首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Interference between outcomes, spontaneous recovery, and context effects as measured by a cued response reaction time task: evidence for associative retrieval models. 结果、自发恢复和情境效应之间的干扰,由提示反应反应时间任务测量:关联检索模型的证据。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-10-01 Epub Date: 2012-08-20 DOI: 10.1037/a0029517
Estrella González-Martín, Pedro L Cobos, Joaquín Morís, Francisco J López

The most common associative explanation of interference is based on a retrieval failure. Retrieval, in turn, is considered as the result of an associative activation mechanism that is thought to be fast and automatic. However, up-to-date, there is no evidence of interference based on dependent measures specifically related to this kind of low level processes. The objective of the present study was to test whether interference phenomena can be observed by using a cued response task designed to detect low level retrieval processes. Experiment 1 evaluated whether the cued response task served to show a priming effect. Such effect allowed us to interpret the results found in the remaining experiments of the series. Experiment 2 aimed to find the interference effect by using the cued response task. Experiments 3 and 4 were conducted to assess whether spontaneous recovery and context-change effects could also be observed. The results showed that interference and recovery from interference phenomena can be attributable to fast retrieval processes, which is consistent with associative accounts of interference.

对干扰最常见的联想解释是基于检索失败。反过来,检索被认为是联想激活机制的结果,该机制被认为是快速和自动的。然而,到目前为止,还没有证据表明这种低水平过程的干扰是基于与这种低水平过程相关的依赖度量。本研究的目的是测试干扰现象是否可以通过设计一个线索反应任务来检测低水平检索过程。实验1评估提示反应任务是否表现出启动效应。这种效应使我们能够解释在该系列的其余实验中发现的结果。实验2旨在通过提示反应任务来发现干扰效应。实验3和实验4评估了是否也能观察到自发恢复和环境变化效应。结果表明,干扰和干扰现象的恢复可归因于快速的检索过程,这与干扰的联想解释一致。
{"title":"Interference between outcomes, spontaneous recovery, and context effects as measured by a cued response reaction time task: evidence for associative retrieval models.","authors":"Estrella González-Martín,&nbsp;Pedro L Cobos,&nbsp;Joaquín Morís,&nbsp;Francisco J López","doi":"10.1037/a0029517","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029517","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The most common associative explanation of interference is based on a retrieval failure. Retrieval, in turn, is considered as the result of an associative activation mechanism that is thought to be fast and automatic. However, up-to-date, there is no evidence of interference based on dependent measures specifically related to this kind of low level processes. The objective of the present study was to test whether interference phenomena can be observed by using a cued response task designed to detect low level retrieval processes. Experiment 1 evaluated whether the cued response task served to show a priming effect. Such effect allowed us to interpret the results found in the remaining experiments of the series. Experiment 2 aimed to find the interference effect by using the cued response task. Experiments 3 and 4 were conducted to assess whether spontaneous recovery and context-change effects could also be observed. The results showed that interference and recovery from interference phenomena can be attributable to fast retrieval processes, which is consistent with associative accounts of interference.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0029517","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30845362","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
Asymmetrical generalization of conditioning and extinction from compound to element and element to compound. 从化合物到元素和元素到化合物的条件反射和消光的不对称泛化。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-10-01 Epub Date: 2012-08-27 DOI: 10.1037/a0029726
Mark E Bouton, Caleb Doyle-Burr, Drina Vurbic

Four appetitive conditioning experiments studied generalization between compound conditional stimuli (AB) and their elements (e.g., A or B). In Experiments 1 and 2, rats received conditioning with A and AB, and then extinction with either A or AB. During subsequent testing, there was more generalization of extinction (nonresponding) from the compound (AB) to the element (A) than from the element (A) to the compound (AB). This asymmetry was consistent with earlier results involving temporal discrimination learning in which short and long temporal intervals played the roles of A and AB. In Experiment 3, rats received conditioning with either A or AB, and then testing with A and AB. Consistent with elemental models of conditioning, there was more generalization of conditioned responding from A to AB than from AB to A. Experiment 4 found that these asymmetries in the generalization of extinction (Experiments 1 and 2) and conditioning (Experiment 3) both contribute to the feature-positive effect. Overall, the parallel between the current findings and previous results with temporal discrimination learning supports an associative analysis of interval timing. Implications for elemental and configural theories of conditioning and generalization are also discussed.

