首页 > 最新文献

Shinrigaku Kenkyu最新文献

英文 中文
[Social capital is associated with students’ subjective well-being in 1st year university life]. [社会资本与大学生大学一年级主观幸福感相关]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.15010
Michimasa Haga, Keisuke Takano, Kazunori Hanyu, Masayuki Nishikawa, Shinji Sakamoto

The purpose of this study was to examine how university social capital and subjective social capital could predict undergraduate students’ subjective well-being including depression, school satisfaction, and life satisfaction. In this cross-sectional study, we conducted multilevel structural equation modeling on the data of 2,021 students at 38 universities in Japan. At the university level, we found the associations between social capital (fellows) and depression, social capital (classmates) and life satisfaction, and social capital (faculty) and school satisfaction. At the student level, all subjective social capital (fellows, classmates, and faculty) were associated with all the factors of subjective well-being. These results suggest the influence of university social capital and that of subjective social capital are associated with students’ subjective well-being.

摘要本研究旨在探讨大学社会资本和主观社会资本对大学生主观幸福感(抑郁、学业满意度和生活满意度)的预测作用。在横断面研究中,我们对日本38所大学的2021名学生的数据进行了多层次结构方程建模。在大学层面,我们发现社会资本(同伴)与抑郁、社会资本(同学)与生活满意度、社会资本(教师)与学校满意度之间存在关联。在学生水平上,所有主观社会资本(同伴、同学和教师)都与主观幸福感的所有因素相关。这些结果表明,大学社会资本和主观社会资本的影响与学生的主观幸福感有关。
{"title":"[Social capital is associated with students’ subjective well-being in 1st year university life].","authors":"Michimasa Haga,&nbsp;Keisuke Takano,&nbsp;Kazunori Hanyu,&nbsp;Masayuki Nishikawa,&nbsp;Shinji Sakamoto","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.15010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15010","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of this study was to examine how university social capital and subjective social capital could predict undergraduate students’ subjective well-being including depression, school satisfaction, and life satisfaction. In this cross-sectional study, we conducted multilevel structural equation modeling on the data of 2,021 students at 38 universities in Japan. At the university level, we found the associations between social capital (fellows) and depression, social capital (classmates) and life satisfaction, and social capital (faculty) and school satisfaction. At the student level, all subjective social capital (fellows, classmates, and faculty) were associated with all the factors of subjective well-being. These results suggest the influence of university social capital and that of subjective social capital are associated with students’ subjective well-being.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15010","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987884","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
[The characteristics of altruistic behavior that can sustain generalized exchange in a society composed of two groups]. [在由两个群体组成的社会中能够维持普遍交换的利他行为的特征]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.14080
Ryoichi Onoda, Nobuyuki Takahashi

Previous studies on generalized exchange have argued that group plays an important role in the emergenceof cooperative society. To examine to what extent the role of a group is important, we conducted computersimulations in which players decide whether to give resources to members of a society composed of two groups.We examined whether a society consisting of any of the possible conceivable strategies (65536 strategies total)could resist invasion by an unconditional defector (ALLD) and an unconditional cooperator (ALLC). Theresults showed that universalist strategies, which give resources to both in-group members and out-groupmembers equally, and in-group favoring strategies, which give resources to in-group members more than outgroupmembers, could resist invasion. Furthermore, we found that in-group favoring strategies could excludeALLC from the circle of resource flow more easily than universalist strategies. These results imply that it may benecessary to employ an in-group favoring strategy that utilizes the group membership information of other peoplein order to maintain generalized exchange in a society composed of two groups.

