首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Phonetics最新文献

英文 中文
Phonetic differences between nouns and verbs in their typical syntactic positions in a tonal language: Evidence from disyllabic noun–verb ambiguous words in Standard Mandarin Chinese 调性语言中名词和动词在其典型句法位置上的语音差异:来自标准汉语双音节名动歧义词的证据
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101241
Qibin Ran , Kai Gao , Yuzhu Liang , Quansheng Xia , Søren Wichmann

This study investigates how word categories, namely noun and verb, influence acoustic realizations (duration, F0, intensity) in Standard Mandarin Chinese, a language having phonemically distinctive tones and a simple morphological system. Noun-verb ambiguous words were selected and presented in the final positions of typical syntactic contexts in order to avoid the interference of prosodic boundary, syntactic complexity, contextual predictability, tonal environment, F0 range and syllable properties (consonant, vowel, tone, syllable length). Linear mixed models were fitted to duration, and generative additive mixed models were fitted to F0 and intensity. The results showed that phonetic differences between nouns and verbs were still evident in duration, F0 and intensity after lexical frequency, speech rate and some other related factors were taken into consideration in the models. The second syllables of nouns were longer than those of verbs, and both syllables of nouns were higher in F0 and greater in intensity than those of verbs. Since the prosodic boundary, frequency and other factors were controlled for, the phonetic differences between nouns and verbs might be attributed to their differences in information load and number of syllables. This study provided evidence that phonetic differences between nouns and verbs might be driven by the grammatical classes themselves and is not an epiphenomenon of other processes.

本研究探讨了名词和动词这两个词类如何影响普通话的声学实现(持续时间、F0、强度)。普通话是一种具有独特音调和简单词形系统的语言。为了避免韵律边界、句法复杂性、上下文可预测性、音调环境、F0范围和音节特性(辅音、元音、音调、音节长度)的干扰,选择名词-动词歧义词并将其呈现在典型句法语境的最终位置。线性混合模型与持续时间拟合,生成加性混合模型与F0和强度拟合。结果表明,在模型中考虑了词频、语速等相关因素后,名词和动词在持续时间、F0和强度上的语音差异仍然明显。名词的第二音节比动词长,名词的两个音节的F0都比动词高,强度也比动词大。由于韵律边界、频率等因素受到控制,名词和动词之间的语音差异可能归因于它们在信息量和音节数量方面的差异。这项研究提供了证据,证明名词和动词之间的语音差异可能是由语法类别本身驱动的,而不是其他过程的附带现象。
{"title":"Phonetic differences between nouns and verbs in their typical syntactic positions in a tonal language: Evidence from disyllabic noun–verb ambiguous words in Standard Mandarin Chinese","authors":"Qibin Ran ,&nbsp;Kai Gao ,&nbsp;Yuzhu Liang ,&nbsp;Quansheng Xia ,&nbsp;Søren Wichmann","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101241","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101241","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study investigates how word categories, namely noun and verb, influence acoustic realizations (duration, F0, intensity) in Standard Mandarin Chinese, a language having phonemically distinctive tones and a simple morphological system. Noun-verb ambiguous words were selected and presented in the final positions of typical syntactic contexts in order to avoid the interference of prosodic boundary, syntactic complexity, contextual predictability, tonal environment, F0 range and syllable properties (consonant, vowel, tone, syllable length). Linear mixed models were fitted to duration, and generative additive mixed models were fitted to F0 and intensity. The results showed that phonetic differences between nouns and verbs were still evident in duration, F0 and intensity after lexical frequency, speech rate and some other related factors were taken into consideration in the models. The second syllables of nouns were longer than those of verbs, and both syllables of nouns were higher in F0 and greater in intensity than those of verbs. Since the prosodic boundary, frequency and other factors were controlled for, the phonetic differences between nouns and verbs might be attributed to their differences in information load and number of syllables. This study provided evidence that phonetic differences between nouns and verbs might be driven by the grammatical classes themselves and is not an epiphenomenon of other processes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101241"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49754239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The change in breathy voice after tone split: A production study of Suzhou Wu Chinese 分音后呼吸声的变化:苏州吴语生产研究
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101239
Chunyu Ge , Wenwei Xu , Wentao Gu , Peggy Pik Ki Mok

In some languages, breathy voice plays a pivotal role in tone split. After tone split, breathy voice can undergo further changes. Suzhou Wu Chinese used to have a voicing contrast in initial obstruents, which has transphonologized to a tone contrast and resulted in a two-way tone split, with breathy voice in the low register tones. This study investigates the change in breathy voice after the tone split in Suzhou Wu with apparent-time data from speakers from three age groups. Simultaneous audio and electroglottographic recordings were collected. Principal component analysis and linear discriminant analysis conducted on the acoustic measurements indicate that breathy voice is used less by younger speakers. Generalized Additive Mixed Models were conducted to reveal the changes in breathy voice during the time course of the vowel with regard to different low register tones. It is also found that T2 and T8 are undergoing a decrease in breathy voice with tone changes, but breathy voice is decreasing without tone change in T6. Younger female speakers are ahead of younger male speakers in the decrease in breathy voice. This paper provides a valuable investigation of the change in breathy voice after tone split and contributes to our understanding of the development of phonation types.