在实验1和实验2中,大鼠先接受A和AB的条件反射,然后再接受A或AB的条件反射消退。在随后的实验中,化合物(AB)对元素(A)的消退(无反应)普遍化程度高于元素(A)对化合物(AB)的消退(无反应)普遍化程度。这种不对称性与之前关于时间分辨学习的结果一致,即短时间间隔和长时间间隔分别扮演A和AB的角色。在实验3中,大鼠先接受A或AB的条件反射,然后再接受A和AB的测试。从A到AB的条件反应泛化程度高于从AB到A的条件反应泛化程度。实验4发现,消灭(实验1和2)和条件反射(实验3)泛化的不对称性都有助于特征积极效应的产生。总的来说,当前的研究结果与之前的时间辨别学习结果之间的相似性支持了间隔时间的关联分析。对条件作用和泛化的基本理论和构形理论的含义也进行了讨论。
{"title":"Asymmetrical generalization of conditioning and extinction from compound to element and element to compound.","authors":"Mark E Bouton,&nbsp;Caleb Doyle-Burr,&nbsp;Drina Vurbic","doi":"10.1037/a0029726","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029726","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Four appetitive conditioning experiments studied generalization between compound conditional stimuli (AB) and their elements (e.g., A or B). In Experiments 1 and 2, rats received conditioning with A and AB, and then extinction with either A or AB. During subsequent testing, there was more generalization of extinction (nonresponding) from the compound (AB) to the element (A) than from the element (A) to the compound (AB). This asymmetry was consistent with earlier results involving temporal discrimination learning in which short and long temporal intervals played the roles of A and AB. In Experiment 3, rats received conditioning with either A or AB, and then testing with A and AB. Consistent with elemental models of conditioning, there was more generalization of conditioned responding from A to AB than from AB to A. Experiment 4 found that these asymmetries in the generalization of extinction (Experiments 1 and 2) and conditioning (Experiment 3) both contribute to the feature-positive effect. Overall, the parallel between the current findings and previous results with temporal discrimination learning supports an associative analysis of interval timing. Implications for elemental and configural theories of conditioning and generalization are also discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0029726","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30859535","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 23
Pavlovian conditioning with sexually relevant UCS: which is the necessary UCR? 巴甫洛夫条件反射与性相关的UCR:哪个是必要的UCR?
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/a0030231
Xi Chu, Anders Ågmo

In Experiment 1, four groups of male rats were subjected to Pavlovian conditioning of an approach response. In one group the reinforcer was one mount with a sexually receptive female and in another group it was one intromission. A third group was deprived of food for 8 h before every session and a fourth group was deprived for 16 h. These latter groups received a 94 mg food pellet as reinforcement. The conditional stimulus (CS)+ and CS- were different lights. Rats reinforced with mount, intromission, or food after 16 h of deprivation learned to approach the CS+ more than the CS-. There was no difference in performance between the groups making one mount and one intromission. Rats deprived of food for 8 h failed to discriminate between the CS+ and the CS-. The results obtained with food reinforcement show that performance in the conditioning procedure depends on the level of motivation, which should be the case in any valid procedure. In Experiment 2 we determined whether access to female odor, exposure to an inaccessible female, one mount, and one intromission could reinforce classical conditioning. Only mount and intromission were efficient. These data show that the earliest event in the sequence of sexual behaviors that can promote conditioning is mounting. The fact that mount and intromission are equally efficient for reinforcing Pavlovian conditioning shows that the somatic and visceral responses associated with these behavior patterns rather than sensory feedback from the genitals are crucial.