以往关于广义交换的研究认为,群体在合作社会的产生中起着重要作用。为了检验一个群体的角色在多大程度上是重要的,我们进行了计算机模拟,让玩家决定是否向由两个群体组成的社会成员提供资源。我们研究了一个由任何可能的可想象策略(65536个策略)组成的社会是否能够抵御无条件叛逆者(ALLD)和无条件合作者(ALLC)的入侵。结果表明,普遍主义策略(对内和对外给予同等资源)和内群体偏好策略(对内比外群体给予更多资源)均能抵御外来入侵。此外,我们发现群体内偏好策略比普遍主义策略更容易将企业排除在资源流动圈之外。这些结果表明,为了在一个由两个群体组成的社会中维持普遍的交换,可能有必要采用一种利用其他人的群体成员信息的群体内偏好策略。
{"title":"[The characteristics of altruistic behavior that can sustain generalized exchange in a society composed of two groups].","authors":"Ryoichi Onoda,&nbsp;Nobuyuki Takahashi","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.14080","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.14080","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies on generalized exchange have argued that group plays an important role in the emergence\u0000of cooperative society. To examine to what extent the role of a group is important, we conducted computer\u0000simulations in which players decide whether to give resources to members of a society composed of two groups.\u0000We examined whether a society consisting of any of the possible conceivable strategies (65536 strategies total)\u0000could resist invasion by an unconditional defector (ALLD) and an unconditional cooperator (ALLC). The\u0000results showed that universalist strategies, which give resources to both in-group members and out-group\u0000members equally, and in-group favoring strategies, which give resources to in-group members more than outgroup\u0000members, could resist invasion. Furthermore, we found that in-group favoring strategies could exclude\u0000ALLC from the circle of resource flow more easily than universalist strategies. These results imply that it may be\u0000necessary to employ an in-group favoring strategy that utilizes the group membership information of other people\u0000in order to maintain generalized exchange in a society composed of two groups.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35989472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Validation of a Japanese version of the Experience in Close Relationship- Relationship Structure]. [验证日本版的亲密关系经验-关系结构]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.15208
Kentaro Komura, Tatsuya Murakami, Koji Toda

The purpose of this study was to translate the Experience of Close Relationship-Relationship Structure (ECRRS) and evaluate its validity. In study 1 (N = 982), evidence based internal structure (factor structure, internal consistency, and correlation among sub-scales) and evidence based relations to other variables (depression, reassurance seeking and self-esteem) were confirmed. In study 2 (N = 563), evidence based on internal structure was reconfirmed, and evidence based relations to other variables (IWMS, RQ, and ECR-GO) were confirmed. In study 3 (N = 342), evidence based internal structure (test-retest reliability) was confirmed. Based on these results, we concluded that ECR-RS was valid for measuring adult attachment style.

本研究的目的是翻译亲密关系-关系结构的经验(ECRRS),并评估其效度。研究1 (N = 982)证实了基于证据的内部结构(因子结构、内部一致性、子量表间的相关性)和与其他变量(抑郁、寻求安慰和自尊)的关系。在研究2 (N = 563)中,再次确认了基于内部结构的证据,并确认了与其他变量(IWMS、RQ、ECR-GO)的证据关系。在研究3 (N = 342)中,证实了基于证据的内部结构(重测信度)。基于这些结果,我们得出结论,ECR-RS是有效的测量成人依恋类型。
{"title":"[Validation of a Japanese version of the Experience in Close Relationship- Relationship Structure].","authors":"Kentaro Komura,&nbsp;Tatsuya Murakami,&nbsp;Koji Toda","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.15208","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15208","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of this study was to translate the Experience of Close Relationship-Relationship Structure (ECRRS) and evaluate its validity. In study 1 (N = 982), evidence based internal structure (factor structure, internal consistency, and correlation among sub-scales) and evidence based relations to other variables (depression, reassurance seeking and self-esteem) were confirmed. In study 2 (N = 563), evidence based on internal structure was reconfirmed, and evidence based relations to other variables (IWMS, RQ, and ECR-GO) were confirmed. In study 3 (N = 342), evidence based internal structure (test-retest reliability) was confirmed. Based on these results, we concluded that ECR-RS was valid for measuring adult attachment style.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15208","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35987885","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
[The effects of security camera, past littering, environment, and signboards on littering prevention]. [监控摄像头、过去乱扔垃圾、环境、招牌对防止乱扔垃圾的影响]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.14057
Tomoko Nakamata, Tsuneyuki Abe