在某些语言中,呼吸声在音调分裂中起着关键作用。音调分裂后,呼吸的声音会发生进一步的变化。苏州吴语在声母中往往存在着声母对比,这种对比已经转化为声母对比并导致了双向的声母分裂,在低音域中出现了喘息声。本研究利用三个年龄组说话人的表观时间数据,研究了苏州吴声调分裂后呼吸音的变化。同时采集音频和声门电图记录。对声学测量进行的主成分分析和线性判别分析表明,年轻说话者较少使用呼吸音。采用广义加性混合模型,揭示了在元音的时间过程中,不同的低声域声调对呼吸音的影响。研究还发现,T2和T8的呼吸音随着音调的变化而减少,但T6的呼吸音在没有音调变化的情况下减少。在呼吸音的下降方面,年轻的女性演讲者领先于年轻的男性演讲者。本文对声调分裂后呼吸音的变化进行了有价值的研究,有助于我们理解发声类型的发展。
{"title":"The change in breathy voice after tone split: A production study of Suzhou Wu Chinese","authors":"Chunyu Ge ,&nbsp;Wenwei Xu ,&nbsp;Wentao Gu ,&nbsp;Peggy Pik Ki Mok","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101239","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101239","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In some languages, breathy voice plays a pivotal role in tone split. After tone split, breathy voice can undergo further changes. Suzhou Wu Chinese used to have a voicing contrast in initial obstruents, which has transphonologized to a tone contrast and resulted in a two-way tone split, with breathy voice in the low register tones. This study investigates the change in breathy voice after the tone split in Suzhou Wu with apparent-time data from speakers from three age groups. Simultaneous audio and electroglottographic recordings were collected. Principal component analysis and linear discriminant analysis conducted on the acoustic measurements indicate that breathy voice is used less by younger speakers. Generalized Additive Mixed Models were conducted to reveal the changes in breathy voice during the time course of the vowel with regard to different low register tones. It is also found that T2 and T8 are undergoing a decrease in breathy voice with tone changes, but breathy voice is decreasing without tone change in T6. Younger female speakers are ahead of younger male speakers in the decrease in breathy voice. This paper provides a valuable investigation of the change in breathy voice after tone split and contributes to our understanding of the development of phonation types.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"98 ","pages":"Article 101239"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49754734","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceptions of laboratory animal veterinarians regarding institutional transparency. 实验动物兽医对机构透明度的看法。
IF 1.2 1区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-03-17 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/awf.2023.27
Michael W Brunt, Daniel M Weary

Institutions using animals for research typically have a veterinarian who is responsible for the veterinary care programme and compliance with regulatory obligations. These veterinarians operate at the interface between the institution's animal research programme and senior management. Veterinarians have strong public trust and are well positioned to share information about animals used for scientific purposes, but their perspectives on sharing information with the public are not well documented and their perceptions of transparency may influence how institutional policies are developed and applied. The objective of our study was to analyse the perceptions of institutional transparency among laboratory animal veterinarians working at different universities. Semi-structured, open-ended interviews were used to describe perceptions of 16 attending veterinarians relating to animal research transparency. Three themes were drawn from the interviews: (i) reflections on transparency; (ii) reflections on culture; and (iii) reflections on self. Veterinarians reflected on their personal priorities regarding transparency and when combined with barriers to change within the institutions, sometimes resulted in reported inaction. For example, sometimes veterinarians chose not to pursue available opportunities for change at seemingly willing universities, while others had their initiatives for change blocked by more senior administrators. The sharing of information regarding the animals used for scientific purposes varied in how it was conceptualised by attending veterinarians: (i) true transparency; communication of information for the sake of openness; (ii) strategic transparency; attempt to educate people about animal research because then they will support it; (iii) agenda-driven transparency; selective release of positive stories to direct public opinion; and (iv) fearful non-transparency; not communicating any information for fear of negative opposition to animal research. Transparency was not perceived as an institutional priority by many of the veterinarians and a cohesive action plan to increase transparency that involves multiple universities was identified as a promising avenue to overcome existing barriers.

