首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Phonetics最新文献

英文 中文
Prosodic marking of information status in Italian 意大利语信息状态的韵律标记
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101212
Simona Sbranna, Caterina Ventura, Aviad Albert, Martine Grice

Previous studies on the prosodic marking of information status argue that Italian tends to resist deaccentuation of given elements. In particular, Italian reportedly always accents post-focal given information within noun phrases (NPs), so that it is not possible to reliably reconstruct the information status of the items from the acoustic signal. However, descriptions have so far been concerned with categorical accent patterns, lacking crucial information about continuous phonetic parameters and their distribution in the utterance in ways that can contribute to prosodic marking. In this paper, we use a novel approach based on periodic-energy-related measures to explore how speakers of the Neapolitan variety of Italian modulate continuous prosodic parameters to differentiate information structure. We show that, contrary to previous findings, Italian speakers of the Neapolitan variety do mark information status prosodically within noun phrases. The discrepancy with previous work is explained by the fact that the prosodic marking of post-focal givenness is not achieved through the categorical presence or absence of a pitch accent on one specific syllable, but through the gradual modulation of phonetic parameters at various locations. Moreover, we find that these modulations occur early in the noun phrase. We also show that native speakers can make use of their knowledge of these modulations to reliably identify post-focal given elements in the absence of the pragmatic context, that is, directly from the acoustic signal.

以往关于信息状态的韵律标记的研究认为,意大利语倾向于抵制给定元素的重读。特别是,据报道,意大利语总是在名词短语(NPs)中重音后焦点给定信息,因此不可能从声学信号中可靠地重建项目的信息状态。然而,到目前为止,描述都是关于绝对的口音模式,缺乏关于连续语音参数及其在话语中的分布的关键信息,这些信息可以有助于韵律标记。在本文中,我们使用一种基于周期性能量相关测量的新方法来探索那不勒斯意大利语的发言者如何调节连续韵律参数来区分信息结构。我们表明,与之前的发现相反,那不勒斯意大利语的使用者在名词短语中确实用韵律标记信息状态。与以往工作的差异可以解释为,后焦点给定的韵律标记不是通过特定音节的音高重音的绝对存在或不存在来实现的,而是通过不同位置的语音参数的逐渐调制来实现的。此外,我们发现这些变化出现在名词短语的早期。我们还表明,母语人士可以利用他们对这些调制的了解,在没有语用上下文的情况下,即直接从声学信号中可靠地识别出后焦点给定元素。
{"title":"Prosodic marking of information status in Italian","authors":"Simona Sbranna,&nbsp;Caterina Ventura,&nbsp;Aviad Albert,&nbsp;Martine Grice","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101212","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101212","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous studies on the prosodic marking of information status argue that Italian tends to resist deaccentuation of given elements. In particular, Italian reportedly always accents post-focal given information within noun phrases (NPs), so that it is not possible to reliably reconstruct the information status of the items from the acoustic signal. However, descriptions have so far been concerned with categorical accent patterns, lacking crucial information about continuous phonetic parameters and their distribution in the utterance in ways that can contribute to prosodic marking. In this paper, we use a novel approach based on periodic-energy-related measures to explore how speakers of the Neapolitan variety of Italian modulate continuous prosodic parameters to differentiate information structure. We show that, contrary to previous findings, Italian speakers of the Neapolitan variety do mark information status prosodically within noun phrases. The discrepancy with previous work is explained by the fact that the prosodic marking of post-focal givenness is not achieved through the categorical presence or absence of a pitch accent on one specific syllable, but through the gradual modulation of phonetic parameters at various locations. Moreover, we find that these modulations occur early in the noun phrase. We also show that native speakers can make use of their knowledge of these modulations to reliably identify post-focal given elements in the absence of the pragmatic context, that is, directly from the acoustic signal.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101212"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49864738","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flexibility and stability of speech sounds: The time course of lexically-driven recalibration 语音的灵活性和稳定性:词汇驱动的重新校准的时间过程
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101222
Yi Zheng , Arthur G. Samuel

Perceptual stability is obviously advantageous, but being able to adjust to the prevailing environment is also adaptive. Previous research has identified ways in which the categorization of speech sounds shifts as a function of recently heard speech. Dozens of studies have examined “lexically driven recalibration”, an adjustment to categorization after listeners hear a number of words with a particular speech sound designed to be perceptually ambiguous. Despite the large number of these studies, little is known about how long the adjustment endures. Using two different stimulus sets, we assess the recovery time after lexically driven recalibration. In addition, we examine whether the size of the recalibration effect diminishes during the identification test used to measure it, and whether the recalibration effect is stronger for one side of a tested contrast or the other. The effect did in fact decline during its measurement, and one side of the contrast (/s/) produced stronger shifts than others (/ʃ/ or /θ/) under the conditions typically examined in recalibration studies. Recalibration was quite robust after 24 hours for both stimulus sets, and still measurable after one week for one of them. This time course is strikingly different than the recovery times reported in previous studies for two other adjustment processes – selective adaptation and audiovisually driven recalibration. The vastly different time courses pose a major challenge for models that ascribe these phenomena to the same adjustment function. Thus, such models will need to be substantially modified, or alternative models will need to be developed.

