首页 > 最新文献

Chemosensory Perception最新文献

英文 中文
Sommelier Students Display Superior Abilities to Identify but Not to Detect or Discriminate Odors Early in their Training 侍酒师的学生在他们的训练中表现出卓越的识别能力,但没有发现或区分气味
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2019-01-15 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-019-09256-w
Daphnée Poupon, Pauline Fernandez, Johannes Frasnelli

Experts acquire superior abilities in their specific domains by training. Sommelier students, who are future olfaction experts, could be an excellent model to study the effects of olfactory training.

We tested whether sommelier students display superior olfactory abilities early in their education: within the first 2?months of education, we examined the olfactory function, i.e., discrimination and identification of odors as well as olfactory threshold and olfactory memory, of n?=?25 sommelier students and compared them to n?=?29 control students. We also tested episodic and working memory.

We found that sommelier students outperformed controls in free and cued identification, but we did not observe any difference in discrimination or threshold tasks. There was also no difference in memory tasks.

Early in their education, sommelier students appear to be better at identifying odors, but do not display other superior olfactory abilities.

Results suggest that sommeliers are better at identifying odors than the average person, either because they enter into training with superior identifications skills or are able to learn to identify odors at a very fast rate.

专家通过训练在其特定领域获得卓越的能力。侍酒师学生是未来的嗅觉专家,他们可以成为研究嗅觉训练效果的一个很好的模型。我们测试了品酒师的学生是否在他们的早期教育中表现出卓越的嗅觉能力:在最初的2?经过几个月的教育,我们检查了n?=?的嗅觉功能,即对气味的辨别和识别以及嗅觉阈值和嗅觉记忆。并将25名侍酒师学生与n?=?对照组29名。我们还测试了情景记忆和工作记忆。我们发现,品酒师学生在自由和线索识别方面的表现优于对照组,但在辨别和阈值任务方面没有观察到任何差异。在记忆任务上也没有差异。在接受教育的早期,品酒师的学生似乎更善于识别气味,但没有表现出其他优越的嗅觉能力。结果表明,侍酒师比普通人更擅长识别气味,要么是因为他们接受过识别技能更高的培训,要么是因为他们能够以非常快的速度学会识别气味。
{"title":"Sommelier Students Display Superior Abilities to Identify but Not to Detect or Discriminate Odors Early in their Training","authors":"Daphnée Poupon,&nbsp;Pauline Fernandez,&nbsp;Johannes Frasnelli","doi":"10.1007/s12078-019-09256-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-019-09256-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Experts acquire superior abilities in their specific domains by training. Sommelier students, who are future olfaction experts, could be an excellent model to study the effects of olfactory training.</p><p>We tested whether sommelier students display superior olfactory abilities early in their education: within the first 2?months of education, we examined the olfactory function, i.e., discrimination and identification of odors as well as olfactory threshold and olfactory memory, of <i>n</i>?=?25 sommelier students and compared them to <i>n</i>?=?29 control students. We also tested episodic and working memory.</p><p>We found that sommelier students outperformed controls in free and cued identification, but we did not observe any difference in discrimination or threshold tasks. There was also no difference in memory tasks.</p><p>Early in their education, sommelier students appear to be better at identifying odors, but do not display other superior olfactory abilities.</p><p>Results suggest that sommeliers are better at identifying odors than the average person, either because they enter into training with superior identifications skills or are able to learn to identify odors at a very fast rate.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2019-01-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-019-09256-w","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4607138","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Association Between Olfactory Performance and Affective Symptoms in Children 儿童嗅觉表现与情感症状的关系
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-12-04 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9253-y
Anne Schienle, Carina Schlintl

Correlations between olfactory performance and particular personality traits (e.g., disgust proneness), as well as symptoms of specific mental disorders (e.g., depression) have been found in numerous studies with adults. The present questionnaire study examined whether similar associations already exist in childhood.

The olfactory discrimination ability of 66 children (32 boys and 34 girls aged between 7 and 11?years) was tested. In addition, the children filled out screening questionnaires to assess the severity of symptoms related to depression, various anxiety disorders (e.g., panic disorder, social anxiety disorder), and disgust proneness. A multiple regression analysis was calculated with olfactory performance as criterion variable and questionnaire scores as predictor variables.

The results showed that depression and disgust proneness were negatively associated with olfactory discrimination ability. Moreover, high levels of social anxiety and low levels of panic symptoms were found to be positive predictors of olfactory performance.

This investigation identified specific associations between olfactory performance and affective symptoms in children.

Future tests with pediatric samples (children with anxiety disorders, depression) are recommended.