使用动物进行研究的机构通常会配备一名兽医,负责兽医护理计划和遵守监管义务。这些兽医在机构的动物研究计划与高级管理层之间开展工作。兽医深受公众信任,完全有能力分享科研用动物的相关信息,但他们对与公众分享信息的看法却没有很好的记录,他们对透明度的看法可能会影响机构政策的制定和应用。我们的研究旨在分析在不同大学工作的实验动物兽医对机构透明度的看法。我们采用了半结构化、开放式访谈的方式,描述了 16 位实验动物兽医对动物研究透明度的看法。从访谈中总结出三个主题:(i) 对透明度的反思;(ii) 对文化的反思;(iii) 对自我的反思。兽医们反思了他们在透明度方面的个人优先考虑事项,再加上机构内部的变革障碍,有时会导致报告中的不作为。例如,有时兽医们选择不寻求那些看似愿意变革的大学提供的机会,而另一些兽医则被更高级的管理者阻止了他们的变革倡议。与会兽医对科学研究所用动物相关信息的共享有不同的理解:(i) 真正的透明;为公开而交流信息;(ii) 策略性透明;试图教育人们了解动物研究,因为这样他们就会支持动物研究;(iii) 议程驱动的透明;有选择地发布正面报道,引导公众舆论;(iv) 恐惧的不透明;因害怕动物研究遭到负面反对而不交流任何信息。许多兽医不认为透明度是机构的优先事项,而一项涉及多所大学的提高透明度的统一行动计划被认为是克服现有障碍的可行途径。
{"title":"Perceptions of laboratory animal veterinarians regarding institutional transparency.","authors":"Michael W Brunt, Daniel M Weary","doi":"10.1017/awf.2023.27","DOIUrl":"10.1017/awf.2023.27","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Institutions using animals for research typically have a veterinarian who is responsible for the veterinary care programme and compliance with regulatory obligations. These veterinarians operate at the interface between the institution's animal research programme and senior management. Veterinarians have strong public trust and are well positioned to share information about animals used for scientific purposes, but their perspectives on sharing information with the public are not well documented and their perceptions of transparency may influence how institutional policies are developed and applied. The objective of our study was to analyse the perceptions of institutional transparency among laboratory animal veterinarians working at different universities. Semi-structured, open-ended interviews were used to describe perceptions of 16 attending veterinarians relating to animal research transparency. Three themes were drawn from the interviews: (i) reflections on transparency; (ii) reflections on culture; and (iii) reflections on self. Veterinarians reflected on their personal priorities regarding transparency and when combined with barriers to change within the institutions, sometimes resulted in reported inaction. For example, sometimes veterinarians chose not to pursue available opportunities for change at seemingly willing universities, while others had their initiatives for change blocked by more senior administrators. The sharing of information regarding the animals used for scientific purposes varied in how it was conceptualised by attending veterinarians: (i) true transparency; communication of information for the sake of openness; (ii) strategic transparency; attempt to educate people about animal research because then they will support it; (iii) agenda-driven transparency; selective release of positive stories to direct public opinion; and (iv) fearful non-transparency; not communicating any information for fear of negative opposition to animal research. Transparency was not perceived as an institutional priority by many of the veterinarians and a cohesive action plan to increase transparency that involves multiple universities was identified as a promising avenue to overcome existing barriers.</p>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"10 1","pages":"e32"},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2023-03-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10936364/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77894212","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Same vowels but different contrasts: Mandarin listeners’ perception of English /ei/-/iː/ in unfamiliar phonotactic contexts 元音相同但对比不同:普通话听众在陌生表音语境中对英语的感知
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101221
Yizhou Wang , Rikke L. Bundgaard-Nielsen , Brett J. Baker , Olga Maxwell

The study presented here examines how adult L2 listeners’ L1 phonotactics interferes with L2 vowel perception in different consonantal contexts. We examined Mandarin listeners’ perception of the English /ei/-/iː/ vowel contrast in three onset consonantal contexts, /p f w/, which represent different phonotactic scenarios with respect to the permissibility of Mandarin phonology. L1 Mandarin listeners (N = 42) completed a series of three tasks: a categorisation task, a vowel identification task, and an AXB discrimination task. The results show that English /ei/-/iː/ are perceived as highly contrastive in the /p/ context because both /pei/ and /piː/ constitute a licit sequence in Mandarin phonology. However, participants experience substantial /ei/-/iː/ category confusion in the /f/ and /w/ contexts, where Mandarin listeners repair perceptually by modifying the vowel quality in illicit (unattested) consonant–vowel sequences, i.e., */fiː/ → /fei/ and */wiː/ → /wei/. Further exploratory analyses indicate that L2 listeners’ vowel perception in unfamiliar phonotactic contexts is associated with their target language experience, typically indicated by their L2 vocabulary size. The findings thus suggest that the acquisition of novel phonotactic regularities is tied to increased experience with the L2 lexicon.

本文研究了成年二语听众在不同辅音语境中的一级发音策略对二级元音感知的影响。我们研究了普通话听众在三个起首辅音语境/p-fw/中对英语/ei/-/iõ/元音对比的感知,这三个语境代表了关于普通话音韵学允许性的不同表音策略场景。一级汉语听众(N=42)完成了一系列三项任务:分类任务、元音识别任务和AXB辨别任务。结果表明,在/p/语境中,英语/ei/-/i被认为是高度对比的,因为/pi/和/pi/在普通话音韵学中都构成了一个合法的序列。然而,参与者在/f/和/w/上下文中经历了大量的/ei/-/iõ/类别混淆,普通话听众通过修改非法(未经测试)辅音-元音序列中的元音质量来进行感知修复,即*/fiõ/→/fi/和*/wi→/wei/。进一步的探索性分析表明,二语听众在不熟悉的表音语境中的元音感知与他们的目标语言体验有关,通常由他们的二语词汇大小来指示。因此,研究结果表明,新的发音规律的获得与二语词典经验的增加有关。
{"title":"Same vowels but different contrasts: Mandarin listeners’ perception of English /ei/-/iː/ in unfamiliar phonotactic contexts","authors":"Yizhou Wang ,&nbsp;Rikke L. Bundgaard-Nielsen ,&nbsp;Brett J. Baker ,&nbsp;Olga Maxwell","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101221","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101221","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The study presented here examines how adult L2 listeners’ L1 phonotactics interferes with L2 vowel perception in different consonantal contexts. We examined Mandarin listeners’ perception of the English /ei/-/iː/ vowel contrast in three onset consonantal contexts, /p f w/, which represent different phonotactic scenarios with respect to the permissibility of Mandarin phonology. L1 Mandarin listeners (<em>N</em> = 42) completed a series of three tasks: a categorisation task, a vowel identification task, and an AXB discrimination task. The results show that English /ei/-/iː/ are perceived as highly contrastive in the /p/ context because both /pei/ and /piː/ constitute a licit sequence in Mandarin phonology. However, participants experience substantial /ei/-/iː/ category confusion in the /f/ and /w/ contexts, where Mandarin listeners repair perceptually by modifying the vowel quality in illicit (unattested) consonant–vowel sequences, i.e., */fiː/ → /fei/ and */wiː/ → /wei/. Further exploratory analyses indicate that L2 listeners’ vowel perception in unfamiliar phonotactic contexts is associated with their target language experience, typically indicated by their L2 vocabulary size. The findings thus suggest that the acquisition of novel phonotactic regularities is tied to increased experience with the L2 lexicon.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101221"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816842","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The final lengthening of pre-boundary syllables turns into final shortening as boundary strength levels increase 边界前音节的最后延长随着边界强度的增加而变成最后缩短
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101225
Gerrit Kentner , Isabelle Franz , Christine A. Knoop , Winfried Menninghaus

