首页 > 最新文献

Shinrigaku Kenkyu最新文献

英文 中文
[The mutual interference of facial and vocal information in Chinese and Japanese people's perception of emotions]. [面部和声音信息在中日两国人情绪感知中的相互干扰]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.15032
Lian Cao, Shinkichi Sugimori, Fumiaki Taka

In this study, we investigated cultural differences in multisensory perception of emotion between Chinese and Japanese participants, focusing on mutual interference of visual and auditory emotional information. In this experiment, the face-voice pairs were consisted of congruent or incongruent emotions (e.g., a happy (an angry) face with a happy (an angry) voice in congruent pairs, and a happy (an angry) face with an angry (a happy) voice in incongruent pairs). Participants were asked to judge the emotion of targets focusing on either face or voice while ignoring the other modality’s information. In the voice-focus condition, the effect of to-be-ignored facial information was smaller in Japanese than Chinese participants, only when the participant and the target belonged to the same cultures (in-group). This indicated that Japanese people were more likely to be based on the voice information in multisensory perception of emotion of in-group. Our study illuminated that although both Japanese and Chinese people belonged to the Eastern culture, there were cultural differences in perceiving emotion from visual and auditory cues.

在本研究中,我们考察了中日两国被试在情绪多感官知觉上的文化差异,重点关注视觉和听觉情绪信息的相互干扰。在这个实验中,面孔-声音对由一致或不一致的情绪组成(例如,一致的组中是一张高兴(生气)的脸配一个高兴(生气)的声音,不一致的组中是一张高兴(生气)的脸配一个生气(高兴)的声音)。参与者被要求在忽略其他情态信息的情况下,只关注面部或声音来判断目标的情绪。在语音聚焦条件下,只有当被试和被试属于同一文化(群体内)时,日语被试对被忽略面部信息的影响小于汉语被试。这表明日本人更有可能基于语音信息进行多感官群体情绪感知。我们的研究表明,虽然日本人和中国人都属于东方文化,但在通过视觉和听觉线索感知情感方面存在文化差异。
{"title":"[The mutual interference of facial and vocal information in Chinese and Japanese people's perception of emotions].","authors":"Lian Cao,&nbsp;Shinkichi Sugimori,&nbsp;Fumiaki Taka","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.15032","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15032","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this study, we investigated cultural differences in multisensory perception of emotion between Chinese and Japanese participants, focusing on mutual interference of visual and auditory emotional information. In this experiment, the face-voice pairs were consisted of congruent or incongruent emotions (e.g., a happy (an angry) face with a happy (an angry) voice in congruent pairs, and a happy (an angry) face with an angry (a happy) voice in incongruent pairs). Participants were asked to judge the emotion of targets focusing on either face or voice while ignoring the other modality’s information. In the voice-focus condition, the effect of to-be-ignored facial information was smaller in Japanese than Chinese participants, only when the participant and the target belonged to the same cultures (in-group). This indicated that Japanese people were more likely to be based on the voice information in multisensory perception of emotion of in-group. Our study illuminated that although both Japanese and Chinese people belonged to the Eastern culture, there were cultural differences in perceiving emotion from visual and auditory cues.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15032","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987696","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Effects of help-seeker's predictions about helper's emotions and costs of help-seeking intentions]. [求助者对帮助者情绪的预测与求助意愿成本的影响]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.15331
Yasuko Takegahara, Hideo Ambo

The present study examined effects of predictions made by a help-seeker about the emotions and costs of a helper on help-seeking behavior. We surveyed undergraduate and graduate students who completed a questionnaire and scenario task. The scenario presented that a person would face the trouble. The questionnaire presented a case where a person was troubled. Respondents were required to answer the following questions: (a) whether they would seek help; (b) whether the helper would respond to help-seeking; (c) the helper’s costs; (d) the negative moods of the helper. Covariance structure analysis of 189 participant responses revealed that help-seeker predictions about the helper’s emotions and costs were used to predict the intentions of the helper, which affected the intentions to seek help. We discuss these results with respect to interpersonal cognition in intimate relationships.