感知的稳定性显然是有利的,但能够适应主流环境也是适应性的。先前的研究已经确定了语音分类随着最近听到的语音而变化的方式。数十项研究考察了“词汇驱动的重新校准”,即在听众听到一些特定发音的单词后,对分类进行的调整。尽管进行了大量的此类研究,但人们对这种调整能持续多久知之甚少。使用两个不同的刺激集,我们评估词汇驱动的重新校准后的恢复时间。此外,我们还检查了在用于测量的识别测试过程中,重新校准效应的大小是否减小,以及测试对比度的一侧或另一侧的重新校准效应是否更强。事实上,在测量过程中,这种效果确实有所下降,在重新校准研究中通常检查的条件下,对比度的一侧(/s/)比另一侧(/?/或/θ/)产生了更强的偏移。两个刺激组在24小时后重新校准非常稳健,其中一个在一周后仍然可以测量。这一时间过程与之前研究中报道的另外两个调整过程——选择性适应和视听驱动的重新校准——的恢复时间有着惊人的不同。对于将这些现象归因于同一调整函数的模型来说,截然不同的时间进程带来了重大挑战。因此,需要对此类模型进行实质性修改,或者需要开发替代模型。
{"title":"Flexibility and stability of speech sounds: The time course of lexically-driven recalibration","authors":"Yi Zheng ,&nbsp;Arthur G. Samuel","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101222","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101222","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Perceptual stability is obviously advantageous, but being able to adjust to the prevailing environment is also adaptive. Previous research has identified ways in which the categorization of speech sounds shifts as a function of recently heard speech. Dozens of studies have examined “lexically driven recalibration”, an adjustment to categorization after listeners hear a number of words with a particular speech sound designed to be perceptually ambiguous. Despite the large number of these studies, little is known about how long the adjustment endures. Using two different stimulus sets, we assess the recovery time after lexically driven recalibration. In addition, we examine whether the size of the recalibration effect diminishes during the identification test used to measure it, and whether the recalibration effect is stronger for one side of a tested contrast or the other. The effect did in fact decline during its measurement, and one side of the contrast (/s/) produced stronger shifts than others (/ʃ/ or /θ/) under the conditions typically examined in recalibration studies. Recalibration was quite robust after 24 hours for both stimulus sets, and still measurable after one week for one of them. This time course is strikingly different than the recovery times reported in previous studies for two other adjustment processes – selective adaptation and audiovisually driven recalibration. The vastly different time courses pose a major challenge for models that ascribe these phenomena to the same adjustment function. Thus, such models will need to be substantially modified, or alternative models will need to be developed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101222"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816843","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Discriminative segmental cues to vowel height and consonantal place and voicing in whispered speech 轻声语音中元音高度、辅音位置和发音的判别性分段线索
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101223
Luis M.T. Jesus , Sara Castilho , Aníbal Ferreira , Maria Conceição Costa

Purpose

The acoustic signal attributes of whispered speech potentially carry sufficiently distinct information to define vowel spaces and to disambiguate consonant place and voicing, but what these attributes are and the underlying production mechanisms are not fully known. The purpose of this study was to define segmental cues to place and voicing of vowels and sibilant fricatives and to develop an articulatory interpretation of acoustic data.

Method

Seventeen speakers produced sustained sibilants and oral vowels, disyllabic words, sentences and read a phonetically balanced text. All the tasks were repeated in voiced and whispered speech, and the sound source and filter analysed using the following parameters: Fundamental frequency, spectral peak frequencies and levels, spectral slopes, sound pressure level and durations. Logistic linear mixed-effects models were developed to understand what acoustic signal attributes carry sufficiently distinct information to disambiguate /i, a/ and /s, ʃ/.

Results

Vowels were produced with significantly different spectral slope, sound pressure level, first and second formant frequencies in voiced and whispered speech. The low frequencies spectral slope of voiced sibilants was significantly different between whispered and voiced speech. The odds of choosing /a/ instead of /i/ were estimated to be lower for whispered speech when compared to voiced speech. Fricatives’ broad peak frequency was statistically significant when discriminating between /s/ and /ʃ/.

Conclusions

First formant frequency and relative duration of vowels are consistently used as height cues, and spectral slope and broad peak frequency are attributes associated with consonantal place of articulation. The relative duration of same-place voiceless fricatives was higher than voiced fricatives both in voiced and whispered speech. The evidence presented in this paper can be used to restore voiced speech signals, and to inform rehabilitation strategies that can safely explore the production mechanisms of whispering.