Phrase-final syllable duration and pauses are generally considered to be positively correlated: The stronger the boundary, the longer the duration of phrase-final syllables, and the more likely or longer a pause. Exploring a large sample of complex literary prose texts read aloud, we examined pause likelihood and duration, pre-boundary syllable duration, and the pitch excursion at prosodic boundaries. Comparing these features across six predicted levels of boundary strength (level 0: no break; 1: simple phrase break; 2: short comma phrase break; 3: long comma phrase break; 4: sentence boundary; 5: direct speech boundary), we find that they are not correlated in a simple monotonic fashion. Whereas pause duration monotonically increases with boundary strength, both pre-boundary syllable duration and the pitch excursion on the pre-boundary syllable are largest for level-2 breaks and decrease significantly through levels 3 to 5. Our analysis suggests that pre-boundary syllable duration is partly contingent on the tonal realization, which is subject to f0 declination as the utterance progresses. We also surmise that pre-boundary syllable duration reflects differences in planning complexity for the different prosodic and syntactic boundaries. Overall, this study shows that a simple monotonic correlation between pause duration and pre-boundary syllable duration is not valid.

短语末音节持续时间和停顿通常被认为是正相关的:边界越强,短语末音节的持续时间越长,停顿的可能性或时间越长。研究了大量朗读的复杂文学散文文本样本,我们检查了停顿的可能性和持续时间、边界前音节持续时间以及韵律边界处的音高偏移。比较六个预测边界强度水平(0级:无中断;1级:简单短语中断;2级:短逗号短语中断;3级:长逗号短语中断,4级:句子边界;5级:直接语音边界)的这些特征,我们发现它们不是以简单单调的方式相关的。尽管停顿持续时间随着边界强度单调增加,但边界前音节持续时间和边界前音节上的音高偏移在2级中断时最大,并且在3至5级时显著减少。我们的分析表明,边界前音节的持续时间在一定程度上取决于音调的实现,而音调的实现随着话语的进行而发生f0的下降。我们还推测,边界前音节持续时间反映了不同韵律和句法边界的规划复杂性的差异。总之,本研究表明,停顿持续时间和边界前音节持续时间之间的简单单调相关性是无效的。
{"title":"The final lengthening of pre-boundary syllables turns into final shortening as boundary strength levels increase","authors":"Gerrit Kentner ,&nbsp;Isabelle Franz ,&nbsp;Christine A. Knoop ,&nbsp;Winfried Menninghaus","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101225","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101225","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Phrase-final syllable duration and pauses are generally considered to be positively correlated: The stronger the boundary, the longer the duration of phrase-final syllables, and the more likely or longer a pause. Exploring a large sample of complex literary prose texts read aloud, we examined pause likelihood and duration, pre-boundary syllable duration, and the pitch excursion at prosodic boundaries. Comparing these features across six predicted levels of boundary strength (level 0: no break; 1: simple phrase break; 2: short comma phrase break; 3: long comma phrase break; 4: sentence boundary; 5: direct speech boundary), we find that they are not correlated in a simple monotonic fashion. Whereas pause duration monotonically increases with boundary strength, both pre-boundary syllable duration and the pitch excursion on the pre-boundary syllable are largest for level-2 breaks and decrease significantly through levels 3 to 5. Our analysis suggests that pre-boundary syllable duration is partly contingent on the tonal realization, which is subject to f0 declination as the utterance progresses. We also surmise that pre-boundary syllable duration reflects differences in planning complexity for the different prosodic and syntactic boundaries. Overall, this study shows that a simple monotonic correlation between pause duration and pre-boundary syllable duration is not valid.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101225"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Prosodic marking of information status in Italian 意大利语信息状态的韵律标记
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101212
Simona Sbranna, Caterina Ventura, Aviad Albert, Martine Grice