在实验1中,四组雄性大鼠接受了趋近反应的巴甫洛夫条件反射。在一组中,强化物是与一个有性接受能力的雌性交配,在另一组中,强化物是一个插入物。第三组在每次会议前被剥夺食物8小时,第四组被剥夺食物16小时。后一组被剥夺食物颗粒94毫克作为加强。条件刺激(CS)+和CS-是不同的光。被剥夺16小时后,用坐垫、注入或食物强化的大鼠更倾向于接近CS+而不是CS-。在进行一次装载和一次插入的组之间,性能没有差异。被剥夺食物8小时的大鼠无法区分CS+和CS-。通过食物强化获得的结果表明,条件作用过程中的表现取决于动机的水平,这在任何有效的过程中都应该是如此。在实验2中,我们确定了接触雌性气味、接触无法接触的雌性、一次装载和一次输入是否可以强化经典条件反射。只有安装和导入是有效的。这些数据表明,在性行为序列中最早能够促进条件反射的事件正在增加。在强化巴甫洛夫条件反射方面,装载和输入同样有效,这一事实表明,与这些行为模式相关的躯体和内脏反应,而不是来自生殖器的感觉反馈,是至关重要的。
{"title":"Pavlovian conditioning with sexually relevant UCS: which is the necessary UCR?","authors":"Xi Chu,&nbsp;Anders Ågmo","doi":"10.1037/a0030231","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030231","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In Experiment 1, four groups of male rats were subjected to Pavlovian conditioning of an approach response. In one group the reinforcer was one mount with a sexually receptive female and in another group it was one intromission. A third group was deprived of food for 8 h before every session and a fourth group was deprived for 16 h. These latter groups received a 94 mg food pellet as reinforcement. The conditional stimulus (CS)+ and CS- were different lights. Rats reinforced with mount, intromission, or food after 16 h of deprivation learned to approach the CS+ more than the CS-. There was no difference in performance between the groups making one mount and one intromission. Rats deprived of food for 8 h failed to discriminate between the CS+ and the CS-. The results obtained with food reinforcement show that performance in the conditioning procedure depends on the level of motivation, which should be the case in any valid procedure. In Experiment 2 we determined whether access to female odor, exposure to an inaccessible female, one mount, and one intromission could reinforce classical conditioning. Only mount and intromission were efficient. These data show that the earliest event in the sequence of sexual behaviors that can promote conditioning is mounting. The fact that mount and intromission are equally efficient for reinforcing Pavlovian conditioning shows that the somatic and visceral responses associated with these behavior patterns rather than sensory feedback from the genitals are crucial.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0030231","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30977561","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Cognitive mechanisms for transitive inference performance in rhesus monkeys: measuring the influence of associative strength and inferred order. 恒河猴反式推理能力的认知机制:测量联想强度和推理顺序的影响。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/a0030306
Regina Paxton Gazes, Nicholas W Chee, Robert R Hampton

If Ben is taller than Emily and Emily is taller than Dina, one can infer that Ben is taller than Dina. This process of inferring relations between stimuli based on shared relations with other stimuli is called transitive inference (TI). Many species solve TI tasks in which they learn pairs of overlapping stimulus discriminations (A+B-, B+C-, etc.) and are tested with non-adjacent novel test pairings (BD). When relations between stimuli are determined by reinforcement (A is reinforced when paired with B, B when paired with C), performance can be controlled by the associative values of individual stimuli or by logical inference. In Experiment 1 rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) chose the higher ranked item on non-adjacent test trials after training on a 7-image TI task. In Experiment 2 we measured the associative values of 7 TI images and found that these values did not correlate with choice in TI tests. In Experiment 3 large experimental manipulations of the associative value of images did influence performance in some TI test pairings, but performance on other pairs was consistent with the implied order. In Experiment 4 monkeys linked two previously learned 7-item lists into one 14-item list after training with a single linking pair. Linking cannot be explained by associative values. Associative value can control choice in TI tests in at least some extreme circumstances. Implied order better explains most TI choices in monkeys, and is a more viable mechanism for TI of social dominance, which has been observed in birds and fish.