嗅觉表现与特定人格特征(如厌恶倾向)以及特定精神障碍症状(如抑郁症)之间的相关性已在许多成人研究中被发现。目前的问卷研究调查了在儿童时期是否已经存在类似的关联。对66名7 ~ 11岁儿童(男32名,女34名)的嗅觉辨别能力进行了测试。此外,孩子们还填写了筛选问卷,以评估与抑郁、各种焦虑障碍(如惊恐障碍、社交焦虑障碍)和厌恶倾向相关的症状的严重程度。以嗅觉表现为判据变量,问卷得分为预测变量,进行多元回归分析。结果表明,抑郁倾向和厌恶倾向与嗅觉辨别能力呈负相关。此外,高水平的社交焦虑和低水平的恐慌症状被发现是嗅觉表现的积极预测因素。这项调查确定了儿童嗅觉表现和情感症状之间的特殊联系。建议将来对儿童样本(患有焦虑症、抑郁症的儿童)进行检测。
{"title":"Association Between Olfactory Performance and Affective Symptoms in Children","authors":"Anne Schienle,&nbsp;Carina Schlintl","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9253-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9253-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Correlations between olfactory performance and particular personality traits (e.g., disgust proneness), as well as symptoms of specific mental disorders (e.g., depression) have been found in numerous studies with adults. The present questionnaire study examined whether similar associations already exist in childhood.</p><p>The olfactory discrimination ability of 66 children (32 boys and 34 girls aged between 7 and 11?years) was tested. In addition, the children filled out screening questionnaires to assess the severity of symptoms related to depression, various anxiety disorders (e.g., panic disorder, social anxiety disorder), and disgust proneness. A multiple regression analysis was calculated with olfactory performance as criterion variable and questionnaire scores as predictor variables.</p><p>The results showed that depression and disgust proneness were negatively associated with olfactory discrimination ability. Moreover, high levels of social anxiety and low levels of panic symptoms were found to be positive predictors of olfactory performance.</p><p>This investigation identified specific associations between olfactory performance and affective symptoms in children.</p><p>Future tests with pediatric samples (children with anxiety disorders, depression) are recommended.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-12-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9253-y","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4495159","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Retronasal Habituation: Characterization and Impact on Flavor Perception Using Time-Intensity 后鼻习惯化:表征和影响风味感知使用时间强度
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-11-29 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9254-x
Robert Pellegrino, Addison Atchley, Simrah Ali, Joel Shingleton, Curtis R. Luckett

Olfactory habituation results from prolonged exposure to an odor, leading to perceptual changes defined by several characteristics. To date, human habituation research has focused on orthonasal olfaction which is perceived externally while ignoring internal routes of odor perception related to flavor. In our study, we conducted two experiments to characterize retronasal olfactory habituation and measured its impact on flavor perception.

In Experiment 1, participants were presented a food odor and non-food odor retronasally, using an orally adhered strip. Each participant rated the odor intensity using a time-intensity procedure. After exposure, the participants ate a lime-flavored gummy and rated the lime flavor. In experiment 2, the same procedure was performed for a low-level lime odor, a simple (lime oil) and complex (lime oil + sucrose + citric acid) beverage as the flavor stimuli.

Our results demonstrated two known principles of habituation for retronasally presented odors: (1) prolonged exposure leads to decreased perception and (2) weaker stimuli lead to more rapid habituation. Additionally, we found that the non-food odor habituated slower than the food odor; however, the participants seemed to recover simultaneously upon food and beverage consumption leading to no change in flavor perception.

The findings of this study give evidence that we habituate to different odors at different rates; more specifically, we provide evidence that differentiates between odor origin and concentration.

This is the first time-intensity characterization of retronasal odor habituation. Additionally, a novel method of administering retronasal odors is presented.

嗅觉习惯化是由于长时间暴露在气味中,导致由几个特征定义的感知变化。迄今为止,人类习惯化研究主要集中在外部感知的正鼻嗅觉上,而忽略了与风味相关的内部气味感知途径。在我们的研究中,我们进行了两个实验来表征后鼻嗅觉习惯,并测量了其对风味感知的影响。在实验1中,参与者使用口腔粘附条,经鼻向其提供食物气味和非食物气味。每个参与者使用时间强度程序评估气味强度。暴露后,参与者吃了一种酸橙味的软糖,并对酸橙味进行了评级。在实验2中,同样的程序被用于低石灰气味,简单(石灰油)和复杂(石灰油+蔗糖+柠檬酸)饮料作为风味刺激。我们的研究结果证明了两个已知的习惯原理:(1)长时间暴露导致感知能力下降;(2)较弱的刺激导致更快的习惯。此外,我们发现非食物气味的习惯速度比食物气味慢;然而,参与者似乎在食物和饮料消耗后同时恢复,导致味道感知没有变化。这项研究的结果证明,我们对不同气味的适应速度不同;更具体地说,我们提供证据区分气味来源和浓度。这是第一个后鼻气味习惯化的时间强度表征。此外,一种新的方法管理鼻后气味提出。
{"title":"Retronasal Habituation: Characterization and Impact on Flavor Perception Using Time-Intensity","authors":"Robert Pellegrino,&nbsp;Addison Atchley,&nbsp;Simrah Ali,&nbsp;Joel Shingleton,&nbsp;Curtis R. Luckett","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9254-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9254-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Olfactory habituation results from prolonged exposure to an odor, leading to perceptual changes defined by several characteristics. To date, human habituation research has focused on orthonasal olfaction which is perceived externally while ignoring internal routes of odor perception related to flavor. In our study, we conducted two experiments to characterize retronasal olfactory habituation and measured its impact on flavor perception.</p><p>In Experiment 1, participants were presented a food odor and non-food odor retronasally, using an orally adhered strip. Each participant rated the odor intensity using a time-intensity procedure. After exposure, the participants ate a lime-flavored gummy and rated the lime flavor. In experiment 2, the same procedure was performed for a low-level lime odor, a simple (lime oil) and complex (lime oil + sucrose + citric acid) beverage as the flavor stimuli.</p><p>Our results demonstrated two known principles of habituation for retronasally presented odors: (1) prolonged exposure leads to decreased perception and (2) weaker stimuli lead to more rapid habituation. Additionally, we found that the non-food odor habituated slower than the food odor; however, the participants seemed to recover simultaneously upon food and beverage consumption leading to no change in flavor perception.</p><p>The findings of this study give evidence that we habituate to different odors at different rates; more specifically, we provide evidence that differentiates between odor origin and concentration.</p><p>This is the first time-intensity characterization of retronasal odor habituation. Additionally, a novel method of administering retronasal odors is presented.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9254-x","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5132452","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Correction to: Temporal Encoding During Unimodal and Bimodal Odor Processing in the Human Brain 修正:人类大脑中单峰和双峰气味处理过程中的时间编码
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-11-07 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9252-z
Anna Oleszkiewicz, Robert Pellegrino, Cagdas Guducu, Linda Farschi, Jonathan Warr, Thomas Hummel
{"title":"Correction to: Temporal Encoding During Unimodal and Bimodal Odor Processing in the Human Brain","authors":"Anna Oleszkiewicz,&nbsp;Robert Pellegrino,&nbsp;Cagdas Guducu,&nbsp;Linda Farschi,&nbsp;Jonathan Warr,&nbsp;Thomas Hummel","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9252-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9252-z","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9252-z","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4320091","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Temporal Encoding During Unimodal and Bimodal Odor Processing in the Human Brain 人脑单峰和双峰气味处理过程中的时间编码
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-09-20 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9251-0
Anna Oleszkiewicz, Robert Pellegrino, Cagdas Guducu, Linda Farschi, Jonathan Warr, Thomas Hummel