目的低声说话的声学信号属性可能携带足够独特的信息来定义元音空间,消除辅音位置和发音的歧义,但这些属性是什么以及潜在的产生机制尚不完全清楚。本研究的目的是定义元音和嘶嘶擦音的位置和发音的分段线索,并开发声学数据的发音解释。方法17名说话人发出持续的嘶嘶声和口头元音、双音节单词、句子,并阅读语音平衡的文本。所有任务都在浊音和耳语中重复,并使用以下参数分析声源和滤波器:基频、频谱峰值频率和电平、频谱斜率、声压电平和持续时间。建立了Logistic线性混合效应模型,以了解哪些声学信号属性携带足够清晰的信息来消除/i、a/和/s的歧义。轻声和浊音的浊音的低频谱斜率有显著差异。据估计,与浊音语音相比,选择/a/而不是/i/的几率更低。在区分/s/和/?/时,擦音的宽峰频率具有统计学意义。结论元音的第一共振峰频率和相对持续时间一直被用作高度线索,谱斜率和宽峰频率是与辅音发音位置相关的属性。在浊音和轻声两种语音中,同一地点的无声擦音的相对时长均高于有声擦音。本文提供的证据可用于恢复有声语音信号,并为安全探索窃窃私语产生机制的康复策略提供信息。
{"title":"Discriminative segmental cues to vowel height and consonantal place and voicing in whispered speech","authors":"Luis M.T. Jesus ,&nbsp;Sara Castilho ,&nbsp;Aníbal Ferreira ,&nbsp;Maria Conceição Costa","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101223","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101223","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>The acoustic signal attributes of whispered speech potentially carry sufficiently distinct information to define vowel spaces and to disambiguate consonant place and voicing, but what these attributes are and the underlying production mechanisms are not fully known. The purpose of this study was to define segmental cues to place and voicing of vowels and sibilant fricatives and to develop an articulatory interpretation of acoustic data.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>Seventeen speakers produced sustained sibilants and oral vowels, disyllabic words, sentences and read a phonetically balanced text. All the tasks were repeated in voiced and whispered speech, and the sound source and filter analysed using the following parameters: Fundamental frequency, spectral peak frequencies and levels, spectral slopes, sound pressure level and durations. Logistic linear mixed-effects models were developed to understand what acoustic signal attributes carry sufficiently distinct information to disambiguate /i, a/ and /s, ʃ/.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Vowels were produced with significantly different spectral slope, sound pressure level, first and second formant frequencies in voiced and whispered speech. The low frequencies spectral slope of voiced sibilants was significantly different between whispered and voiced speech. The odds of choosing /a/ instead of /i/ were estimated to be lower for whispered speech when compared to voiced speech. Fricatives’ broad peak frequency was statistically significant when discriminating between /s/ and /ʃ/.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>First formant frequency and relative duration of vowels are consistently used as height cues, and spectral slope and broad peak frequency are attributes associated with consonantal place of articulation. The relative duration of same-place voiceless fricatives was higher than voiced fricatives both in voiced and whispered speech. The evidence presented in this paper can be used to restore voiced speech signals, and to inform rehabilitation strategies that can safely explore the production mechanisms of whispering.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101223"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816846","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Production and perception of prevelar merger: Two-dimensional comparisons using Pillai scores and confusion matrices 以前合并的产生和感知:使用Pillai分数和混淆矩阵的二维比较
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101213
Valerie Freeman

Vowel merger production is quantified with gradient acoustic measures, while phonemic perception methods are often coarser, complicating comparisons within mergers in progress. This study implements a perception experiment in two-dimensional formant space (F1 × F2), allowing unified plotting, quantification, and statistics with production data. Production and perception are compared within 20 speakers for a two-part prevelar merger in progress in Pacific Northwest English, where mid-front /ɛ, e/ approximate or merge before voiced velar /ɡ/ (leg–vague merger), and low-front prevelar /æɡ/ raises toward them (bag-raising). Distributions are visualized with kernel density plots and overlap quantified with Pillai scores and confusion matrices from linear discriminant analysis models. Results suggest that leg–vague merger is perceived as more complete than it is produced (in both the sample and community), while bag-raising is highly variable in production but rejected in perception. Relationships between production and perception varied by age, with raising and merger progressing across two generations in production but not perception, followed by younger adults perceiving leg–vague merger but not producing it and varying in (minimal) raising perception while varying in bag-raising in production. Thus, prevelar raising/merger may be progressing among some social groups but reversing in others.