本研究考察了求助者对求助者的情绪和成本的预测对求助者行为的影响。我们调查了完成问卷和情景任务的本科生和研究生。这个场景展示了一个人将面临的麻烦。问卷显示了一个人有问题的案例。被访者须回答以下问题:(一)他们会否寻求帮助;(二)佣工会否回应求助;(c)佣工的费用;(d)帮助者的消极情绪。对189名被试回答的协方差结构分析表明,求助者对帮助者情绪和成本的预测可以用来预测帮助者的意向,从而影响帮助者的求助意向。我们就亲密关系中的人际认知来讨论这些结果。
{"title":"[Effects of help-seeker's predictions about helper's emotions and costs of help-seeking intentions].","authors":"Yasuko Takegahara,&nbsp;Hideo Ambo","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.15331","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15331","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study examined effects of predictions made by a help-seeker about the emotions and costs of a helper on help-seeking behavior. We surveyed undergraduate and graduate students who completed a questionnaire and scenario task. The scenario presented that a person would face the trouble. The questionnaire presented a case where a person was troubled. Respondents were required to answer the following questions: (a) whether they would seek help; (b) whether the helper would respond to help-seeking; (c) the helper’s costs; (d) the negative moods of the helper. Covariance structure analysis of 189 participant responses revealed that help-seeker predictions about the helper’s emotions and costs were used to predict the intentions of the helper, which affected the intentions to seek help. We discuss these results with respect to interpersonal cognition in intimate relationships.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15331","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987587","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Do the explanations regarding the safety of Fukushima-produced foods ease consumer's concerns about disease?] 关于福岛生产的食品安全的解释是否减轻了消费者对疾病的担忧?]
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.16004
Osamu Higuchi, Kenji Hanita

Consumers have had concerns over the safety of Fukushima-produced foods since the Fukushima-Daiichi nuclear power plant accident. To dispel these concerns, the public administration has distributed the informational leaflets, which guarantee the safety of Fukushima-produced foods in the marketplace. We investigated the effectiveness of the leaflets. Previous research showed that the activation of behavioral immune system exacerbated prejudice toward out-group members. Therefore, we investigated whether reading the leaflets about the safety of foods would increase prejudice toward foreigners. Participants (N = 50) were asked to read a leaflet either relevant or irrelevant to the safety of Fukushima-produced foods and then complete a Japanese-Foreigners Implicit Association Test and Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Scale. As predicted, participants high in chronic germ aversion (GA) were more prejudiced against foreigners when reading the leaflet relevant to the safety of Fukushima-produced foods than when reading the leaflet irrelevant to the issue. No such effect was observed among participants low in GA. These results indicated the possibility that the current leaflet about the safety of Fukushima foods might backfire.

自福岛第一核电站事故以来,消费者一直对福岛生产的食品的安全性感到担忧。为了消除这些担忧,公共行政部门分发了信息传单,保证福岛生产的食品在市场上的安全。我们调查了传单的效果。先前的研究表明,行为免疫系统的激活加剧了对群体外成员的偏见。因此,我们调查阅读有关食品安全的传单是否会增加对外国人的偏见。参与者(N = 50)被要求阅读与福岛生产的食品安全相关或无关的传单,然后完成日本-外国人内隐联想测试和感知疾病脆弱性量表。正如预测的那样,在阅读与福岛生产的食品安全相关的传单时,慢性细菌厌恶(GA)高的参与者比阅读与该问题无关的传单时对外国人更有偏见。在GA低的参与者中没有观察到这种影响。这些结果表明,目前关于福岛食品安全的传单可能会适得其反。
{"title":"[Do the explanations regarding the safety of Fukushima-produced foods ease consumer's concerns about disease?]","authors":"Osamu Higuchi,&nbsp;Kenji Hanita","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.16004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.16004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Consumers have had concerns over the safety of Fukushima-produced foods since the Fukushima-Daiichi nuclear power plant accident. To dispel these concerns, the public administration has distributed the informational leaflets, which guarantee the safety of Fukushima-produced foods in the marketplace. We investigated the effectiveness of the leaflets. Previous research showed that the activation of behavioral immune system exacerbated prejudice toward out-group members. Therefore, we investigated whether reading the leaflets about the safety of foods would increase prejudice toward foreigners. Participants (N = 50) were asked to read a leaflet either relevant or irrelevant to the safety of Fukushima-produced foods and then complete a Japanese-Foreigners Implicit Association Test and Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Scale. As predicted, participants high in chronic germ aversion (GA) were more prejudiced against foreigners when reading the leaflet relevant to the safety of Fukushima-produced foods than when reading the leaflet irrelevant to the issue. No such effect was observed among participants low in GA. These results indicated the possibility that the current leaflet about the safety of Fukushima foods might backfire.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987589","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Development of a Japanese version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index]. [日语版人际反应指数的发展]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.15218
Toshiyuki Himichi, Hidekazu Osanai, Takayuki Goto, Hiroyo Fujita, Yuta Kawamura, Mark H Davis, Michio Nomura