In daily life, people encounter a wide range of odors, most of which contain multiple chemical substances. So-called bimodal odors stimulate both the olfactory and trigeminal nerve, and the interaction between these two systems shapes the perception of the odor. However, temporal encoding of these sensory systems during bimodal odor processing has received limited scientific attention.

To investigate this, we recorded the electrophysiological response in 17 participants to relatively unimodal olfactory (strawberry), trigeminal (l-isopulegol) and strongly bimodal (strawberry and l-isopulegol) stimuli.

ERP amplitudes and intensity ratings were significantly bigger for bimodal stimulation, as compared to unimodal stimulations. No significant difference was observed between N1 and P2 response latencies to olfactory and bimodal stimuli while responses to both stimuli showed longer latencies compared to the response of the trigeminal stimulus.

Results provide further evidence of interaction between olfactory and trigeminal systems; additional activation of the olfactory system results in more vigorous electrophysiological responses and the experience of higher intensity. In addition, results indicate that the trigeminal system is faster to react to stimulation.

The current study offers a view on the temporal processing of bimodal odorants that are most likely to be encountered in the environment. We link the more vigorous electrophysiological response to a more complex odorant with activation of different neural structures.

在日常生活中,人们会接触到各种各样的气味,其中大多数气味含有多种化学物质。所谓的双峰气味同时刺激嗅觉神经和三叉神经,这两个系统之间的相互作用形成了对气味的感知。然而,在双峰气味处理过程中,这些感觉系统的时间编码受到了有限的科学关注。为了研究这一点,我们记录了17名参与者对相对单峰嗅觉(草莓)、三叉嗅觉(l-异戊二酚)和强烈双峰嗅觉(草莓和l-异戊二酚)刺激的电生理反应。与单峰刺激相比,双峰刺激的ERP振幅和强度等级明显更大。嗅觉刺激和双峰刺激的N1和P2反应潜伏期无显著差异,但两者的反应潜伏期均比三叉神经刺激的反应潜伏期长。研究结果进一步证明了嗅觉和三叉神经系统之间的相互作用;嗅觉系统的额外激活导致更强烈的电生理反应和更高强度的体验。此外,研究结果表明,三叉神经系统对刺激的反应速度更快。目前的研究提供了一个关于最有可能在环境中遇到的双峰气味的时间处理的观点。我们将更强烈的电生理反应与更复杂的气味与不同神经结构的激活联系起来。
{"title":"Temporal Encoding During Unimodal and Bimodal Odor Processing in the Human Brain","authors":"Anna Oleszkiewicz,&nbsp;Robert Pellegrino,&nbsp;Cagdas Guducu,&nbsp;Linda Farschi,&nbsp;Jonathan Warr,&nbsp;Thomas Hummel","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9251-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9251-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In daily life, people encounter a wide range of odors, most of which contain multiple chemical substances. So-called bimodal odors stimulate both the olfactory and trigeminal nerve, and the interaction between these two systems shapes the perception of the odor. However, temporal encoding of these sensory systems during bimodal odor processing has received limited scientific attention.</p><p>To investigate this, we recorded the electrophysiological response in 17 participants to relatively unimodal olfactory (strawberry), trigeminal (l-isopulegol) and strongly bimodal (strawberry and l-isopulegol) stimuli.</p><p>ERP amplitudes and intensity ratings were significantly bigger for bimodal stimulation, as compared to unimodal stimulations. No significant difference was observed between N1 and P2 response latencies to olfactory and bimodal stimuli while responses to both stimuli showed longer latencies compared to the response of the trigeminal stimulus.</p><p>Results provide further evidence of interaction between olfactory and trigeminal systems; additional activation of the olfactory system results in more vigorous electrophysiological responses and the experience of higher intensity. In addition, results indicate that the trigeminal system is faster to react to stimulation.</p><p>The current study offers a view on the temporal processing of bimodal odorants that are most likely to be encountered in the environment. We link the more vigorous electrophysiological response to a more complex odorant with activation of different neural structures.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9251-0","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4805907","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Characterization and Evaluation of Changes in the Aroma-Active Components in Szechuan Pepper (Zanthoxylum bungeanum Maxim) Under Different Cooking Temperatures Using Gas Chromatography-Olfactometry 不同蒸煮温度下花椒香气活性成分变化的气相色谱-嗅觉表征与评价
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-09-19 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9249-7
Zhihua Li, Jin Jeon, So Young Kwon, Rui Huang, Hyung-Hee Baek