元音合并的产生是用梯度声学测量来量化的,而音位感知方法往往更粗糙,使正在进行的合并中的比较复杂化。本研究在二维共振峰空间(F1×F2)中实现了感知实验,允许使用生产数据进行统一的绘图、量化和统计。在太平洋西北英语中,对20个说话者的产生和感知进行了比较,这是一个由两部分组成的prevelar合并过程,其中中前/Ş,e/approximate或merge先于浊音velar/å/(leg–vague合并),低前prevelar/æ/向他们举起(举起袋子)。分布用核密度图可视化,重叠用Pillai分数和线性判别分析模型的混淆矩阵量化。结果表明,腿部模糊合并被认为比生产更完整(在样本和社区中),而养袋在生产中变化很大,但在感知中被拒绝。生产和感知之间的关系因年龄而异,两代人在生产中进行饲养和合并,但没有感知,其次是年轻人感知腿——模糊的合并但没有产生,(最小)饲养感知不同,而在生产中饲养袋的感知不同。因此,在一些社会群体中,婚前养育/合并可能正在取得进展,但在另一些群体中却在逆转。
{"title":"Production and perception of prevelar merger: Two-dimensional comparisons using Pillai scores and confusion matrices","authors":"Valerie Freeman","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101213","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101213","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Vowel merger production is quantified with gradient acoustic measures, while phonemic perception methods are often coarser, complicating comparisons within mergers in progress. This study implements a perception experiment in two-dimensional formant space (F1 × F2), allowing unified plotting, quantification, and statistics with production data. Production and perception are compared within 20 speakers for a two-part prevelar merger in progress in Pacific Northwest English, where mid-front /ɛ, e/ approximate or merge before voiced velar /ɡ/ (<span>leg–vague</span> merger), and low-front prevelar /æɡ/ raises toward them (<span>bag-</span>raising). Distributions are visualized with kernel density plots and overlap quantified with Pillai scores and confusion matrices from linear discriminant analysis models. Results suggest that <span>leg–vague</span> merger is perceived as more complete than it is produced (in both the sample and community), while <span>bag-</span>raising is highly variable in production but rejected in perception. Relationships between production and perception varied by age, with raising and merger progressing across two generations in production but not perception, followed by younger adults perceiving <span>leg–vague</span> merger but not producing it and varying in (minimal) raising perception while varying in <span>bag</span>-raising in production. Thus, prevelar raising/merger may be progressing among some social groups but reversing in others.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101213"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9879351/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10576296","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Speakers coarticulate less in response to both real and imagined communicative challenges: An acoustic analysis of the LUCID corpus 说话者在应对真实和想象的交际挑战时较少联合发音:LUCID语料库的声学分析
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101210
Zhe-chen Guo, Rajka Smiljanic

Overlap of adjacent articulatory gestures leads to coarticulation. Understanding how hyperarticulated intelligibility-enhancing clear speech modifications affect coarticulation can inform theories of phonetic variation and speech intelligibility. However, prior research yielded mixed findings regarding the relationship between hyperarticulation and coarticulatory patterns. This study extends previous work by analyzing the degree of coarticulation across several different communicative conditions in the LUCID corpus (Baker & Hazan, 2010). Southern British English speakers completed an interactive spot-the-difference task with a partner with and without a communicative barrier (e.g., speech degraded by talker babble). They also read sentences without an interlocutor casually and clearly. Diphones in keywords produced in both tasks were analyzed using two whole-spectrum measures, with greater spectral distance and shorter coarticulatory overlap between the diphones indexing less coarticulation. Results revealed that speakers coarticulated less in response to both real (interactive task) and imaginary (sentence-reading) communicative challenges. Speakers furthermore varied the degree of coarticulatory resistance in different real communicative barriers. Diphones with greater consonant articulatory constraint were less sensitive to differences between the conditions, suggesting a limit to the hyperarticulation-induced phonetic variation. The findings agree with the models of targeted speaker adaptations assuming coarticulatory resistance in hyperarticulated clear speech (the H&H theory: Lindblom, 1990).