如果本比艾米丽高,而艾米丽比迪娜高,那么我们就可以推断出本比迪娜高。这种根据与其他刺激物的共同关系来推断刺激物之间关系的过程被称为反式推理(TI)。许多物种都能完成反式推理任务,即学习成对的重叠刺激辨别(A+B-、B+C-等),然后用不相邻的新测试配对(BD)进行测试。当刺激之间的关系由强化决定时(A与B配对时得到强化,B与C配对时得到强化),学习成绩可以由单个刺激的联想值或逻辑推理来控制。在实验 1 中,恒河猴(Macaca mulatta)经过 7 图像 TI 任务训练后,在非相邻测试试验中选择了排名较高的项目。在实验 2 中,我们测量了 7 幅 TI 图像的联想值,发现这些联想值与 TI 测试中的选择并不相关。在实验 3 中,对图像联想值的大量实验操作确实影响了某些 TI 测试配对的表现,但其他配对的表现与隐含顺序一致。在实验 4 中,猴子在经过单个链接对的训练后,将两个先前学习过的 7 项列表链接成一个 14 项列表。链接不能用联想值来解释。至少在某些极端情况下,联想值可以控制TI测试中的选择。隐含顺序能更好地解释猴子的大多数TI选择,也是社会支配力TI的一种更可行的机制,这种机制在鸟类和鱼类中也被观察到。
{"title":"Cognitive mechanisms for transitive inference performance in rhesus monkeys: measuring the influence of associative strength and inferred order.","authors":"Regina Paxton Gazes, Nicholas W Chee, Robert R Hampton","doi":"10.1037/a0030306","DOIUrl":"10.1037/a0030306","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>If Ben is taller than Emily and Emily is taller than Dina, one can infer that Ben is taller than Dina. This process of inferring relations between stimuli based on shared relations with other stimuli is called transitive inference (TI). Many species solve TI tasks in which they learn pairs of overlapping stimulus discriminations (A+B-, B+C-, etc.) and are tested with non-adjacent novel test pairings (BD). When relations between stimuli are determined by reinforcement (A is reinforced when paired with B, B when paired with C), performance can be controlled by the associative values of individual stimuli or by logical inference. In Experiment 1 rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) chose the higher ranked item on non-adjacent test trials after training on a 7-image TI task. In Experiment 2 we measured the associative values of 7 TI images and found that these values did not correlate with choice in TI tests. In Experiment 3 large experimental manipulations of the associative value of images did influence performance in some TI test pairings, but performance on other pairs was consistent with the implied order. In Experiment 4 monkeys linked two previously learned 7-item lists into one 14-item list after training with a single linking pair. Linking cannot be explained by associative values. Associative value can control choice in TI tests in at least some extreme circumstances. Implied order better explains most TI choices in monkeys, and is a more viable mechanism for TI of social dominance, which has been observed in birds and fish.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3774320/pdf/nihms497355.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30978607","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pigeons make errors as a result of interval timing in a visual, but not a visual-spatial, midsession reversal task. 鸽子犯错是由于在视觉上的间隔时间,而不是在视觉空间的中间反转任务中。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-10-01 DOI: 10.1037/a0030192
Neil McMillan, William A Roberts

It has been shown previously that pigeons make surprising errors on a visually based midsession reversal task (Cook & Rosen, 2010; Rayburn-Reeves, Molet, & Zentall, 2011). We trained birds with red and green sidekeys, with one color rewarded in the first 40 trials (S1) and the other color rewarded in the latter 40 trials (S2). Importantly, in Phases 1 and 3, red and green were always presented on the same side, whereas in Phase 2 sidekeys were presented on the left and right equally often. In Phases 2 and 3, probe sessions with intertrial intervals (ITIs) longer or shorter than the training intertribal interval (ITI) were interjected among baseline sessions. Results showed that pigeons presented with visual-only cues used interval duration since the beginning of the session to predict when the reversal of reward contingency would occur, but pigeons presented with color and spatial dimensions confounded for predicting reward tended to use a more optimal reward-following strategy of choice based on local reinforcement.