Empathy is a multi-dimensional concept with emotional and cognitive components. The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is a multi-dimensional scale of empathic traits. Although some researchers have attempted to translate the IRI into Japanese, these translated scales had limitations with content and construct validity, and measurement invariance. We therefore attempt to overcome these limitations by developing a new Japanese version of the IRI (IRI-J). We used three approaches to assess the validity and measurement invariance of the IRI-J. In Study 1, content validity was tested using back-translation, and construct validity was confirmed through a comprehensive investigation of a web-based survey using six other scales. Results indicate that the factor structure of the IRI-J was equivalent to that of the original version, and that the IRI-J had adequate reliability and construct validity. In Study 2, measurement invariance by gender was confirmed using data from four web-based surveys. These results suggest that the factor model of IRI-J for each gender is equivalent. The present study thus provides an improved measure of empathic traits for the Japanese population.

共情是一个包含情感和认知成分的多维概念。人际反应指数(IRI)是一种多维共情特征量表。虽然一些研究者尝试将IRI量表翻译成日语,但这些翻译的量表在内容效度和结构效度以及测量不稳定性方面存在局限性。因此,我们试图通过开发一种新的日本版IRI (IRI- j)来克服这些限制。我们使用三种方法来评估IRI-J的效度和测量不变性。在研究1中,使用反翻译测试内容效度,并通过使用其他六个量表对基于网络的调查进行综合调查来确认结构效度。结果表明,本量表的因子结构与原量表相当,具有足够的信度和构念效度。在研究2中,使用四项基于网络的调查数据证实了性别的测量不变性。这些结果表明,不同性别的IRI-J因子模型是相同的。因此,本研究为日本人提供了一种改进的共情特征测量方法。
{"title":"[Development of a Japanese version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index].","authors":"Toshiyuki Himichi,&nbsp;Hidekazu Osanai,&nbsp;Takayuki Goto,&nbsp;Hiroyo Fujita,&nbsp;Yuta Kawamura,&nbsp;Mark H Davis,&nbsp;Michio Nomura","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.15218","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15218","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Empathy is a multi-dimensional concept with emotional and cognitive components. The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is a multi-dimensional scale of empathic traits. Although some researchers have attempted to translate the IRI into Japanese, these translated scales had limitations with content and construct validity, and measurement invariance. We therefore attempt to overcome these limitations by developing a new Japanese version of the IRI (IRI-J). We used three approaches to assess the validity and measurement invariance of the IRI-J. In Study 1, content validity was tested using back-translation, and construct validity was confirmed through a comprehensive investigation of a web-based survey using six other scales. Results indicate that the factor structure of the IRI-J was equivalent to that of the original version, and that the IRI-J had adequate reliability and construct validity. In Study 2, measurement invariance by gender was confirmed using data from four web-based surveys. These results suggest that the factor model of IRI-J for each gender is equivalent. The present study thus provides an improved measure of empathic traits for the Japanese population.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15218","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987592","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 44
[Effects of perceived benefits about when and how to use a learning strategy]. [关于何时以及如何使用学习策略的感知利益的影响]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.16007
Tsuyoshi Yamaguchi