相邻发音手势的重叠导致共同发音。了解超发音可懂度增强清晰语音修饰如何影响协同发音,可以为语音变异和语音可懂度理论提供信息。然而,先前的研究对关节过度和共关节模式之间的关系得出了喜忧参半的结果。本研究通过分析LUCID语料库中几种不同交际条件下的共同发音程度来扩展先前的工作(Baker和Hazan,2010)。讲英国南部英语的人在有或没有交流障碍的情况下(例如,由于谈话者的胡言乱语而导致的言语退化)与伴侣完成了一项互动点差异任务。他们也在没有对话者的情况下随意而清晰地阅读句子。使用两种全谱测量来分析两项任务中产生的关键词中的双音,双音之间的谱距离较大,共发音重叠较短,共发音较少。结果显示,说话者在应对真实(互动任务)和想象(句子阅读)交际挑战时,共同发音较少。此外,在不同的实际交际障碍中,说话者会改变共同言语阻力的程度。辅音发音限制较大的双元音对条件之间的差异不太敏感,这表明发音过度引起的语音变化是有限的。这些发现与假设超发音清晰语音中的共同发音阻力的有针对性的说话人适应模型一致(H&;H理论:Lindblom,1990)。
{"title":"Speakers coarticulate less in response to both real and imagined communicative challenges: An acoustic analysis of the LUCID corpus","authors":"Zhe-chen Guo,&nbsp;Rajka Smiljanic","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101210","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101210","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Overlap of adjacent articulatory gestures leads to coarticulation. Understanding how hyperarticulated intelligibility-enhancing clear speech modifications affect coarticulation can inform theories of phonetic variation and speech intelligibility. However, prior research yielded mixed findings regarding the relationship between hyperarticulation and coarticulatory patterns. This study extends previous work by analyzing the degree of coarticulation across several different communicative conditions in the LUCID corpus (<span>Baker &amp; Hazan, 2010</span>). Southern British English speakers completed an interactive spot-the-difference task with a partner with and without a communicative barrier (e.g., speech degraded by talker babble). They also read sentences without an interlocutor casually and clearly. Diphones in keywords produced in both tasks were analyzed using two whole-spectrum measures, with greater spectral distance and shorter coarticulatory overlap between the diphones indexing less coarticulation. Results revealed that speakers coarticulated less in response to both real (interactive task) and imaginary (sentence-reading) communicative challenges. Speakers furthermore varied the degree of coarticulatory resistance in different real communicative barriers. Diphones with greater consonant articulatory constraint were less sensitive to differences between the conditions, suggesting a limit to the hyperarticulation-induced phonetic variation. The findings agree with the models of targeted speaker adaptations assuming coarticulatory resistance in hyperarticulated clear speech (the H&amp;H theory: <span>Lindblom, 1990</span>).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101210"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Speaker-specificity in speech production: The contribution of source and filter 语音生成中的说话人特异性:源和滤波器的贡献
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101224
Vincent Hughes , Amanda Cardoso , Paul Foulkes , Peter French , Amelia Gully , Philip Harrison

This study examines the extent to which speaker-specific information is encoded in different features of vocal output and the relationships between those features. A range of acoustic features, grouped as source (laryngeal voice quality measures and fundamental frequency) and filter features (formants and Mel-frequency cepstral coefficients; MFCCs), were extracted from the vocalic portion of the hesitation marker um for 90 male speakers of Standard Southern British English. Little overall correlation between the sets of features was observed, suggesting no strong interdependence between source and filter in our data. Although filter features were consistently better at discriminating between same- and different-speaker pairs compared with source features, combining source and filter has the potential of producing the lowest error rates and the strongest speaker discrimination scores. Taken together, results show that source and filter provide complementary speaker-specific information. However, the extent of the improvements in speaker discrimination performance when combining source and filter varied across speakers. We explore potential explanations for this finding and discuss the implications for source-filter theory, and for applied fields such as speaker recognition and forensic speech science.

这项研究考察了说话者特定信息在多大程度上被编码在声音输出的不同特征中,以及这些特征之间的关系。一系列声学特征,分为源(喉音质量测量和基频)和滤波器特征(共振峰和梅尔频率倒谱系数;MFCC),从90名标准南方英国英语男性说话者的犹豫标记um的发声部分提取。观察到特征集之间的总体相关性很小,这表明我们的数据中的源和过滤器之间没有很强的相互依赖性。尽管与源特征相比,滤波器特征在区分相同和不同的说话者对方面始终更好,但将源和滤波器相结合有可能产生最低的错误率和最强的说话者区分分数。总之,结果表明,源和滤波器提供了互补的说话者特定信息。然而,当组合源和滤波器时,扬声器辨别性能的改善程度因扬声器而异。我们探索了这一发现的潜在解释,并讨论了对源滤波器理论以及说话人识别和取证语音科学等应用领域的启示。
{"title":"Speaker-specificity in speech production: The contribution of source and filter","authors":"Vincent Hughes ,&nbsp;Amanda Cardoso ,&nbsp;Paul Foulkes ,&nbsp;Peter French ,&nbsp;Amelia Gully ,&nbsp;Philip Harrison","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101224","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101224","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study examines the extent to which speaker-specific information is encoded in different features of vocal output and the relationships between those features. A range of acoustic features, grouped as source (laryngeal voice quality measures and fundamental frequency) and filter features (formants and Mel-frequency cepstral coefficients; MFCCs), were extracted from the vocalic portion of the hesitation marker <em>um</em> for 90 male speakers of Standard Southern British English. Little overall correlation between the sets of features was observed, suggesting no strong interdependence between source and filter in our data. Although filter features were consistently better at discriminating between same- and different-speaker pairs compared with source features, combining source and filter has the potential of producing the lowest error rates and the strongest speaker discrimination scores. Taken together, results show that source and filter provide complementary speaker-specific information. However, the extent of the improvements in speaker discrimination performance when combining source and filter varied across speakers. We explore potential explanations for this finding and discuss the implications for source-filter theory, and for applied fields such as speaker recognition and forensic speech science.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101224"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Articulatory and acoustic variation in Polish palatalised retroflexes compared with plain ones 波兰语腭状后屈与普通后屈的发音和声学变化
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101181
Anita Lorenc , Marzena Żygis , Łukasz Mik , Daniel Pape , Márton Sóskuthy

The present paper investigates articulatory and acoustic variation in Polish palatalised retroflex sibilants compared with their plain counterparts. It tests the hypothesis advanced by Hamann (2003: 44) that palatalised retroflexes are non-existent and that retroflexes in Polish change to palato-alveolars [ʃ ʒ t͡ʃ d͡ʒ] when being palatalised. Based on articulatory data from 20 speakers we provide evidence that at least part of the data (53.5%) are palatalised retroflexes [ʂʲ ʐʲ ʈ͡ʂʲ ɖ͡ʐʲ]. The plain counterparts are shown to be retroflex, as proposed by Hamann (2003).