Two experiments were conducted to assess litter control at a riverside location. Experiment 1 examined theeffects of a security camera (presence/absence), past littering (presence/absence), and environmental features(tussock/plain ground/flowerbed). Two scenes containing combinations of these factors were presented.Participants chose the scene in which they felt it was to easier to litter. Participants also reported their emotionalresponse to the presence/absence of a security camera and environmental features in scenes with litter. The resultsrevealed that the presence of a security camera, no past littering, flowerbeds, and plain ground inhibited littering.Littering in the presence of a security camera facilitated discomfort, anger, and shame, and littering in flowerbedscaused discomfort, anger, shame, and sadness. Using a similar method, Experiment 2 addressed the particulareffects of a security camera combined with other factors: past littering, environmental features, and signboards(no sign/sign with eyes/security camera images). The results demonstrated the effectiveness of a security camera,no past littering, flowerbeds, plain ground, signboards presenting eyes, and images from a security camera inpreventing littering.

进行了两项实验,以评估河滨地区的垃圾控制。实验1考察了安全摄像头(存在/不存在)、过去乱扔垃圾(存在/不存在)和环境特征(草丛/平原/花坛)的影响。提出了两个包含这些因素组合的场景。参与者选择了他们认为更容易乱扔垃圾的场景。参与者还报告了他们对是否有监控摄像头和有垃圾场景的环境特征的情绪反应。结果显示,有安全摄像头、过去没有乱扔垃圾、花圃和平坦的地面都抑制了乱扔垃圾。在监控摄像头前乱扔垃圾会让人感到不适、愤怒和羞耻,而在花坛乱扔垃圾会让人感到不适、愤怒、羞耻和悲伤。使用类似的方法,实验2解决了安全摄像头结合其他因素的特殊影响:过去的垃圾,环境特征和招牌(没有标志/有眼睛的标志/安全摄像头图像)。结果表明,安装监控摄像头、不乱扔垃圾、花圃、平整的地面、展示眼睛的指示牌、监控摄像头拍摄的图像等措施在防止乱扔垃圾方面是有效的。
{"title":"[The effects of security camera, past littering, environment, and signboards on littering prevention].","authors":"Tomoko Nakamata,&nbsp;Tsuneyuki Abe","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.14057","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.14057","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two experiments were conducted to assess litter control at a riverside location. Experiment 1 examined the\u0000effects of a security camera (presence/absence), past littering (presence/absence), and environmental features\u0000(tussock/plain ground/flowerbed). Two scenes containing combinations of these factors were presented.\u0000Participants chose the scene in which they felt it was to easier to litter. Participants also reported their emotional\u0000response to the presence/absence of a security camera and environmental features in scenes with litter. The results\u0000revealed that the presence of a security camera, no past littering, flowerbeds, and plain ground inhibited littering.\u0000Littering in the presence of a security camera facilitated discomfort, anger, and shame, and littering in flowerbeds\u0000caused discomfort, anger, shame, and sadness. Using a similar method, Experiment 2 addressed the particular\u0000effects of a security camera combined with other factors: past littering, environmental features, and signboards\u0000(no sign/sign with eyes/security camera images). The results demonstrated the effectiveness of a security camera,\u0000no past littering, flowerbeds, plain ground, signboards presenting eyes, and images from a security camera in\u0000preventing littering.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.87.14057","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35989470","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
[Effects of parental parenting attitudes on adolescents’ socialization via adolescents’ perceived parenting]. 父母教养态度对青少年社会化的影响[j]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.15013
Ryosuke Asano, Hiroyuki Yoshizawa, Takuya Yoshida, Chika Harada, Ryuichi Tamai, Toshikazu Yoshida

The domain-specific approach to socialization has classified socialization mechanisms into several domains, including the protection and control domains, and postulates that parent–child interactions that promote socialization in each domain are different. However, there are few empirical investigations of the domain–specific approach. This study examined whether parental parenting attitudes affected early adolescents’ empathy, including empathic concern and perspective taking, and social cognitive biases, including cognitive distortion and general beliefs about aggression, through the mediation of adolescents’ perceptions. Junior high school students and their parents (N = 448) completed a questionnaire. Results of structural equation modeling indicated (a) parental acceptance and control increased empathy via adolescents’ perceived acceptance and control, (b) parental acceptance and control decreased social cognitive biases via adolescents’ perceived acceptance and control, and (c) parental control directly increased empathy. In addition, multiple group analyses indicated the validity of gender- and age-invariant models. These findings suggest that parental parenting attitudes are essential for appropriate socialization during early adolescence.