Previous studies have shown a positive correlation between the use of a learning strategy and perceived benefits. However, the impact of perceived benefits as conditional knowledge in metacognitive knowledge has not been examined. The present study investigated the effect of perceived benefits about when (short and long) and how (persistent and suitable) to use learning strategies using two surveys. Each survey utilized a sample of undergraduate students from three different Japanese universities, and the Bayesian hierarchical modeling and within-person variance were used to verify the relationship between the use of a learning strategy and perceived benefits. The students completed questionnaires regarding cognitive (Survey 2a) and metacognitive (Survey 2b) strategies (Survey 1 included two strategies), and achievement goals (without Survey 1). Findings revealed the effects of long-term and short-term persistent perceived benefits on the use of a learning strategy in all the surveys. These results suggest that persistent benefits be emphasized in order to promote the use of learning strategies, and that the appropriate perceived benefits be emphasized in order to become an adaptive learner.

先前的研究表明,学习策略的使用与感知收益之间存在正相关关系。然而,知觉利益作为条件知识在元认知知识中的影响尚未得到检验。本研究通过两项调查,探讨了在何时(短期和长期)以及如何(持续和适当)使用学习策略的感知利益的影响。每次调查都使用了来自日本三所不同大学的本科生样本,并使用贝叶斯分层模型和人内方差来验证学习策略的使用与感知收益之间的关系。学生完成了关于认知(调查2a)和元认知(调查2b)策略(调查1包括两个策略)和成就目标(不包括调查1)的问卷调查。调查结果揭示了所有调查中长期和短期持续感知利益对学习策略使用的影响。这些结果表明,为了促进学习策略的使用,应强调持续的利益;为了成为一个适应性学习者,应强调适当的感知利益。
{"title":"[Effects of perceived benefits about when and how to use a learning strategy].","authors":"Tsuyoshi Yamaguchi","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.16007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.16007","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have shown a positive correlation between the use of a learning strategy and perceived benefits. However, the impact of perceived benefits as conditional knowledge in metacognitive knowledge has not been examined. The present study investigated the effect of perceived benefits about when (short and long) and how (persistent and suitable) to use learning strategies using two surveys. Each survey utilized a sample of undergraduate students from three different Japanese universities, and the Bayesian hierarchical modeling and within-person variance were used to verify the relationship between the use of a learning strategy and perceived benefits. The students completed questionnaires regarding cognitive (Survey 2a) and metacognitive (Survey 2b) strategies (Survey 1 included two strategies), and achievement goals (without Survey 1). Findings revealed the effects of long-term and short-term persistent perceived benefits on the use of a learning strategy in all the surveys. These results suggest that persistent benefits be emphasized in order to promote the use of learning strategies, and that the appropriate perceived benefits be emphasized in order to become an adaptive learner.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987591","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
[Information on student counseling center websites in Japan, the United States, the United Kingdom, and Taiwan]. 【日本、美国、英国、台湾地区学生咨询中心网站信息】
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.16302
Naoki Ito
This study aimed to compare information provided on student counseling center websites of universities and colleges in Japan, the United States, the United Kingdom, and Taiwan. A survey was conducted on websites of 315 centers in Japan, 282 centers in the United States, 70 centers in the United Kingdom and 61 centers in Taiwan. Trends in the provision of information on websites in each country were analyzed and compared for the rate and quantity of information published. Results of multiple correspondence analyses indicated two basic dimensions of information that could effectively distinguish information provided in the four countries. These were provision of necessary information and provision of information for use of individual counseling or support of community. Finally, issues related to websites in student counseling centers of Japanese universities and colleges are discussed.
本研究旨在比较日本、美国、英国及台湾的高校学生辅导中心网站所提供的资讯。此次调查是在日本315个、美国282个、英国70个、台湾61个中心的网站上进行的。对每个国家在网站上提供信息的趋势进行了分析,并就发布信息的速度和数量进行了比较。多重对应分析的结果表明,信息的两个基本维度可以有效地区分四个国家提供的信息。这些是提供必要的信息和提供个人咨询或支持社区的信息。最后,对日本高校学生辅导中心网站的相关问题进行了探讨。
{"title":"[Information on student counseling center websites in Japan, the United States, the United Kingdom, and Taiwan].","authors":"Naoki Ito","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.16302","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.16302","url":null,"abstract":"This study aimed to compare information provided on student counseling center websites of universities and colleges in Japan, the United States, the United Kingdom, and Taiwan. A survey was conducted on websites of 315 centers in Japan, 282 centers in the United States, 70 centers in the United Kingdom and 61 centers in Taiwan. Trends in the provision of information on websites in each country were analyzed and compared for the rate and quantity of information published. Results of multiple correspondence analyses indicated two basic dimensions of information that could effectively distinguish information provided in the four countries. These were provision of necessary information and provision of information for use of individual counseling or support of community. Finally, issues related to websites in student counseling centers of Japanese universities and colleges are discussed.","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987588","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Relationships between motivational regulation strategies, motivational factors, and learning behaviors outside the classroom]. [动机调节策略、动机因素与课堂外学习行为的关系]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.16308
Takatoyo Umemoto, Kenshiro Tanaka