The flavor of Szechuan pepper plays an important role in Sichuan cuisine, where it is responsible for the tingling and numbing characteristic. The aims of this study were to characterize and identify aroma-active compounds of Szechuan pepper and to evaluate changes in flavor components between room and high temperature (ca. 70–80?°C).

Solvent-assisted flavor evaporation (SAFE) and headspace solid-phase microextraction (HS-SPME) followed by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) and gas chromatography-olfactometry (GC-O) were used.

In this study, linalool, linalyl acetate, and limonene were identified as the major components by both SAFE and HS-SPME. Seven compounds, namely myrcene, limonene, 1,8-cineole, linalool, 2-phenylethanol, 4-methylacetophenone, and trans-carveol, were identified as aroma-active compounds using GC-O. The most intense aroma-active compounds were 1,8-cineole and linalool. Moreover, 2-phenylethanol and 4-methylacetophenone were identified as aroma-active components for the first time in Szechuan pepper. The relative contents of 1,8-cineole, linalool, 2-phenylethanol, 4-methylacetophenone, and trans-carveol were significantly increased as simulated cooking temperature increased from 40 to 75?°C, whereas myrcene, limonene, and hexanal were significantly decreased; 1,8-cineole was not detected at 40?°C but was detected at 75?°C.

A total of 83 volatile compounds were identified, including 62 by SAFE and 57 by HS-SPME. Of these, 34 volatile compounds were identified by both SAFE and HS-SPME. For the first time, 2-phenylethanol and 4-methylacetophenone were identified as key aroma-active components in Szechuan pepper. The overall flavor of Szechuan pepper might be influenced by a result of the relative ratios of key aroma-active components. Cooking temperature should be taken into consideration when Szechuan pepper is used as an ingredient in food.

These results could be beneficial for furthering our understanding of aroma-active compounds in Szechuan pepper and improving dishes prepared with this ingredient.

花椒的味道在川菜中扮演着重要的角色,它是川菜中麻麻的特点。本研究的目的是表征和鉴定花椒的香气活性化合物,并评价室温和高温(约70-80°C)下风味成分的变化。采用溶剂辅助香料蒸发法(SAFE)、顶空固相微萃取法(HS-SPME)、气相色谱-质谱法(GC-MS)和气相色谱-嗅觉法(GC-O)。在本研究中,通过SAFE和HS-SPME鉴定了芳樟醇、醋酸芳樟醇和柠檬烯为主要成分。采用气相色谱- o法鉴定了7种具有芳香活性的化合物,即月桂烯、柠檬烯、1,8-桉树脑、芳樟醇、2-苯乙醇、4-甲基苯乙酮和反式卡维罗。香气活性最强的化合物为1,8-桉树脑和芳樟醇。此外,2-苯乙醇和4-甲基苯乙酮首次在花椒中被鉴定为香气活性成分。1,8-桉树脑、芳樟醇、2-苯乙醇、4-甲基苯乙酮和反式卡维罗的相对含量随着模拟烹饪温度从40℃升高到75℃而显著增加。°C时,月桂烯、柠檬烯和己醛含量显著降低;1,8-桉树脑在40?但在75°C时检测到。共鉴定出83种挥发性化合物,其中SAFE法62种,HS-SPME法57种。其中,34种挥发性化合物均通过SAFE和HS-SPME鉴定。首次鉴定出2-苯乙醇和4-甲基苯乙酮是花椒中的关键香气活性成分。关键香气活性成分的相对比例可能会影响花椒的整体风味。在使用花椒作为食材时,应考虑烹调温度。这些结果将有助于进一步了解花椒的香气活性成分,并改进用花椒制作的菜肴。
{"title":"Characterization and Evaluation of Changes in the Aroma-Active Components in Szechuan Pepper (Zanthoxylum bungeanum Maxim) Under Different Cooking Temperatures Using Gas Chromatography-Olfactometry","authors":"Zhihua Li,&nbsp;Jin Jeon,&nbsp;So Young Kwon,&nbsp;Rui Huang,&nbsp;Hyung-Hee Baek","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9249-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9249-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The flavor of Szechuan pepper plays an important role in Sichuan cuisine, where it is responsible for the tingling and numbing characteristic. The aims of this study were to characterize and identify aroma-active compounds of Szechuan pepper and to evaluate changes in flavor components between room and high temperature (ca. 70–80?°C).</p><p>Solvent-assisted flavor evaporation (SAFE) and headspace solid-phase microextraction (HS-SPME) followed by gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) and gas chromatography-olfactometry (GC-O) were used.</p><p>In this study, linalool, linalyl acetate, and limonene were identified as the major components by both SAFE and HS-SPME. Seven compounds, namely myrcene, limonene, 1,8-cineole, linalool, 2-phenylethanol, 4-methylacetophenone, and <i>trans</i>-carveol, were identified as aroma-active compounds using GC-O. The most intense aroma-active compounds were 1,8-cineole and linalool. Moreover, 2-phenylethanol and 4-methylacetophenone were identified as aroma-active components for the first time in Szechuan pepper. The relative contents of 1,8-cineole, linalool, 2-phenylethanol, 4-methylacetophenone, and <i>trans</i>-carveol were significantly increased as simulated cooking temperature increased from 40 to 75?°C, whereas myrcene, limonene, and hexanal were significantly decreased; 1,8-cineole was not detected at 40?°C but was detected at 75?°C.</p><p>A total of 83 volatile compounds were identified, including 62 by SAFE and 57 by HS-SPME. Of these, 34 volatile compounds were identified by both SAFE and HS-SPME. For the first time, 2-phenylethanol and 4-methylacetophenone were identified as key aroma-active components in Szechuan pepper. The overall flavor of Szechuan pepper might be influenced by a result of the relative ratios of key aroma-active components. Cooking temperature should be taken into consideration when Szechuan pepper is used as an ingredient in food.</p><p>These results could be beneficial for furthering our understanding of aroma-active compounds in Szechuan pepper and improving dishes prepared with this ingredient.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9249-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4779009","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 17
Sex Differences in the Olfactory System: a Functional MRI Study 嗅觉系统的性别差异:功能性MRI研究
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-09-17 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9250-1
Helena Melero, Susana Borromeo, Alexandra Cristobal-Huerta, Eva Manzanedo, Guillermo Luna, Adolfo Toledano, Juan Antonio Hernández-Tamames

