Trial 1 scores less than 34. For revised TOMM cutoffs, a Trial 1 score above 46 was predictive of a valid score, and a TOMMe10 score of three or more errors or a Trial 1 score below 36 was associated with invalid TOMM performance. Conditional abbreviation of the TOMM is feasible in a vast majority of cases without sacrificing information regarding performance validity. Decision trees are provided to facilitate administration of the three trials.</p>","PeriodicalId":50741,"journal":{"name":"Applied Neuropsychology-Adult","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39728152","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Applied Neuropsychology-Adult
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1