This study examined the relationships among motivational regulation strategies, motivational factors, and learning behaviors outside the classroom. There are three subtypes of motivational regulation strategies: autonomous regulation strategies, cooperative strategies, and performance-focused strategies. Motivational factors included in the investigation were self-efficacy and task value, while behavioral and emotional engagement and study time were selected as learning behaviors outside the classroom. A self-report questionnaire was administered to 322 undergraduates from two universities. Multiple regression analysis revealed the use of autonomous regulation strategies, and that task value was positively correlated with engagement and study time. Moreover, self-efficacy positively predicted study time. In contrast, the use of performance strategies negatively predicted engagement. The use of cooperative strategies did not predict learning behaviors. These results indicate that motivation, as well as the regulation of motivation, were important for learning outside the classroom. The effects of regulation of motivation and motivation on learning outside the classroom are discussed in light of the current findings.

本研究探讨动机调节策略、动机因素与课堂外学习行为的关系。动机调节策略有三种类型:自主调节策略、合作调节策略和绩效调节策略。调查的动机因素包括自我效能感和任务价值,而行为和情感投入和学习时间作为课堂外的学习行为。对来自两所大学的322名大学生进行了自我报告问卷调查。多元回归分析显示自主调节策略的使用,任务价值与投入和学习时间呈正相关。此外,自我效能正向预测学习时间。相反,绩效策略的使用对敬业度有负面预测。合作策略的使用并不能预测学习行为。这些结果表明,动机以及对动机的调节对课堂外的学习很重要。根据目前的研究结果,讨论了动机调节和动机对课堂外学习的影响。
{"title":"[Relationships between motivational regulation strategies, motivational factors, and learning behaviors outside the classroom].","authors":"Takatoyo Umemoto,&nbsp;Kenshiro Tanaka","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.16308","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.16308","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined the relationships among motivational regulation strategies, motivational factors, and learning behaviors outside the classroom. There are three subtypes of motivational regulation strategies: autonomous regulation strategies, cooperative strategies, and performance-focused strategies. Motivational factors included in the investigation were self-efficacy and task value, while behavioral and emotional engagement and study time were selected as learning behaviors outside the classroom. A self-report questionnaire was administered to 322 undergraduates from two universities. Multiple regression analysis revealed the use of autonomous regulation strategies, and that task value was positively correlated with engagement and study time. Moreover, self-efficacy positively predicted study time. In contrast, the use of performance strategies negatively predicted engagement. The use of cooperative strategies did not predict learning behaviors. These results indicate that motivation, as well as the regulation of motivation, were important for learning outside the classroom. The effects of regulation of motivation and motivation on learning outside the classroom are discussed in light of the current findings.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.88.16308","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987590","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
[Japanese response bias: Cross-level and cross-national comparisons on response styles]. [日本人的反应偏差:反应风格的跨水平和跨国比较]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.15065
Katsuya Tasaki, Jiwon Shin