特定领域的社会化方法将社会化机制分为几个领域,包括保护领域和控制领域,并假设在每个领域中促进社会化的亲子互动是不同的。然而,对领域特定方法的实证研究很少。本研究考察了父母教养态度是否通过青少年感知的中介影响早期青少年的共情(包括共情关怀和观点采纳)和社会认知偏差(包括认知扭曲和对攻击的一般信念)。初中生及其家长(N = 448)填写问卷。结构方程模型结果表明:(a)父母接受和控制通过青少年的接受和控制感知增加共情,(b)父母接受和控制通过青少年的接受和控制感知减少社会认知偏差,(c)父母控制直接增加共情。此外,多组分析表明了性别和年龄不变模型的有效性。这些发现表明,父母的教养态度对青少年早期适当的社会化至关重要。
{"title":"[Effects of parental parenting attitudes on adolescents’ socialization via adolescents’ perceived parenting].","authors":"Ryosuke Asano,&nbsp;Hiroyuki Yoshizawa,&nbsp;Takuya Yoshida,&nbsp;Chika Harada,&nbsp;Ryuichi Tamai,&nbsp;Toshikazu Yoshida","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.15013","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15013","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The domain-specific approach to socialization has classified socialization mechanisms into several domains, including the protection and control domains, and postulates that parent–child interactions that promote socialization in each domain are different. However, there are few empirical investigations of the domain–specific approach. This study examined whether parental parenting attitudes affected early adolescents’ empathy, including empathic concern and perspective taking, and social cognitive biases, including cognitive distortion and general beliefs about aggression, through the mediation of adolescents’ perceptions. Junior high school students and their parents (N = 448) completed a questionnaire. Results of structural equation modeling indicated (a) parental acceptance and control increased empathy via adolescents’ perceived acceptance and control, (b) parental acceptance and control decreased social cognitive biases via adolescents’ perceived acceptance and control, and (c) parental control directly increased empathy. In addition, multiple group analyses indicated the validity of gender- and age-invariant models. These findings suggest that parental parenting attitudes are essential for appropriate socialization during early adolescence.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15013","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35989474","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
[Motivational influences of task difficulty information on task effort]. [任务难度信息对任务努力的动机影响]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.15008
Kenshiro Ichimura, Yoshiyuki Ueda, Takashi Kusumi

Two studies were conducted to investigate the motivational influences of information about task difficulty on task effort. In both studies, an anagram task was used as the experimental task and task motivation was measured with rating scales. In experiment 1, 60 participants were presented anagrams labeled as “easy” or “difficult”, both of which were actually impossible to solve. Results revealed that participants low in intrinsic motivation put in more effort on anagrams labeled “easy” than “difficult”. In experiment 2, 60 participants were assigned to two groups (30 each) and task outcomes were manipulated (positive and negative). Results revealed that participants with positive outcomes could maintain task motivation and put in more effort on the challenging anagrams than those with negative outcomes. These results suggest that information about task difficulty can promote regulation of task effort and task motivation for sustained studying, especially for computer-based learning.