此前已有研究表明,鸽子在基于视觉的中途逆转任务中会犯令人惊讶的错误(Cook & Rosen, 2010;Rayburn-Reeves, Molet, & Zentall, 2011)。我们用红色和绿色的侧翼训练鸟类,在前40次试验中奖励一种颜色(S1),在后40次试验中奖励另一种颜色(S2)。重要的是,在阶段1和阶段3中,红色和绿色总是出现在同一侧,而在阶段2中,侧键出现在左侧和右侧的频率相同。在第2阶段和第3阶段,试验间隔(ITIs)长于或短于训练部落间间隔(ITI)的探测会话被插入基线会话中。结果表明,在视觉提示下的鸽子在预测奖励偶然性时,会使用间隔时间,而在预测奖励时,颜色和空间维度相混淆的鸽子倾向于使用基于局部强化的更优奖励跟随策略。
{"title":"Pigeons make errors as a result of interval timing in a visual, but not a visual-spatial, midsession reversal task.","authors":"Neil McMillan,&nbsp;William A Roberts","doi":"10.1037/a0030192","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030192","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has been shown previously that pigeons make surprising errors on a visually based midsession reversal task (Cook & Rosen, 2010; Rayburn-Reeves, Molet, & Zentall, 2011). We trained birds with red and green sidekeys, with one color rewarded in the first 40 trials (S1) and the other color rewarded in the latter 40 trials (S2). Importantly, in Phases 1 and 3, red and green were always presented on the same side, whereas in Phase 2 sidekeys were presented on the left and right equally often. In Phases 2 and 3, probe sessions with intertrial intervals (ITIs) longer or shorter than the training intertribal interval (ITI) were interjected among baseline sessions. Results showed that pigeons presented with visual-only cues used interval duration since the beginning of the session to predict when the reversal of reward contingency would occur, but pigeons presented with color and spatial dimensions confounded for predicting reward tended to use a more optimal reward-following strategy of choice based on local reinforcement.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0030192","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30977563","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 43
Location and salience of unique features in human perceptual learning. 人类感知学习中独特特征的定位与突出。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-10-01 Epub Date: 2012-09-17 DOI: 10.1037/a0029733
Tony Wang, Yvonna Lavis, Geoffrey Hall, Chris J Mitchell

Preexposure to intermixed presentations of a pair of similar stimuli (AX and BX, where A and B represent distinctive features, and X the features the stimuli hold in common) facilitates subsequent discrimination between them. This perceptual learning effect has been interpreted as indicating that the loss of effective salience resulting from repeated presentation of a stimulus is attenuated or reversed by a salience-modulation process that operates on the unique stimulus features A and B during intermixed preexposure. In 3 experiments, we examined discrimination after intermixed preexposure to AX and BX, making comparison with a condition in which novel unique features were added to the preexposed background (CX and DX). In all experiments, we also monitored eye gaze during both preexposure and the test. Experiments 1 and 2 found discrimination of the preexposed stimuli to be superior. This result cannot be explained by salience-modulation theories that suppose that intermixed preexposure merely attenuates loss of salience to the unique features A and B; it suggests, rather, that intermixed preexposure to AX and BX enhances the salience of, or attention paid to, the distinctive features. Experiment 3 demonstrated that exposure increases sensitivity to the spatial location of the features, a conclusion confirmed by analysis of eye gaze.

预先暴露于一对相似刺激的混合呈现(AX和BX,其中a和B代表不同的特征,X代表刺激的共同特征)有助于随后对它们进行区分。这种知觉学习效应被解释为表明,在混合预暴露期间,在独特的刺激特征a和B上操作的显著性调制过程减弱或逆转了由于重复呈现刺激而导致的有效显著性损失。在3个实验中,我们检测了混合预曝光于AX和BX后的识别,并与在预曝光背景(CX和DX)中添加新的独特特征的情况进行了比较。在所有实验中,我们还在预曝光和测试期间监测眼睛的注视。实验1和2发现对预暴露刺激的辨别能力更强。这一结果不能用显著性调制理论来解释,该理论认为混合预曝光只是衰减了对独特特征A和B的显著性损失;相反,它表明混合预暴露于AX和BX增强了显著性或对独特特征的关注。实验3表明,暴露增加了对特征空间位置的敏感性,这一结论得到了眼睛注视分析的证实。
{"title":"Location and salience of unique features in human perceptual learning.","authors":"Tony Wang,&nbsp;Yvonna Lavis,&nbsp;Geoffrey Hall,&nbsp;Chris J Mitchell","doi":"10.1037/a0029733","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029733","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Preexposure to intermixed presentations of a pair of similar stimuli (AX and BX, where A and B represent distinctive features, and X the features the stimuli hold in common) facilitates subsequent discrimination between them. This perceptual learning effect has been interpreted as indicating that the loss of effective salience resulting from repeated presentation of a stimulus is attenuated or reversed by a salience-modulation process that operates on the unique stimulus features A and B during intermixed preexposure. In 3 experiments, we examined discrimination after intermixed preexposure to AX and BX, making comparison with a condition in which novel unique features were added to the preexposed background (CX and DX). In all experiments, we also monitored eye gaze during both preexposure and the test. Experiments 1 and 2 found discrimination of the preexposed stimuli to be superior. This result cannot be explained by salience-modulation theories that suppose that intermixed preexposure merely attenuates loss of salience to the unique features A and B; it suggests, rather, that intermixed preexposure to AX and BX enhances the salience of, or attention paid to, the distinctive features. Experiment 3 demonstrated that exposure increases sensitivity to the spatial location of the features, a conclusion confirmed by analysis of eye gaze.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0029733","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30912242","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 21
Interval, blocking, and marking effects during the development of schedule-induced drinking in rats. 大鼠时间表性饮酒发展过程中的间隔、阻断和标记效应。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-07-01 Epub Date: 2012-05-21 DOI: 10.1037/a0027788
Angela E Patterson, Robert A Boakes