Our averaged results indicate that both palatalised and plain retroflexes show a convex tongue shape. However, individual data reveals a wide range of realisations, from a bunched dorsum to flat and even hollowed tongue shapes. Taking this variability into account, we propose a new tongue shape classification based on Heron’s Formula – i.e. concave, slightly concave, flat, convex and slightly convex. The different tongue shapes are also visualised in the form of videos created using GAMMs.

Regarding acoustic results, our analysis reveals that the strongest correlate of palatalised retroflex sibilants is longer duration of frication in palatalised sibilants followed by higher Centre of Gravity (COG) and m1 spectral slope.

本文研究了波兰腭化曲后鼻音与普通鼻音相比的发音和声学变化。它检验了Hamann(2003:44)提出的假设,即腭化后屈是不存在的,并且波兰语中的后屈在腭化时会变为腭肺泡[ʒʒ。基于20位说话者的发音数据,我们提供了证据,证明至少有一部分数据(53.5%)是腭化后屈[ʂ。正如Hamann(2003)所提出的,平的对应物显示为后屈。我们的平均结果表明,腭化和平的后屈都显示出凸起的舌头形状。然而,个人数据揭示了广泛的现实,从成束的背到扁平甚至中空的舌头形状。考虑到这种可变性,我们基于Heron公式提出了一种新的舌形分类,即凹形、微凹形、平形、凸形和微凸形。不同的舌头形状也以使用GAMM创建的视频的形式呈现出来。关于声学结果,我们的分析表明,腭化后曲鼻音的最强相关性是腭化鼻音的摩擦持续时间更长,其次是更高的重心(COG)和m1谱斜率。
{"title":"Articulatory and acoustic variation in Polish palatalised retroflexes compared with plain ones","authors":"Anita Lorenc ,&nbsp;Marzena Żygis ,&nbsp;Łukasz Mik ,&nbsp;Daniel Pape ,&nbsp;Márton Sóskuthy","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101181","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101181","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present paper investigates articulatory and acoustic variation in Polish palatalised retroflex sibilants compared with their plain counterparts. It tests the hypothesis advanced by Hamann (2003: 44) that palatalised retroflexes are non-existent and that retroflexes in Polish change to palato-alveolars [ʃ ʒ t͡ʃ d͡ʒ] when being palatalised. Based on articulatory data from 20 speakers we provide evidence that at least part of the data (53.5%) are palatalised retroflexes [ʂʲ ʐʲ ʈ͡ʂʲ ɖ͡ʐʲ]. The plain counterparts are shown to be retroflex, as proposed by Hamann (2003).</p><p>Our averaged results indicate that both palatalised and plain retroflexes show a convex tongue shape. However, individual data reveals a wide range of realisations, from a bunched dorsum to flat and even hollowed tongue shapes. Taking this variability into account, we propose a new tongue shape classification based on Heron’s Formula – i.e. concave, slightly concave, flat, convex and slightly convex. The different tongue shapes are also visualised in the form of videos created using GAMMs.</p><p>Regarding acoustic results, our analysis reveals that the strongest correlate of palatalised retroflex sibilants is longer duration of frication in palatalised sibilants followed by higher Centre of Gravity (COG) and m1 spectral slope.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"96 ","pages":"Article 101181"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49754776","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Schwa’s duration and acoustic position in American English 弱读音在美式英语中的音长和位置
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101198
Uriel Cohen Priva, Emily Strand

Is American English schwa’s position determined solely by the context in which it appears? Do vowels neutralize to schwa when their duration is shorter? We address these two inter-related questions using the Buckeye corpus to study vowel behavior across multiple contexts of spontaneous speech. We find that all except tense high vowels shift to lower F1 values when their duration is relatively short, including lax high vowels and lexical schwas, rather than toward a mid-vowel position that schwa occupies when its duration is long. However, we also replicate the finding that schwa is more dependent on both context and duration than other vowels. The results are not consistent with the idea that schwa’s position is determined exclusively by the context in which it appears. However, schwa’s shift to higher F1 values when its duration is longer is not necessarily different from other vowels’ shift to higher F1 values when their duration is longer, making it unnecessary to argue that schwa’s mid-vowel properties are due to having a target in F1 terms.