两项研究探讨了任务难度信息对任务努力的动机影响。两项研究均采用字谜任务作为实验任务,并采用评定量表测量任务动机。在实验1中,60名参与者被展示了标记为“容易”或“困难”的字谜,这两个字谜实际上都是不可能解决的。结果显示,内在动机低的参与者在“容易”字谜上比“困难”字谜投入更多的精力。在实验2中,60名参与者被分为两组(每组30人),任务结果被操纵(积极和消极)。结果表明,积极结果的参与者比消极结果的参与者更能保持任务动机,并在具有挑战性的字谜上投入更多的精力。这些结果表明,任务难度信息可以促进任务努力和任务动机的调节,以促进持续学习,特别是在基于计算机的学习中。
{"title":"[Motivational influences of task difficulty information on task effort].","authors":"Kenshiro Ichimura,&nbsp;Yoshiyuki Ueda,&nbsp;Takashi Kusumi","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.15008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15008","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Two studies were conducted to investigate the motivational influences of information about task difficulty on task effort. In both studies, an anagram task was used as the experimental task and task motivation was measured with rating scales. In experiment 1, 60 participants were presented anagrams labeled as “easy” or “difficult”, both of which were actually impossible to solve. Results revealed that participants low in intrinsic motivation put in more effort on anagrams labeled “easy” than “difficult”. In experiment 2, 60 participants were assigned to two groups (30 each) and task outcomes were manipulated (positive and negative). Results revealed that participants with positive outcomes could maintain task motivation and put in more effort on the challenging anagrams than those with negative outcomes. These results suggest that information about task difficulty can promote regulation of task effort and task motivation for sustained studying, especially for computer-based learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15008","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35989475","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
[Dual process in large number estimation under uncertainty]. [不确定条件下大量估计的对偶过程]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.14077
Miki Matsumuro, Kazuhisa Miwa, Hitoshi Terai, Kento Yamada

According to dual process theory, there are two systems in the mind: an intuitive and automatic System 1 anda logical and effortful System 2. While many previous studies about number estimation have focused on simpleheuristics and automatic processes, the deliberative System 2 process has not been sufficiently studied. This studyfocused on the System 2 process for large number estimation. First, we described an estimation process basedon participants’ verbal reports. The task, corresponding to the problem-solving process, consisted of creatingsubgoals, retrieving values, and applying operations. Second, we investigated the influence of such deliberativeprocess by System 2 on intuitive estimation by System 1, using anchoring effects. The results of the experimentshowed that the System 2 process could mitigate anchoring effects.

根据双重过程理论,大脑有两个系统:直觉和自动的系统1和逻辑和努力的系统2。虽然以前许多关于数字估计的研究都集中在简单的启发式和自动过程上,但审议系统2的过程尚未得到充分的研究。本研究的重点是系统2过程的大数字估计。首先,我们描述了一个基于参与者口头报告的评估过程。与问题解决过程相对应的任务由创建子目标、检索值和应用操作组成。其次,我们利用锚定效应研究了系统2的审议过程对系统1直觉估计的影响。实验结果表明,系统2过程可以减轻锚定效应。
{"title":"[Dual process in large number estimation under uncertainty].","authors":"Miki Matsumuro,&nbsp;Kazuhisa Miwa,&nbsp;Hitoshi Terai,&nbsp;Kento Yamada","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.14077","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.14077","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to dual process theory, there are two systems in the mind: an intuitive and automatic System 1 and\u0000a logical and effortful System 2. While many previous studies about number estimation have focused on simple\u0000heuristics and automatic processes, the deliberative System 2 process has not been sufficiently studied. This study\u0000focused on the System 2 process for large number estimation. First, we described an estimation process based\u0000on participants’ verbal reports. The task, corresponding to the problem-solving process, consisted of creating\u0000subgoals, retrieving values, and applying operations. Second, we investigated the influence of such deliberative\u0000process by System 2 on intuitive estimation by System 1, using anchoring effects. The results of the experiment\u0000showed that the System 2 process could mitigate anchoring effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35989471","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
[Effective encoding for the recognition of spatial configuration and color]. [空间形态和颜色识别的有效编码]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-08-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.15004
Taku Morimoto

Three experiments examined the types of encoding that were effective for the recognition of spatial and color information. In Experiments 1 and 2, four experimental groups (each asked to form a different type of mental image of stimuli) and one control group (not asked to form an image) were presented spatial configuration patterns with different numbers of black dots. In both experiments, for the group that formed motor images with actual movement, the average score was higher for stimuli with a larger number of dots than for stimuli with fewer dots. Two groups, which formed dynamic visual images and motor images with no actual movement, respectively, showed similar limited effects. In Experiment 3, the five groups were presented two types of chromatic stimuli (colored panels and colored dots). Static visual images were effective for encoding the colored panels; however, static visual images and motor images with actual movement were effective for encoding the colored dots. These results suggest that motor and dynamic encodings facilitate memory for objects where spatial configuration is important for identification, while static visual images of the whole picture facilitate memory for objects where multiple colors are significant.