This study attempts to identify response styles of Japanese students by cross-level and cross-national analyses. Tasaki and Ninomiya (2013) demonstrated the existence of an acquiescence response behavior of Japanese students based on the model by Billet and McClendon(2000), who measured latent response characteristics as “style factor.” In this study, we used a different response style measurement model proposed by Weijters, Schillewaert, & Geuens (2008) positing that response styles are psychological constructs and observed response style variables are summary indicators derived from sets of various, but independent, test items. Results of cross-national analyses indicated that Japanese and Korean participants were more likely to choose a mid-point response (MRS) and less likely to choose an acquiescence response (ARS) than American counterparts, but no significant difference was observed for an extreme response (ERS) among participants from the three nations. Within the Japanese response style model, cross-level analyses showed that ARS scores were significantly higher than ERS scores, indicating that Japanese participants had an acquiescence response style, consistent with the findings from Tasaki & Ninomiya (2013).

本研究试图通过跨水平和跨国家的分析来确定日本学生的反应风格。Tasaki和Ninomiya(2013)基于Billet和McClendon(2000)的模型证明了日本学生存在默认反应行为,后者将潜在反应特征测量为“风格因素”。在本研究中,我们使用了Weijters, Schillewaert, & Geuens(2008)提出的一种不同的反应风格测量模型,该模型假设反应风格是心理建构,观察到的反应风格变量是来自一系列不同但独立的测试项目的汇总指标。跨国分析的结果表明,日本和韩国的参与者比美国的参与者更倾向于选择中点反应(MRS),而不太可能选择默许反应(ARS),但在极端反应(ERS)上,三个国家的参与者没有观察到显著差异。在日本反应风格模型中,跨水平分析显示,ARS得分显著高于ERS得分,表明日本参与者具有默认反应风格,这与Tasaki & Ninomiya(2013)的研究结果一致。
{"title":"[Japanese response bias: Cross-level and cross-national comparisons on response styles].","authors":"Katsuya Tasaki,&nbsp;Jiwon Shin","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.15065","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15065","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study attempts to identify response styles of Japanese students by cross-level and cross-national analyses. Tasaki and Ninomiya (2013) demonstrated the existence of an acquiescence response behavior of Japanese students based on the model by Billet and McClendon(2000), who measured latent response characteristics as “style factor.” In this study, we used a different response style measurement model proposed by Weijters, Schillewaert, & Geuens (2008) positing that response styles are psychological constructs and observed response style variables are summary indicators derived from sets of various, but independent, test items. Results of cross-national analyses indicated that Japanese and Korean participants were more likely to choose a mid-point response (MRS) and less likely to choose an acquiescence response (ARS) than American counterparts, but no significant difference was observed for an extreme response (ERS) among participants from the three nations. Within the Japanese response style model, cross-level analyses showed that ARS scores were significantly higher than ERS scores, indicating that Japanese participants had an acquiescence response style, consistent with the findings from Tasaki & Ninomiya (2013).</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15065","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987698","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 19
[Independence of old and new item processing in recognition judgment]. [识别判断中新旧项目处理的独立性]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-04-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.88.15061
Akira Takahashi

This study investigated the independence of old and new item processing in recognition judgment. Conventional recognition studies have focused on the “memory” aspects of recognition judgment, while new item processing has been treated as a “residual response” of old item processing. However, prior studies on confidence ratings and the brain research suggest the possibility that old and new items are independently processed. This study conducted recognition judgment tests with the number of learned items as independent variables, and response times for four answer categories (hit, miss, correct rejection, and false alarm) as dependent variables. Hit response times were consistently shorter than misses for all old items, while correct rejection (CR) and false alarm (FA) response times approached equivalence (CR = FA) for new items as the number of old (memorized) items increased. These results suggest that recognition judgment changes according to the number of old (memorized) items. We discuss the idea that new item and old item processing in recognition judgment occur independently.