Previous studies on the prosodic marking of information status argue that Italian tends to resist deaccentuation of given elements. In particular, Italian reportedly always accents post-focal given information within noun phrases (NPs), so that it is not possible to reliably reconstruct the information status of the items from the acoustic signal. However, descriptions have so far been concerned with categorical accent patterns, lacking crucial information about continuous phonetic parameters and their distribution in the utterance in ways that can contribute to prosodic marking. In this paper, we use a novel approach based on periodic-energy-related measures to explore how speakers of the Neapolitan variety of Italian modulate continuous prosodic parameters to differentiate information structure. We show that, contrary to previous findings, Italian speakers of the Neapolitan variety do mark information status prosodically within noun phrases. The discrepancy with previous work is explained by the fact that the prosodic marking of post-focal givenness is not achieved through the categorical presence or absence of a pitch accent on one specific syllable, but through the gradual modulation of phonetic parameters at various locations. Moreover, we find that these modulations occur early in the noun phrase. We also show that native speakers can make use of their knowledge of these modulations to reliably identify post-focal given elements in the absence of the pragmatic context, that is, directly from the acoustic signal.

以往关于信息状态的韵律标记的研究认为,意大利语倾向于抵制给定元素的重读。特别是,据报道,意大利语总是在名词短语(NPs)中重音后焦点给定信息,因此不可能从声学信号中可靠地重建项目的信息状态。然而,到目前为止,描述都是关于绝对的口音模式,缺乏关于连续语音参数及其在话语中的分布的关键信息,这些信息可以有助于韵律标记。在本文中,我们使用一种基于周期性能量相关测量的新方法来探索那不勒斯意大利语的发言者如何调节连续韵律参数来区分信息结构。我们表明,与之前的发现相反,那不勒斯意大利语的使用者在名词短语中确实用韵律标记信息状态。与以往工作的差异可以解释为,后焦点给定的韵律标记不是通过特定音节的音高重音的绝对存在或不存在来实现的,而是通过不同位置的语音参数的逐渐调制来实现的。此外,我们发现这些变化出现在名词短语的早期。我们还表明,母语人士可以利用他们对这些调制的了解,在没有语用上下文的情况下,即直接从声学信号中可靠地识别出后焦点给定元素。
{"title":"Prosodic marking of information status in Italian","authors":"Simona Sbranna,&nbsp;Caterina Ventura,&nbsp;Aviad Albert,&nbsp;Martine Grice","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101212","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101212","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous studies on the prosodic marking of information status argue that Italian tends to resist deaccentuation of given elements. In particular, Italian reportedly always accents post-focal given information within noun phrases (NPs), so that it is not possible to reliably reconstruct the information status of the items from the acoustic signal. However, descriptions have so far been concerned with categorical accent patterns, lacking crucial information about continuous phonetic parameters and their distribution in the utterance in ways that can contribute to prosodic marking. In this paper, we use a novel approach based on periodic-energy-related measures to explore how speakers of the Neapolitan variety of Italian modulate continuous prosodic parameters to differentiate information structure. We show that, contrary to previous findings, Italian speakers of the Neapolitan variety do mark information status prosodically within noun phrases. The discrepancy with previous work is explained by the fact that the prosodic marking of post-focal givenness is not achieved through the categorical presence or absence of a pitch accent on one specific syllable, but through the gradual modulation of phonetic parameters at various locations. Moreover, we find that these modulations occur early in the noun phrase. We also show that native speakers can make use of their knowledge of these modulations to reliably identify post-focal given elements in the absence of the pragmatic context, that is, directly from the acoustic signal.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101212"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49864738","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flexibility and stability of speech sounds: The time course of lexically-driven recalibration 语音的灵活性和稳定性:词汇驱动的重新校准的时间过程
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101222
Yi Zheng , Arthur G. Samuel

Perceptual stability is obviously advantageous, but being able to adjust to the prevailing environment is also adaptive. Previous research has identified ways in which the categorization of speech sounds shifts as a function of recently heard speech. Dozens of studies have examined “lexically driven recalibration”, an adjustment to categorization after listeners hear a number of words with a particular speech sound designed to be perceptually ambiguous. Despite the large number of these studies, little is known about how long the adjustment endures. Using two different stimulus sets, we assess the recovery time after lexically driven recalibration. In addition, we examine whether the size of the recalibration effect diminishes during the identification test used to measure it, and whether the recalibration effect is stronger for one side of a tested contrast or the other. The effect did in fact decline during its measurement, and one side of the contrast (/s/) produced stronger shifts than others (/ʃ/ or /θ/) under the conditions typically examined in recalibration studies. Recalibration was quite robust after 24 hours for both stimulus sets, and still measurable after one week for one of them. This time course is strikingly different than the recovery times reported in previous studies for two other adjustment processes – selective adaptation and audiovisually driven recalibration. The vastly different time courses pose a major challenge for models that ascribe these phenomena to the same adjustment function. Thus, such models will need to be substantially modified, or alternative models will need to be developed.