Schedule-induced drinking (SID) can occur when food-deprived rats are given access to water while receiving pellets on an intermittent reinforcement schedule. These conditions can increase water intake excessively. The possible role of adventitious reinforcement of postpellet drinking was assessed by testing whether response-reinforcer contiguity, the relative predictiveness of a response, and whether it is marked are important in the development of SID. Rats exposed to a short interpellet interval acquired SID most rapidly, with this acquired drinking response maintained when animals were transferred to a longer interpellet interval, thus indicating an easy-to-hard effect (Experiment 1). Further experiments demonstrated that a stimulus (a brief-flashing house light) occurring prior to pellet delivery could block the acquisition of SID (Experiment 2), while a lick-contingent tone, intended to increase the associability of this response, produced more rapid acquisition of SID (Experiment 3). Analysis of lick distributions revealed that licking became concentrated in the first half of an interpellet interval only after several sessions. Overall, the results indicated that similar factors affect the acquisition of both SID and instrumental conditioning with delayed reinforcement, as is consistent with a superstitious conditioning account of SID development.

当被剥夺食物的大鼠在间歇性强化计划中接受颗粒时获得水时,会发生计划性饮水(SID)。这些情况会导致饮水过量。通过测试反应-强化物的连续性、反应的相对可预测性以及是否被标记在SID的发展中是否重要,评估了颗粒后饮用的外源性强化的可能作用。暴露于短间隔颗粒的大鼠获得SID的速度最快,当动物被转移到较长的间隔颗粒时,这种获得性饮水反应保持不变,从而表明易-硬效应(实验1)。进一步的实验表明,在颗粒递送之前发生的刺激(短暂闪烁的室内灯光)可以阻止SID的获得(实验2),而舔音,旨在增加这种反应的关联性。对舔舐分布的分析表明,舔舐只在隔粒间隔的前半段集中。总体而言,研究结果表明,类似的因素会影响SID和工具条件反射的习得,并伴有延迟强化,这与迷信条件反射对SID发展的解释是一致的。
{"title":"Interval, blocking, and marking effects during the development of schedule-induced drinking in rats.","authors":"Angela E Patterson,&nbsp;Robert A Boakes","doi":"10.1037/a0027788","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027788","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Schedule-induced drinking (SID) can occur when food-deprived rats are given access to water while receiving pellets on an intermittent reinforcement schedule. These conditions can increase water intake excessively. The possible role of adventitious reinforcement of postpellet drinking was assessed by testing whether response-reinforcer contiguity, the relative predictiveness of a response, and whether it is marked are important in the development of SID. Rats exposed to a short interpellet interval acquired SID most rapidly, with this acquired drinking response maintained when animals were transferred to a longer interpellet interval, thus indicating an easy-to-hard effect (Experiment 1). Further experiments demonstrated that a stimulus (a brief-flashing house light) occurring prior to pellet delivery could block the acquisition of SID (Experiment 2), while a lick-contingent tone, intended to increase the associability of this response, produced more rapid acquisition of SID (Experiment 3). Analysis of lick distributions revealed that licking became concentrated in the first half of an interpellet interval only after several sessions. Overall, the results indicated that similar factors affect the acquisition of both SID and instrumental conditioning with delayed reinforcement, as is consistent with a superstitious conditioning account of SID development.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0027788","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30633558","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Conditioned [corrected] stimulus informativeness governs conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus associability. 条件刺激信息性支配条件刺激-非条件刺激的联想性。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-07-01 Epub Date: 2012-04-02 DOI: 10.1037/a0027621
Ryan D Ward, C R Gallistel, Greg Jensen, Vanessa L Richards, Stephen Fairhurst, Peter D Balsam