Olfactory dysfunction is an early marker of neurological disease and a common symptom in psychotic disorders. Previous anatomical and functional research suggests that sex effects may be crucial in the assessment of the olfactory system. Nonetheless, the neural mechanisms through which the factor sex impacts olfactory perception are still not well understood. In this context, we use fMRI to investigate sex differences in the passive processing of chemical stimuli, in order to obtain new neuroscientific data that may help improve the assessment of odor perception.

Thirty healthy subjects (17 women) were stimulated with mint and butanol (event-related design) in a 3.0-T MRI scanner. A one-sample t test analysis was performed in order to observe olfactory-related activations. Intergroup differences (women vs. men) and the influence of each aroma were analyzed using a 2?×?2 ANOVA and post hoc contrasts.

Men and women showed differential activity (males?>?females) in right superior/middle temporal areas, the right inferior frontal cortex, and the hypothalamus. Both groups showed a predominance of the right hemisphere for the processing of odors.

Functional differences between women and men in olfaction are not restricted to specific sensory areas and reflect a more general sex-dependent effect in multisensory integration processes.

Considering sex differences is essential in order to develop more specific and efficient strategies for the assessment and rehabilitation of the olfactory system and for the interpretation of the olfactory loss as an early biomarker of neurological and psychiatric diseases.

嗅觉功能障碍是神经系统疾病的早期标志,也是精神障碍的常见症状。先前的解剖学和功能研究表明,性别影响在评估嗅觉系统中可能是至关重要的。尽管如此,性别因素影响嗅觉感知的神经机制仍未得到很好的理解。在这种情况下,我们使用功能磁共振成像来研究被动处理化学刺激的性别差异,以获得新的神经科学数据,可能有助于改善气味感知的评估。30名健康受试者(17名女性)在3.0 t MRI扫描仪上接受薄荷和丁醇刺激(事件相关设计)。为了观察嗅觉相关的激活,进行了单样本t检验分析。组间差异(女性vs.男性)和每种香气的影响使用2 × ×?方差分析和事后对比。男性和女性在右颞上/中颞区、右额叶下皮层和下丘脑表现出不同的活动(男性和女性)。两组都显示出处理气味的右半球占优势。女性和男性嗅觉功能的差异并不局限于特定的感觉区域,而是反映了多感觉整合过程中更普遍的性别依赖效应。考虑性别差异是必要的,以便制定更具体和有效的策略来评估和恢复嗅觉系统,并解释嗅觉丧失作为神经和精神疾病的早期生物标志物。
{"title":"Sex Differences in the Olfactory System: a Functional MRI Study","authors":"Helena Melero,&nbsp;Susana Borromeo,&nbsp;Alexandra Cristobal-Huerta,&nbsp;Eva Manzanedo,&nbsp;Guillermo Luna,&nbsp;Adolfo Toledano,&nbsp;Juan Antonio Hernández-Tamames","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9250-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9250-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Olfactory dysfunction is an early marker of neurological disease and a common symptom in psychotic disorders. Previous anatomical and functional research suggests that sex effects may be crucial in the assessment of the olfactory system. Nonetheless, the neural mechanisms through which the factor sex impacts olfactory perception are still not well understood. In this context, we use fMRI to investigate sex differences in the passive processing of chemical stimuli, in order to obtain new neuroscientific data that may help improve the assessment of odor perception.</p><p>Thirty healthy subjects (17 women) were stimulated with mint and butanol (event-related design) in a 3.0-T MRI scanner. A one-sample <i>t</i> test analysis was performed in order to observe olfactory-related activations. Intergroup differences (women vs. men) and the influence of each aroma were analyzed using a 2?×?2 ANOVA and post hoc contrasts.</p><p>Men and women showed differential activity (males?&gt;?females) in right superior/middle temporal areas, the right inferior frontal cortex, and the hypothalamus. Both groups showed a predominance of the right hemisphere for the processing of odors.</p><p>Functional differences between women and men in olfaction are not restricted to specific sensory areas and reflect a more general sex-dependent effect in multisensory integration processes.</p><p>Considering sex differences is essential in order to develop more specific and efficient strategies for the assessment and rehabilitation of the olfactory system and for the interpretation of the olfactory loss as an early biomarker of neurological and psychiatric diseases.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9250-1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4706731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Olfactory Awareness and the Self-Reported Importance of Olfactory Information in Romantic Interest 嗅觉意识和嗅觉信息在恋爱兴趣中的自我报告重要性
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-08-14 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9248-8
Michelle VanHatten, Caitlin Cunningham, Theresa L. White