感知的稳定性显然是有利的,但能够适应主流环境也是适应性的。先前的研究已经确定了语音分类随着最近听到的语音而变化的方式。数十项研究考察了“词汇驱动的重新校准”,即在听众听到一些特定发音的单词后,对分类进行的调整。尽管进行了大量的此类研究,但人们对这种调整能持续多久知之甚少。使用两个不同的刺激集,我们评估词汇驱动的重新校准后的恢复时间。此外,我们还检查了在用于测量的识别测试过程中,重新校准效应的大小是否减小,以及测试对比度的一侧或另一侧的重新校准效应是否更强。事实上,在测量过程中,这种效果确实有所下降,在重新校准研究中通常检查的条件下,对比度的一侧(/s/)比另一侧(/?/或/θ/)产生了更强的偏移。两个刺激组在24小时后重新校准非常稳健,其中一个在一周后仍然可以测量。这一时间过程与之前研究中报道的另外两个调整过程——选择性适应和视听驱动的重新校准——的恢复时间有着惊人的不同。对于将这些现象归因于同一调整函数的模型来说,截然不同的时间进程带来了重大挑战。因此,需要对此类模型进行实质性修改,或者需要开发替代模型。
{"title":"Flexibility and stability of speech sounds: The time course of lexically-driven recalibration","authors":"Yi Zheng ,&nbsp;Arthur G. Samuel","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101222","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101222","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Perceptual stability is obviously advantageous, but being able to adjust to the prevailing environment is also adaptive. Previous research has identified ways in which the categorization of speech sounds shifts as a function of recently heard speech. Dozens of studies have examined “lexically driven recalibration”, an adjustment to categorization after listeners hear a number of words with a particular speech sound designed to be perceptually ambiguous. Despite the large number of these studies, little is known about how long the adjustment endures. Using two different stimulus sets, we assess the recovery time after lexically driven recalibration. In addition, we examine whether the size of the recalibration effect diminishes during the identification test used to measure it, and whether the recalibration effect is stronger for one side of a tested contrast or the other. The effect did in fact decline during its measurement, and one side of the contrast (/s/) produced stronger shifts than others (/ʃ/ or /θ/) under the conditions typically examined in recalibration studies. Recalibration was quite robust after 24 hours for both stimulus sets, and still measurable after one week for one of them. This time course is strikingly different than the recovery times reported in previous studies for two other adjustment processes – selective adaptation and audiovisually driven recalibration. The vastly different time courses pose a major challenge for models that ascribe these phenomena to the same adjustment function. Thus, such models will need to be substantially modified, or alternative models will need to be developed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101222"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816843","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Discriminative segmental cues to vowel height and consonantal place and voicing in whispered speech 轻声语音中元音高度、辅音位置和发音的判别性分段线索
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101223
Luis M.T. Jesus , Sara Castilho , Aníbal Ferreira , Maria Conceição Costa

Purpose

The acoustic signal attributes of whispered speech potentially carry sufficiently distinct information to define vowel spaces and to disambiguate consonant place and voicing, but what these attributes are and the underlying production mechanisms are not fully known. The purpose of this study was to define segmental cues to place and voicing of vowels and sibilant fricatives and to develop an articulatory interpretation of acoustic data.

Method

Seventeen speakers produced sustained sibilants and oral vowels, disyllabic words, sentences and read a phonetically balanced text. All the tasks were repeated in voiced and whispered speech, and the sound source and filter analysed using the following parameters: Fundamental frequency, spectral peak frequencies and levels, spectral slopes, sound pressure level and durations. Logistic linear mixed-effects models were developed to understand what acoustic signal attributes carry sufficiently distinct information to disambiguate /i, a/ and /s, ʃ/.

Results

Vowels were produced with significantly different spectral slope, sound pressure level, first and second formant frequencies in voiced and whispered speech. The low frequencies spectral slope of voiced sibilants was significantly different between whispered and voiced speech. The odds of choosing /a/ instead of /i/ were estimated to be lower for whispered speech when compared to voiced speech. Fricatives’ broad peak frequency was statistically significant when discriminating between /s/ and /ʃ/.

Conclusions

First formant frequency and relative duration of vowels are consistently used as height cues, and spectral slope and broad peak frequency are attributes associated with consonantal place of articulation. The relative duration of same-place voiceless fricatives was higher than voiced fricatives both in voiced and whispered speech. The evidence presented in this paper can be used to restore voiced speech signals, and to inform rehabilitation strategies that can safely explore the production mechanisms of whispering.