In a conditioning protocol, the onset of the conditioned stimulus ([CS]) provides information about when to expect reinforcement (unconditioned stimulus [US]). There are two sources of information from the CS in a delay conditioning paradigm in which the CS-US interval is fixed. The first depends on the informativeness, the degree to which CS onset reduces the average expected time to onset of the next US. The second depends only on how precisely a subject can represent a fixed-duration interval (the temporal Weber fraction). In three experiments with mice, we tested the differential impact of these two sources of information on rate of acquisition of conditioned responding (CS-US associability). In Experiment 1, we showed that associability (the inverse of trials to acquisition) increased in proportion to informativeness. In Experiment 2, we showed that fixing the duration of the US-US interval or the CS-US interval or both had no effect on associability. In Experiment 3, we equated the increase in information produced by varying the C/T ratio with the increase produced by fixing the duration of the CS-US interval. Associability increased with increased informativeness, but, as in Experiment 2, fixing the CS-US duration had no effect on associability. These results are consistent with the view that CS-US associability depends on the increased rate of reward signaled by CS onset. The results also provide further evidence that conditioned responding is temporally controlled when it emerges.

在条件反射方案中,条件刺激([CS])的开始提供了关于何时期望强化(非条件刺激[US])的信息。在CS- us区间固定的延迟条件反射范式中,CS有两个信息源。第一个取决于信息的广泛性,即CS的发作在多大程度上缩短了下一次美国发作的平均预期时间。第二种方法只取决于被试对固定时间间隔(时间韦伯分数)的描述有多精确。在三个小鼠实验中,我们测试了这两种信息来源对条件反应获得率(CS-US联想性)的不同影响。在实验1中,我们表明联想性(与习得相反的试验)与信息量成比例地增加。在实验2中,我们发现固定US-US间隔或CS-US间隔或两者的持续时间对联想性没有影响。在实验3中,我们将改变C/T比率所产生的信息增加等同于固定CS-US间隔的持续时间所产生的信息增加。联想性随信息量的增加而增加,但与实验2一样,固定CS-US持续时间对联想性没有影响。这些结果与CS- us联想性取决于CS发作时奖赏率的增加的观点是一致的。结果还提供了进一步的证据,证明条件反应在出现时是暂时受控制的。
{"title":"Conditioned [corrected] stimulus informativeness governs conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus associability.","authors":"Ryan D Ward,&nbsp;C R Gallistel,&nbsp;Greg Jensen,&nbsp;Vanessa L Richards,&nbsp;Stephen Fairhurst,&nbsp;Peter D Balsam","doi":"10.1037/a0027621","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027621","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a conditioning protocol, the onset of the conditioned stimulus ([CS]) provides information about when to expect reinforcement (unconditioned stimulus [US]). There are two sources of information from the CS in a delay conditioning paradigm in which the CS-US interval is fixed. The first depends on the informativeness, the degree to which CS onset reduces the average expected time to onset of the next US. The second depends only on how precisely a subject can represent a fixed-duration interval (the temporal Weber fraction). In three experiments with mice, we tested the differential impact of these two sources of information on rate of acquisition of conditioned responding (CS-US associability). In Experiment 1, we showed that associability (the inverse of trials to acquisition) increased in proportion to informativeness. In Experiment 2, we showed that fixing the duration of the US-US interval or the CS-US interval or both had no effect on associability. In Experiment 3, we equated the increase in information produced by varying the C/T ratio with the increase produced by fixing the duration of the CS-US interval. Associability increased with increased informativeness, but, as in Experiment 2, fixing the CS-US duration had no effect on associability. These results are consistent with the view that CS-US associability depends on the increased rate of reward signaled by CS onset. The results also provide further evidence that conditioned responding is temporally controlled when it emerges.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0027621","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30545354","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 36
Generalization of contextual fear as a function of familiarity: the role of within- and between-context associations. 熟悉程度对情境恐惧的泛化:情境内和情境间关联的作用。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-07-01 Epub Date: 2012-05-28 DOI: 10.1037/a0028689
M D Iordanova, R C Honey

Three experiments in rats investigated the generalization of conditioned fear from one context (B) to both a preexposed context (A) and a novel context (C). In each experiment, when the conditioning context (B) had been preexposed, there was greater generalization to context A than to context C; but when B was novel at the outset of conditioning this difference between A and C was not observed. The implications of these results for associative treatments of the development of contextual memories are evaluated.