Many people seem to be looking for similar attributes when searching for a potential romantic partner. Olfactory social cues can be important parts of the process, though there are individual differences as to their value. Gay men, for example, value scent less in selecting a romantic partner than do heterosexual men (White and Cunningham, Chemosens Percept 10:31–41, 2017). Is it possible that the relative importance of olfaction in mate selection is simply a natural consequence of being generally aware of odorants?

The present study examined the relationship between odor awareness and odor importance in mating in two studies. Participants in each of the studies completed both the Romantic Interests Survey (Herz and Inzlich, Evol Hum Behav 23:359–364, 2002) and the Odor Awareness Survey (Smeets et al., Chem Senses 33:725–734, 2008). In the first study, 455 college-aged heterosexual individuals were surveyed, while in the second study, 453 individuals varying in sexual preference (142 heterosexual women, 161 heterosexual men, and 150 gay men) completed the questionnaires.

Principle components analyses from both studies revealed two different components underlying scores on the RIS; one component best accounted for OAS scores. Regression analysis for both studies indicated that OAS scores predicted the first RIS principle component, but not the second one.

The value of odorants in selecting a romantic partner seems to reflect two different underlying attitudes. The first attitude values all aspects of the smell of a lover, while the second only finds it important that the lover does not smell badly. Odor awareness is related only to the first of these attitudes.

These findings suggest that odor awareness accounts for some of the attitudes concerning the value of odors in mate selection, but not all of them. Other factors, such as the need to avoid aversive stimuli, may also contribute to the relative importance of olfaction in selecting a partner.

许多人在寻找潜在的浪漫伴侣时,似乎都在寻找相似的品质。嗅觉社会线索可能是这一过程的重要组成部分,尽管它们的价值存在个体差异。例如,在选择浪漫伴侣时,男同性恋者比异性恋者更不看重气味(White和Cunningham, Chemosens perception 10:31-41, 2017)。嗅觉在择偶中的相对重要性是否可能仅仅是人们普遍意识到气味的自然结果?本研究通过两项研究考察了气味意识与交配中气味重要性之间的关系。每个研究的参与者都完成了浪漫兴趣调查(Herz和Inzlich,进化行为23:359-364,2002)和气味意识调查(Smeets et al.,化学感官33:725-734,2008)。在第一项研究中,455名大学年龄的异性恋者接受了调查,而在第二项研究中,453名性取向不同的人(142名异性恋女性,161名异性恋男性和150名同性恋男性)完成了问卷调查。两项研究的主成分分析揭示了RIS得分的两个不同成分;一个组成部分最能说明OAS分数。两项研究的回归分析表明,OAS分数预测RIS第一主成分,但不能预测RIS第二主成分。气味在选择浪漫伴侣时的价值似乎反映了两种不同的潜在态度。第一种态度重视爱人气味的各个方面,而第二种态度只认为爱人的气味不难闻是重要的。气味意识只与第一种态度有关。这些发现表明,气味意识解释了一些关于气味在择偶中的价值的态度,但不是全部。其他因素,如避免厌恶刺激的需要,也可能导致嗅觉在选择伴侣时的相对重要性。
{"title":"Olfactory Awareness and the Self-Reported Importance of Olfactory Information in Romantic Interest","authors":"Michelle VanHatten,&nbsp;Caitlin Cunningham,&nbsp;Theresa L. White","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9248-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9248-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Many people seem to be looking for similar attributes when searching for a potential romantic partner. Olfactory social cues can be important parts of the process, though there are individual differences as to their value. Gay men, for example, value scent less in selecting a romantic partner than do heterosexual men (White and Cunningham, Chemosens Percept 10:31–41, 2017). Is it possible that the relative importance of olfaction in mate selection is simply a natural consequence of being generally aware of odorants?</p><p>The present study examined the relationship between odor awareness and odor importance in mating in two studies. Participants in each of the studies completed both the Romantic Interests Survey (Herz and Inzlich, Evol Hum Behav 23:359–364, 2002) and the Odor Awareness Survey (Smeets et al., Chem Senses 33:725–734, 2008). In the first study, 455 college-aged heterosexual individuals were surveyed, while in the second study, 453 individuals varying in sexual preference (142 heterosexual women, 161 heterosexual men, and 150 gay men) completed the questionnaires.</p><p>Principle components analyses from both studies revealed two different components underlying scores on the RIS; one component best accounted for OAS scores. Regression analysis for both studies indicated that OAS scores predicted the first RIS principle component, but not the second one.</p><p>The value of odorants in selecting a romantic partner seems to reflect two different underlying attitudes. The first attitude values all aspects of the smell of a lover, while the second only finds it important that the lover does not smell badly. Odor awareness is related only to the first of these attitudes.</p><p>These findings suggest that odor awareness accounts for some of the attitudes concerning the value of odors in mate selection, but not all of them. Other factors, such as the need to avoid aversive stimuli, may also contribute to the relative importance of olfaction in selecting a partner.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-08-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9248-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4563565","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
How Differences in Ratings of Odors and Odor Labels Are Associated with Identification Mechanisms 气味等级和气味标签的差异如何与识别机制相关
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-07-26 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9247-9
Kathrin Kaeppler