目的低声说话的声学信号属性可能携带足够独特的信息来定义元音空间,消除辅音位置和发音的歧义,但这些属性是什么以及潜在的产生机制尚不完全清楚。本研究的目的是定义元音和嘶嘶擦音的位置和发音的分段线索,并开发声学数据的发音解释。方法17名说话人发出持续的嘶嘶声和口头元音、双音节单词、句子,并阅读语音平衡的文本。所有任务都在浊音和耳语中重复,并使用以下参数分析声源和滤波器:基频、频谱峰值频率和电平、频谱斜率、声压电平和持续时间。建立了Logistic线性混合效应模型,以了解哪些声学信号属性携带足够清晰的信息来消除/i、a/和/s的歧义。轻声和浊音的浊音的低频谱斜率有显著差异。据估计,与浊音语音相比,选择/a/而不是/i/的几率更低。在区分/s/和/?/时,擦音的宽峰频率具有统计学意义。结论元音的第一共振峰频率和相对持续时间一直被用作高度线索,谱斜率和宽峰频率是与辅音发音位置相关的属性。在浊音和轻声两种语音中,同一地点的无声擦音的相对时长均高于有声擦音。本文提供的证据可用于恢复有声语音信号,并为安全探索窃窃私语产生机制的康复策略提供信息。
{"title":"Discriminative segmental cues to vowel height and consonantal place and voicing in whispered speech","authors":"Luis M.T. Jesus ,&nbsp;Sara Castilho ,&nbsp;Aníbal Ferreira ,&nbsp;Maria Conceição Costa","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101223","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101223","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Purpose</h3><p>The acoustic signal attributes of whispered speech potentially carry sufficiently distinct information to define vowel spaces and to disambiguate consonant place and voicing, but what these attributes are and the underlying production mechanisms are not fully known. The purpose of this study was to define segmental cues to place and voicing of vowels and sibilant fricatives and to develop an articulatory interpretation of acoustic data.</p></div><div><h3>Method</h3><p>Seventeen speakers produced sustained sibilants and oral vowels, disyllabic words, sentences and read a phonetically balanced text. All the tasks were repeated in voiced and whispered speech, and the sound source and filter analysed using the following parameters: Fundamental frequency, spectral peak frequencies and levels, spectral slopes, sound pressure level and durations. Logistic linear mixed-effects models were developed to understand what acoustic signal attributes carry sufficiently distinct information to disambiguate /i, a/ and /s, ʃ/.</p></div><div><h3>Results</h3><p>Vowels were produced with significantly different spectral slope, sound pressure level, first and second formant frequencies in voiced and whispered speech. The low frequencies spectral slope of voiced sibilants was significantly different between whispered and voiced speech. The odds of choosing /a/ instead of /i/ were estimated to be lower for whispered speech when compared to voiced speech. Fricatives’ broad peak frequency was statistically significant when discriminating between /s/ and /ʃ/.</p></div><div><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>First formant frequency and relative duration of vowels are consistently used as height cues, and spectral slope and broad peak frequency are attributes associated with consonantal place of articulation. The relative duration of same-place voiceless fricatives was higher than voiced fricatives both in voiced and whispered speech. The evidence presented in this paper can be used to restore voiced speech signals, and to inform rehabilitation strategies that can safely explore the production mechanisms of whispering.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101223"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816846","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Production and perception of prevelar merger: Two-dimensional comparisons using Pillai scores and confusion matrices 以前合并的产生和感知:使用Pillai分数和混淆矩阵的二维比较
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101213
Valerie Freeman

Vowel merger production is quantified with gradient acoustic measures, while phonemic perception methods are often coarser, complicating comparisons within mergers in progress. This study implements a perception experiment in two-dimensional formant space (F1 × F2), allowing unified plotting, quantification, and statistics with production data. Production and perception are compared within 20 speakers for a two-part prevelar merger in progress in Pacific Northwest English, where mid-front /ɛ, e/ approximate or merge before voiced velar /ɡ/ (leg–vague merger), and low-front prevelar /æɡ/ raises toward them (bag-raising). Distributions are visualized with kernel density plots and overlap quantified with Pillai scores and confusion matrices from linear discriminant analysis models. Results suggest that leg–vague merger is perceived as more complete than it is produced (in both the sample and community), while bag-raising is highly variable in production but rejected in perception. Relationships between production and perception varied by age, with raising and merger progressing across two generations in production but not perception, followed by younger adults perceiving leg–vague merger but not producing it and varying in (minimal) raising perception while varying in bag-raising in production. Thus, prevelar raising/merger may be progressing among some social groups but reversing in others.

元音合并的产生是用梯度声学测量来量化的,而音位感知方法往往更粗糙,使正在进行的合并中的比较复杂化。本研究在二维共振峰空间(F1×F2)中实现了感知实验,允许使用生产数据进行统一的绘图、量化和统计。在太平洋西北英语中,对20个说话者的产生和感知进行了比较,这是一个由两部分组成的prevelar合并过程,其中中前/Ş,e/approximate或merge先于浊音velar/å/(leg–vague合并),低前prevelar/æ/向他们举起(举起袋子)。分布用核密度图可视化,重叠用Pillai分数和线性判别分析模型的混淆矩阵量化。结果表明,腿部模糊合并被认为比生产更完整(在样本和社区中),而养袋在生产中变化很大,但在感知中被拒绝。生产和感知之间的关系因年龄而异,两代人在生产中进行饲养和合并,但没有感知,其次是年轻人感知腿——模糊的合并但没有产生,(最小)饲养感知不同,而在生产中饲养袋的感知不同。因此,在一些社会群体中,婚前养育/合并可能正在取得进展,但在另一些群体中却在逆转。
{"title":"Production and perception of prevelar merger: Two-dimensional comparisons using Pillai scores and confusion matrices","authors":"Valerie Freeman","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101213","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.wocn.2023.101213","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Vowel merger production is quantified with gradient acoustic measures, while phonemic perception methods are often coarser, complicating comparisons within mergers in progress. This study implements a perception experiment in two-dimensional formant space (F1 × F2), allowing unified plotting, quantification, and statistics with production data. Production and perception are compared within 20 speakers for a two-part prevelar merger in progress in Pacific Northwest English, where mid-front /ɛ, e/ approximate or merge before voiced velar /ɡ/ (<span>leg–vague</span> merger), and low-front prevelar /æɡ/ raises toward them (<span>bag-</span>raising). Distributions are visualized with kernel density plots and overlap quantified with Pillai scores and confusion matrices from linear discriminant analysis models. Results suggest that <span>leg–vague</span> merger is perceived as more complete than it is produced (in both the sample and community), while <span>bag-</span>raising is highly variable in production but rejected in perception. Relationships between production and perception varied by age, with raising and merger progressing across two generations in production but not perception, followed by younger adults perceiving <span>leg–vague</span> merger but not producing it and varying in (minimal) raising perception while varying in <span>bag</span>-raising in production. Thus, prevelar raising/merger may be progressing among some social groups but reversing in others.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101213"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9879351/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10576296","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Speakers coarticulate less in response to both real and imagined communicative challenges: An acoustic analysis of the LUCID corpus 说话者在应对真实和想象的交际挑战时较少联合发音:LUCID语料库的声学分析
IF 1.9 1区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101210
Zhe-chen Guo, Rajka Smiljanic