美国英语schwa的立场完全是由它出现的背景决定的吗?元音持续时间较短时会中和为schwa吗?我们使用七叶树语料库来研究自发语音的多个上下文中的元音行为,以解决这两个相互关联的问题。我们发现,除了时态高元音外,当它们的持续时间相对较短时,所有的高元音都会向较低的F1值移动,包括松散的高元音和词汇schwa,而不是向schwa在其持续时间较长时所占据的中元音位置移动。然而,我们也重复了这一发现,即schwa比其他元音更依赖于上下文和持续时间。结果与施瓦的立场完全由其出现的背景决定的观点不一致。然而,schwa在其持续时间较长时向较高F1值的移位与其他元音在其持续期较长时向更高F1值的转移并不一定不同,因此没有必要争论schwa的中元音特性是由于在F1术语中具有目标。
{"title":"Schwa’s duration and acoustic position in American English","authors":"Uriel Cohen Priva,&nbsp;Emily Strand","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101198","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101198","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Is American English schwa’s position determined solely by the context in which it appears? Do vowels neutralize to schwa when their duration is shorter? We address these two inter-related questions using the Buckeye corpus to study vowel behavior across multiple contexts of spontaneous speech. We find that all except tense high vowels shift to lower F1 values when their duration is relatively short, including lax high vowels and lexical schwas, rather than toward a mid-vowel position that schwa occupies when its duration is long. However, we also replicate the finding that schwa is more dependent on both context and duration than other vowels. The results are not consistent with the idea that schwa’s position is determined exclusively by the context in which it appears. However, schwa’s shift to higher F1 values when its duration is longer is not necessarily different from other vowels’ shift to higher F1 values when their duration is longer, making it unnecessary to argue that schwa’s mid-vowel properties are due to having a target in F1 terms.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"96 ","pages":"Article 101198"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49760260","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Red and blue bananas: Time-series f0 analysis of contrastively focused noun phrases in Papuan Malay and Dutch 红香蕉和蓝香蕉:巴布亚马来语和荷兰语对比集中名词短语的时间序列分析
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101200
Constantijn Kaland , Marc Swerts , Nikolaus P. Himmelmann

The prosody of Papuan Malay, spoken in the easternmost provinces of Indonesia, is not fully described and understood. The limited work available suggests that phrase prosody in this language is different from other well-studied (West-Germanic) languages. However, not much is known about possible correlates of focus marking, for which prosody is used extensively in languages like Dutch and English. To gain insight into universal and specific usages of prosody, this study reports two identical production experiments and acoustic analyses carried out for Papuan Malay and Dutch, to investigate the prosody of noun phrases in different contrastive focus conditions. Participants in the experiments described pictures with different shapes and colors using specific matrix phrases. The prosody of these descriptions was examined by time-series measures of f0 and statistically analysed using generalised additive mixed models (GAMMs). Results show that speakers of Papuan Malay do not use f0 to mark contrastively focused noun phrases, unlike Dutch speakers. The main function of f0 in Papuan Malay phrases appears to be boundary marking on the final syllable in the phrase, a function also observed in Dutch. In addition, the pre-final syllable in the Papuan Malay phrase was always marked with a rising f0, whereas in Dutch an interaction between the boundary and focus marking was found. The results are discussed in a typological perspective and provide new insights into the prosody of Papuan Malay.

印尼最东部省份的巴布亚马来语的韵律没有得到充分的描述和理解。有限的研究表明,这种语言中的短语韵律与其他研究良好的(西日耳曼)语言不同。然而,人们对焦点标记可能的相关性知之甚少,因为韵律在荷兰语和英语等语言中被广泛使用。为了深入了解韵律的普遍用法和具体用法,本研究报告了对巴布亚马来语和荷兰语进行的两次相同的产生实验和声学分析,以研究不同对比焦点条件下名词短语的韵律。实验参与者使用特定的矩阵短语描述不同形状和颜色的图片。通过f0的时间序列测量来检查这些描述的韵律,并使用广义加性混合模型(GAMM)进行统计分析。结果表明,巴布亚马来语使用者与荷兰语使用者不同,不使用f0来标记对比集中的名词短语。f0在巴布亚马来语短语中的主要功能似乎是在短语的最后一个音节上进行边界标记,荷兰语中也有这种功能。此外,巴布亚马来语短语的词尾前音节总是用上升的f0标记,而在荷兰语中,边界和焦点标记之间存在相互作用。这些结果从类型学的角度进行了讨论,并为巴布亚马来语的韵律提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Red and blue bananas: Time-series f0 analysis of contrastively focused noun phrases in Papuan Malay and Dutch","authors":"Constantijn Kaland ,&nbsp;Marc Swerts ,&nbsp;Nikolaus P. Himmelmann","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101200","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101200","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The prosody of Papuan Malay, spoken in the easternmost provinces of Indonesia, is not fully described and understood. The limited work available suggests that phrase prosody in this language is different from other well-studied (West-Germanic) languages. However, not much is known about possible correlates of focus marking, for which prosody is used extensively in languages like Dutch and English. To gain insight into universal and specific usages of prosody, this study reports two identical production experiments and acoustic analyses carried out for Papuan Malay and Dutch, to investigate the prosody of noun phrases in different contrastive focus conditions. Participants in the experiments described pictures with different shapes and colors using specific matrix phrases. The prosody of these descriptions was examined by time-series measures of f0 and statistically analysed using generalised additive mixed models (GAMMs). Results show that speakers of Papuan Malay do not use f0 to mark contrastively focused noun phrases, unlike Dutch speakers. The main function of f0 in Papuan Malay phrases appears to be boundary marking on the final syllable in the phrase, a function also observed in Dutch. In addition, the pre-final syllable in the Papuan Malay phrase was always marked with a rising f0, whereas in Dutch an interaction between the boundary and focus marking was found. The results are discussed in a typological perspective and provide new insights into the prosody of Papuan Malay.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"96 ","pages":"Article 101200"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49754732","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Phonological and phonetic contributions to perception of non-native lexical tones by tone language listeners: Effects of memory load and stimulus variability 语音和语音对声调语言听者感知非母语词汇音调的贡献:记忆负荷和刺激变异性的影响
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101199
Juqiang Chen , Mark Antoniou , Catherine T. Best