Odor perception is biased by verbal–semantic processes when cues on an odor’s source are readily available from the context. At the same time, olfaction has been characterized as basically sensation driven when this information is absent. In the present study, we examined whether language effects occur when verbal cues are absent and how expectations about an odor’s identity shape odor evaluations.

A total of 56 subjects were asked to rate 20 unlabeled odor samples on perceptual dimensions as well as quality attributes and to eventually provide an odor source name. In a subsequent session, they performed the same rating tasks on a set of written odor labels that was compiled individually for each participant. It included both the 20 correct odor names (true labels) and in any case of incorrect odor naming in the first session, the self–generated labels (identified labels).

We compared odor ratings to ratings of both types of labels to test whether differences between odor and odor label evaluations were rooted in identification mechanisms. In cases of false identifications, we found higher consistencies between the evaluation of an odor and its identified label than between the description of an odor and its true (yet not associated) label.

These results indicate that odor evaluations are strongly affected by the mental image of an odor rather than the actual sensory codes and that this mental image is built spontaneously. Our findings imply that odors and odor labels are evaluated similarly for identical objects and that the differences found in similar studies may have been rooted in different mental representations being evaluated.

Odor sensations provoke odor naming without explicit demand. These self–generated hypotheses about an odor’s source exert a considerable semantic impact on odor perceptual processing, regardless of their accuracy.

当气味来源的线索很容易从上下文获得时,气味感知会受到语言语义过程的影响。与此同时,嗅觉的特点是,当信息缺失时,基本上是感觉驱动的。在本研究中,我们研究了语言效应是否会在语言线索缺失的情况下发生,以及对气味身份的预期如何影响气味评估。总共56名受试者被要求对20个未标记的气味样本在感知维度和质量属性上进行评分,并最终提供气味来源的名称。在接下来的一段时间里,他们对一组为每个参与者单独编写的书面气味标签执行了相同的评级任务。它既包括20个正确的气味名称(真实标签),也包括在第一次会话中任何不正确的气味命名的情况下,自己生成的标签(已识别的标签)。我们将气味评级与两种类型标签的评级进行比较,以测试气味和气味标签评估之间的差异是否源于识别机制。在错误识别的情况下,我们发现气味评估与其识别标签之间的一致性高于气味描述与其真实(但不相关)标签之间的一致性。这些结果表明,气味评价强烈地受到气味的心理形象的影响,而不是实际的感官编码,这种心理形象是自发建立的。我们的研究结果表明,对于相同的物体,气味和气味标签的评估是相似的,在类似的研究中发现的差异可能源于被评估的不同心理表征。气味感觉在没有明确要求的情况下激发气味命名。这些关于气味来源的自我产生的假设对气味感知处理施加了相当大的语义影响,无论其准确性如何。
{"title":"How Differences in Ratings of Odors and Odor Labels Are Associated with Identification Mechanisms","authors":"Kathrin Kaeppler","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9247-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9247-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Odor perception is biased by verbal–semantic processes when cues on an odor’s source are readily available from the context. At the same time, olfaction has been characterized as basically sensation driven when this information is absent. In the present study, we examined whether language effects occur when verbal cues are absent and how expectations about an odor’s identity shape odor evaluations.</p><p>A total of 56 subjects were asked to rate 20 unlabeled odor samples on perceptual dimensions as well as quality attributes and to eventually provide an odor source name. In a subsequent session, they performed the same rating tasks on a set of written odor labels that was compiled individually for each participant. It included both the 20 correct odor names (true labels) and in any case of incorrect odor naming in the first session, the self–generated labels (identified labels).</p><p>We compared odor ratings to ratings of both types of labels to test whether differences between odor and odor label evaluations were rooted in identification mechanisms. In cases of false identifications, we found higher consistencies between the evaluation of an odor and its identified label than between the description of an odor and its true (yet not associated) label.</p><p>These results indicate that odor evaluations are strongly affected by the mental image of an odor rather than the actual sensory codes and that this mental image is built spontaneously. Our findings imply that odors and odor labels are evaluated similarly for identical objects and that the differences found in similar studies may have been rooted in different mental representations being evaluated.</p><p>Odor sensations provoke odor naming without explicit demand. These self–generated hypotheses about an odor’s source exert a considerable semantic impact on odor perceptual processing, regardless of their accuracy.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9247-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"5409514","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Changes in Taste Threshold, Perceived Intensity, Liking, and Preference in Pregnant Women: a Literature Review 孕妇味觉阈值、感知强度、喜好和偏好的变化:文献综述
IF 1 4区 医学 Q4 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2018-07-09 DOI: 10.1007/s12078-018-9246-x
Hugo Weenen, Annemarie Olsen, Evangelia Nanou, Esmée Moreau, Smita Nambiar, Carel Vereijken, Leilani Muhardi

Studies of changes in taste threshold, perceived intensity, liking, and preference during pregnancy were reviewed, because such changes have the potential to negatively impact nutrient intake in pregnant women.