Overlap of adjacent articulatory gestures leads to coarticulation. Understanding how hyperarticulated intelligibility-enhancing clear speech modifications affect coarticulation can inform theories of phonetic variation and speech intelligibility. However, prior research yielded mixed findings regarding the relationship between hyperarticulation and coarticulatory patterns. This study extends previous work by analyzing the degree of coarticulation across several different communicative conditions in the LUCID corpus (Baker & Hazan, 2010). Southern British English speakers completed an interactive spot-the-difference task with a partner with and without a communicative barrier (e.g., speech degraded by talker babble). They also read sentences without an interlocutor casually and clearly. Diphones in keywords produced in both tasks were analyzed using two whole-spectrum measures, with greater spectral distance and shorter coarticulatory overlap between the diphones indexing less coarticulation. Results revealed that speakers coarticulated less in response to both real (interactive task) and imaginary (sentence-reading) communicative challenges. Speakers furthermore varied the degree of coarticulatory resistance in different real communicative barriers. Diphones with greater consonant articulatory constraint were less sensitive to differences between the conditions, suggesting a limit to the hyperarticulation-induced phonetic variation. The findings agree with the models of targeted speaker adaptations assuming coarticulatory resistance in hyperarticulated clear speech (the H&H theory: Lindblom, 1990).

相邻发音手势的重叠导致共同发音。了解超发音可懂度增强清晰语音修饰如何影响协同发音,可以为语音变异和语音可懂度理论提供信息。然而,先前的研究对关节过度和共关节模式之间的关系得出了喜忧参半的结果。本研究通过分析LUCID语料库中几种不同交际条件下的共同发音程度来扩展先前的工作(Baker和Hazan,2010)。讲英国南部英语的人在有或没有交流障碍的情况下(例如,由于谈话者的胡言乱语而导致的言语退化)与伴侣完成了一项互动点差异任务。他们也在没有对话者的情况下随意而清晰地阅读句子。使用两种全谱测量来分析两项任务中产生的关键词中的双音,双音之间的谱距离较大,共发音重叠较短,共发音较少。结果显示,说话者在应对真实(互动任务)和想象(句子阅读)交际挑战时,共同发音较少。此外,在不同的实际交际障碍中,说话者会改变共同言语阻力的程度。辅音发音限制较大的双元音对条件之间的差异不太敏感,这表明发音过度引起的语音变化是有限的。这些发现与假设超发音清晰语音中的共同发音阻力的有针对性的说话人适应模型一致(H&;H理论:Lindblom,1990)。
{"title":"Speakers coarticulate less in response to both real and imagined communicative challenges: An acoustic analysis of the LUCID corpus","authors":"Zhe-chen Guo,&nbsp;Rajka Smiljanic","doi":"10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101210","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2022.101210","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Overlap of adjacent articulatory gestures leads to coarticulation. Understanding how hyperarticulated intelligibility-enhancing clear speech modifications affect coarticulation can inform theories of phonetic variation and speech intelligibility. However, prior research yielded mixed findings regarding the relationship between hyperarticulation and coarticulatory patterns. This study extends previous work by analyzing the degree of coarticulation across several different communicative conditions in the LUCID corpus (<span>Baker &amp; Hazan, 2010</span>). Southern British English speakers completed an interactive spot-the-difference task with a partner with and without a communicative barrier (e.g., speech degraded by talker babble). They also read sentences without an interlocutor casually and clearly. Diphones in keywords produced in both tasks were analyzed using two whole-spectrum measures, with greater spectral distance and shorter coarticulatory overlap between the diphones indexing less coarticulation. Results revealed that speakers coarticulated less in response to both real (interactive task) and imaginary (sentence-reading) communicative challenges. Speakers furthermore varied the degree of coarticulatory resistance in different real communicative barriers. Diphones with greater consonant articulatory constraint were less sensitive to differences between the conditions, suggesting a limit to the hyperarticulation-induced phonetic variation. The findings agree with the models of targeted speaker adaptations assuming coarticulatory resistance in hyperarticulated clear speech (the H&amp;H theory: <span>Lindblom, 1990</span>).</p></div>","PeriodicalId":51397,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Phonetics","volume":"97 ","pages":"Article 101210"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49816840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Phonetics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1