三个大鼠实验研究了条件反射恐惧从一个情境(B)到预暴露情境(a)和新情境(C)的泛化。在每个实验中,当条件反射情境(B)被预暴露时,情境a的泛化程度高于情境C;但是当B在条件反射开始时是新奇的,A和C之间的差异就没有被观察到。这些结果对情境记忆发展的联想治疗的意义进行了评估。
{"title":"Generalization of contextual fear as a function of familiarity: the role of within- and between-context associations.","authors":"M D Iordanova,&nbsp;R C Honey","doi":"10.1037/a0028689","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028689","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments in rats investigated the generalization of conditioned fear from one context (B) to both a preexposed context (A) and a novel context (C). In each experiment, when the conditioning context (B) had been preexposed, there was greater generalization to context A than to context C; but when B was novel at the outset of conditioning this difference between A and C was not observed. The implications of these results for associative treatments of the development of contextual memories are evaluated.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1037/a0028689","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30652214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
A category-overshadowing effect in pigeons: support for the Common Elements Model of object categorization learning. 鸽子的分类阴影效应:支持物体分类学习的 "共同要素模型"。
IF 1.3 4区 心理学 Pub Date : 2012-07-01 DOI: 10.1037/a0028803
Fabian A Soto, Edward A Wasserman

A model proposing error-driven learning of associations between representations of stimulus properties and responses can account for many findings in the literature on object categorization by nonhuman animals. Furthermore, the model generates predictions that have been confirmed in both pigeons and people, suggesting that these learning processes are widespread across distantly related species. The present work reports evidence of a category-overshadowing effect in pigeons' categorization of natural objects, a novel behavioral phenomenon predicted by the model. Object categorization learning was impaired when a second category of objects provided redundant information about correct responses. The same impairment was not observed when single objects provided redundant information, but the category to which they belonged was uninformative, suggesting that this effect is different from simple overshadowing, arising from competition among stimulus categories rather than individual stimuli during learning.

该模型提出了一种错误驱动的学习模式,即刺激属性表征与反应之间的关联学习模式,可以解释非人类动物物体分类文献中的许多发现。此外,该模型产生的预测结果在鸽子和人身上都得到了证实,表明这些学习过程在远亲物种中广泛存在。本研究报告了鸽子对自然物体进行分类时的分类阴影效应证据,这是该模型所预测的一种新行为现象。当第二类物体提供了关于正确反应的冗余信息时,物体分类学习就会受到影响。当单个物体提供冗余信息,但它们所属的类别没有信息时,没有观察到同样的障碍,这表明这种效应不同于简单的阴影效应,它是由刺激类别之间的竞争而不是单个刺激在学习过程中产生的。
{"title":"A category-overshadowing effect in pigeons: support for the Common Elements Model of object categorization learning.","authors":"Fabian A Soto, Edward A Wasserman","doi":"10.1037/a0028803","DOIUrl":"10.1037/a0028803","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A model proposing error-driven learning of associations between representations of stimulus properties and responses can account for many findings in the literature on object categorization by nonhuman animals. Furthermore, the model generates predictions that have been confirmed in both pigeons and people, suggesting that these learning processes are widespread across distantly related species. The present work reports evidence of a category-overshadowing effect in pigeons' categorization of natural objects, a novel behavioral phenomenon predicted by the model. Object categorization learning was impaired when a second category of objects provided redundant information about correct responses. The same impairment was not observed when single objects provided redundant information, but the category to which they belonged was uninformative, suggesting that this effect is different from simple overshadowing, arising from competition among stimulus categories rather than individual stimuli during learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":51088,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2012-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3513276/pdf/nihms408457.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"30783116","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1