Medline and Web of Science were searched; eligibility was based on inclusion, exclusion, and quality criteria.

Fourteen articles were included: 5 reported taste thresholds, 8 taste intensity, and 13 liking/preferences. Articles addressed sweet, salty, sour, and bitter, not umami. Results of self-reported changes suggested that many women experienced some sort of alteration in taste during pregnancy. Studies with real stimuli demonstrated that the only consistent finding for taste thresholds was that pregnant women showed higher thresholds for bitterness in their first trimester. For taste intensity, no consistent differences between pregnant women and controls were observed. However, over the course of pregnancy, salty intensity seemed to decrease, the intensity of other tastes did not change. During pregnancy, higher saltiness was liked more and salty snacks were consumed more, particularly in the second and/or third trimester. Drinks with lower sweetness were preferred and intake of sweet snacks was highest in the second trimester. Preference for sour and bitter did not seem to be affected.

Self-reports suggested that many women experienced some taste changes during pregnancy, while changes based on studies with real stimuli were limited.

Many women experienced a higher threshold for bitter perception in the first trimester, a preference for sweet snacks in the second trimester, and higher saltiness appetite in the second and third trimester of pregnancy, which may have nutritional consequences.

我们回顾了怀孕期间味觉阈值、感知强度、喜好和偏好变化的研究,因为这些变化有可能对孕妇的营养摄入产生负面影响。检索Medline和Web of Science;入选资格基于纳入、排除和质量标准。包括14篇文章:5篇报道味觉阈值,8篇报道味觉强度,13篇报道喜欢/偏好。文章提到了甜、咸、酸、苦,而不是鲜味。自我报告的变化结果表明,许多女性在怀孕期间经历了某种味觉变化。对真实刺激的研究表明,在味觉阈值方面,唯一一致的发现是,孕妇在妊娠的前三个月对苦味的阈值更高。对于味觉强度,在孕妇和对照组之间没有观察到一致的差异。然而,在怀孕期间,咸的强度似乎降低了,其他口味的强度没有改变。在怀孕期间,人们更喜欢高盐,咸的零食也吃得更多,尤其是在怀孕的第二和/或第三个月。低甜度的饮料是首选,甜零食的摄入量在妊娠中期是最高的。对酸味和苦味的偏好似乎没有受到影响。自我报告表明,许多女性在怀孕期间经历了一些味觉变化,而基于真实刺激的研究的变化是有限的。许多女性在怀孕的前三个月对苦味的感知阈值更高,在怀孕的第二个三个月更喜欢吃甜食,在怀孕的第二个和第三个三个月更喜欢吃咸的东西,这可能会对营养产生影响。
{"title":"Changes in Taste Threshold, Perceived Intensity, Liking, and Preference in Pregnant Women: a Literature Review","authors":"Hugo Weenen,&nbsp;Annemarie Olsen,&nbsp;Evangelia Nanou,&nbsp;Esmée Moreau,&nbsp;Smita Nambiar,&nbsp;Carel Vereijken,&nbsp;Leilani Muhardi","doi":"10.1007/s12078-018-9246-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-018-9246-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studies of changes in taste threshold, perceived intensity, liking, and preference during pregnancy were reviewed, because such changes have the potential to negatively impact nutrient intake in pregnant women.</p><p>Medline and Web of Science were searched; eligibility was based on inclusion, exclusion, and quality criteria.</p><p>Fourteen articles were included: 5 reported taste thresholds, 8 taste intensity, and 13 liking/preferences. Articles addressed sweet, salty, sour, and bitter, not umami. Results of self-reported changes suggested that many women experienced some sort of alteration in taste during pregnancy. Studies with real stimuli demonstrated that the only consistent finding for taste thresholds was that pregnant women showed higher thresholds for bitterness in their first trimester. For taste intensity, no consistent differences between pregnant women and controls were observed. However, over the course of pregnancy, salty intensity seemed to decrease, the intensity of other tastes did not change. During pregnancy, higher saltiness was liked more and salty snacks were consumed more, particularly in the second and/or third trimester. Drinks with lower sweetness were preferred and intake of sweet snacks was highest in the second trimester. Preference for sour and bitter did not seem to be affected.</p><p>Self-reports suggested that many women experienced some taste changes during pregnancy, while changes based on studies with real stimuli were limited.</p><p>Many women experienced a higher threshold for bitter perception in the first trimester, a preference for sweet snacks in the second trimester, and higher saltiness appetite in the second and third trimester of pregnancy, which may have nutritional consequences.</p>","PeriodicalId":516,"journal":{"name":"Chemosensory Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.0,"publicationDate":"2018-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1007/s12078-018-9246-x","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"4388125","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
期刊
Chemosensory Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1