本研究考察了新旧项目加工在识别判断中的独立性。传统的识别研究主要集中在识别判断的“记忆”方面,而新项目加工一直被视为旧项目加工的“剩余反应”。然而,先前关于信心评级和大脑研究的研究表明,新旧物品可能是独立处理的。本研究以学习项目的数量为自变量,以命中、未命中、正确拒绝和误报四种答案类别的反应时间为因变量,进行识别判断测试。对于所有旧项目,命中响应时间始终短于未命中,而随着旧(记忆)项目数量的增加,新项目的正确拒绝(CR)和误报(FA)响应时间接近相等(CR = FA)。这些结果表明,识别判断根据旧(记忆)项目的数量而变化。我们讨论了识别判断中新项目和旧项目加工是独立发生的观点。
{"title":"[Independence of old and new item processing in recognition judgment].","authors":"Akira Takahashi","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.88.15061","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15061","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated the independence of old and new item processing in recognition judgment. Conventional recognition studies have focused on the “memory” aspects of recognition judgment, while new item processing has been treated as a “residual response” of old item processing. However, prior studies on confidence ratings and the brain research suggest the possibility that old and new items are independently processed. This study conducted recognition judgment tests with the number of learned items as independent variables, and response times for four answer categories (hit, miss, correct rejection, and false alarm) as dependent variables. Hit response times were consistently shorter than misses for all old items, while correct rejection (CR) and false alarm (FA) response times approached equivalence (CR = FA) for new items as the number of old (memorized) items increased. These results suggest that recognition judgment changes according to the number of old (memorized) items. We discuss the idea that new item and old item processing in recognition judgment occur independently.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.88.15061","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987173","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
[Prisoners’ reasons for confessions and interviewing styles]. [犯人招供的理由和约谈方式]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2017-02-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.16002
Taeko Wachi, Kazumi Watanabe, Kaeko Yokota, Yusuke Otsuka, Michael E Lamb

This study examined the relationships between the reasons for confessions and interviewing styles by administering a self-reported questionnaire to new male adult prison inmates across Japan. The three factors proposed by Gudjonsson and his colleagues (1991, 1992, 1994, 1999), namely, perception of proof, internal pressure, and external pressure, were investigated. When participants had decided to confess prior to interviews, they were more likely to confess due to perception of proof and internal pressure compared to their counterparts. Furthermore, participants who experienced a relationship-focused interviewing style, which stressed active listening and rapport-building while talking about the criminal incidents directly, were more likely to confess due to internal pressure and less likely to confess due to external pressure.

本研究通过对日本各地新入监狱的成年男性囚犯进行自我报告问卷调查,考察了供述原因与访谈方式之间的关系。对Gudjonsson及其同事(1991,1992,1994,1999)提出的证据感知、内部压力和外部压力三个因素进行了研究。当参与者在采访前决定坦白时,他们更有可能因为对证据的感知和内心压力而坦白。此外,经历过以关系为中心的采访风格的参与者,在直接谈论犯罪事件时强调积极倾听和建立融洽关系,更有可能在内部压力下坦白,而在外部压力下不太可能坦白。
{"title":"[Prisoners’ reasons for confessions and interviewing styles].","authors":"Taeko Wachi,&nbsp;Kazumi Watanabe,&nbsp;Kaeko Yokota,&nbsp;Yusuke Otsuka,&nbsp;Michael E Lamb","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.16002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.16002","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined the relationships between the reasons for confessions and interviewing styles by administering a self-reported questionnaire to new male adult prison inmates across Japan. The three factors proposed by Gudjonsson and his colleagues (1991, 1992, 1994, 1999), namely, perception of proof, internal pressure, and external pressure, were investigated. When participants had decided to confess prior to interviews, they were more likely to confess due to perception of proof and internal pressure compared to their counterparts. Furthermore, participants who experienced a relationship-focused interviewing style, which stressed active listening and rapport-building while talking about the criminal incidents directly, were more likely to confess due to internal pressure and less likely to confess due to external pressure.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987262","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Shinrigaku Kenkyu
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1