The present study examined native language phonological and phonetic factors in non-native lexical tone perception by tone language listeners, manipulating memory load and stimulus variability to bias listeners towards a more phonological or more phonetic mode of perception. Mandarin and Vietnamese listeners categorised the five Thai lexical tones to their native tones, and discriminated five selected Thai tone contrasts that were predicted by the Perceptual Assimilation Model (PAM, Best, 1995) to be discriminated differently. Categorisation responses showed more phonologically-based patterns under high than low memory load but were unaffected by talker and vowel variability, whereas discrimination accuracy was reduced by talker and vowel variability but not by memory load. Phonological factors indicated by type of categorisation and category overlap generally predicted the discrimination of non-native tone contrasts in line with PAM principles. Phonetic factors reflected in category overlap scores and fit index difference scores predicted variations in discriminating contrasts of the same contrast categorisation type. These findings uphold the extension of PAM principles to non-native tone perception by native listeners of other tone languages. Native phonological and phonetic contributions to non-native speech perception differ between categorisation and discrimination tasks, as reflected in differential modulation by memory load and stimulus variability.

本研究考察了声调语言听众在非母语词汇声调感知中的母语语音和语音因素,操纵记忆负荷和刺激变异性,使听众倾向于更语音或更语音的感知模式。普通话和越南语听众将五种泰语词汇音调分类为他们的母语音调,并区分五种选定的泰语音调对比,这些对比是由感知同化模型(PAM,Best,1995)预测的,可以进行不同的区分。在高记忆负荷下,分类反应显示出更多基于语音的模式,但不受说话者和元音变异性的影响,而辨别准确性因说话者和元音的变异性而降低,但不因记忆负荷而降低。根据PAM原理,由分类类型和分类重叠指示的语音因素通常预测非母语音调对比的辨别。类别重叠分数和拟合指数差异分数中反映的语音因素预测了相同对比度分类类型的辨别对比度的变化。这些发现支持PAM原则对其他声调语言的母语听众的非母语声调感知的扩展。分类和辨别任务对非母语语音感知的母语语音和语音贡献不同,这反映在记忆负荷和刺激可变性的差异调制中。
{"title":"Phonological and phonetic contributions to perception of non-native lexical tones by tone language listeners: Effects of memory load and stimulus variability","authors":"Juqiang Chen ,&nbsp;Mark Antoniou ,&nbsp;Catherine T. Best","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101199","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101199","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The present study examined native language phonological and phonetic factors in non-native lexical tone perception by tone language listeners, manipulating memory load and stimulus variability to bias listeners towards a more phonological or more phonetic mode of perception. Mandarin and Vietnamese listeners categorised the five Thai lexical tones to their native tones, and discriminated five selected Thai tone contrasts that were predicted by the Perceptual Assimilation Model (PAM, <span>Best, 1995</span>) to be discriminated differently. Categorisation responses showed more phonologically-based patterns under high than low memory load but were unaffected by talker and vowel variability, whereas discrimination accuracy was reduced by talker and vowel variability but not by memory load. Phonological factors indicated by type of categorisation and category overlap generally predicted the discrimination of non-native tone contrasts in line with PAM principles. Phonetic factors reflected in category overlap scores and fit index difference scores predicted variations in discriminating contrasts of the same contrast categorisation type. These findings uphold the extension of PAM principles to non-native tone perception by native listeners of other tone languages. Native phonological and phonetic contributions to non-native speech perception differ between categorisation and discrimination tasks, as reflected in differential modulation by memory load and stimulus variability.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"96 ","pages":"Article 101199"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49760261","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Journal of Phonetics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1