三个实验检验了有效识别空间和颜色信息的编码类型。在实验1和2中,四个实验组(每个实验组被要求形成不同类型的刺激心理图像)和一个对照组(不被要求形成图像)被呈现不同数量的黑点的空间构型模式。在这两个实验中,对于用实际运动形成运动图像的那一组来说,点多的刺激的平均得分要高于点少的刺激。两组分别形成动态视觉图像和没有实际运动的运动图像,显示出相似的有限效果。在实验3中,五组被试分别被给予两种类型的彩色刺激(彩色面板和彩色点)。静态视觉图像对彩色面板的编码是有效的;然而,静态视觉图像和实际运动的运动图像对彩色点的编码是有效的。这些结果表明,运动和动态编码有助于对空间结构对识别很重要的物体的记忆,而整体图像的静态视觉图像有助于对多种颜色重要的物体的记忆。
{"title":"[Effective encoding for the recognition of spatial configuration and color].","authors":"Taku Morimoto","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.15004","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Three experiments examined the types of encoding that were effective for the recognition of spatial and color information. In Experiments 1 and 2, four experimental groups (each asked to form a different type of mental image of stimuli) and one control group (not asked to form an image) were presented spatial configuration patterns with different numbers of black dots. In both experiments, for the group that formed motor images with actual movement, the average score was higher for stimuli with a larger number of dots than for stimuli with fewer dots. Two groups, which formed dynamic visual images and motor images with no actual movement, respectively, showed similar limited effects. In Experiment 3, the five groups were presented two types of chromatic stimuli (colored panels and colored dots). Static visual images were effective for encoding the colored panels; however, static visual images and motor images with actual movement were effective for encoding the colored dots. These results suggest that motor and dynamic encodings facilitate memory for objects where spatial configuration is important for identification, while static visual images of the whole picture facilitate memory for objects where multiple colors are significant.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"35989473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
[Intention for self-change across the life span: Focusing on concern about self-change]. [一生中自我改变的意向:关注自我改变]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-06-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.15201
Yuta Chishima

The purpose of the present study was to examine intention for self-change across the life span using measures of self-esteem, frequency of self-reflection, and concern about self-change. We hypothesized that: (a) Intention for self-change decreases with age because of increased self-esteem, decreased self-reflection and concern about self-change, and (b) Associations among self-esteem, frequency of self-reflection, and intention for self-change are mediated by concern about self-change. Participants (N = 997; age range, 15 to 69 yrs) completed an internet survey. ANOVA results suggested that intention for self-change, concern about self-change, and frequency of self-reflection decreased with age, and that self-esteem-scores increased with age. Simultaneous analysis of multiple age groups showed that for all groups of low self-esteem and frequent self-reflection promoted intention for self-change and that there were significant mediating effects for concern about self-change. Therefore, these findings supported out research hypotheses.

本研究的目的是通过自尊、自我反思的频率和对自我改变的关注来检验一生中自我改变的意图。我们假设:(a)随着年龄的增长,自我改变的意愿会减少,这是因为自尊的增加、自我反思的减少和对自我改变的关注;(b)自尊、自我反思的频率和自我改变的意愿之间的关联是由对自我改变的关注介导的。参与者(N = 997;年龄范围:15岁至69岁)完成了一项网络调查。方差分析结果显示,自我改变的意愿、自我改变的关注和自我反思的频率随年龄的增长而下降,自尊得分随年龄的增长而增加。多年龄组同时分析发现,低自尊和频繁的自我反思对所有组的自我改变意图都有促进作用,对自我改变的关注有显著的中介作用。因此,这些发现支持了我们的研究假设。
{"title":"[Intention for self-change across the life span: Focusing on concern about self-change].","authors":"Yuta Chishima","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.15201","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15201","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of the present study was to examine intention for self-change across the life span using measures of self-esteem, frequency of self-reflection, and concern about self-change. We hypothesized that: (a) Intention for self-change decreases with age because of increased self-esteem, decreased self-reflection and concern about self-change, and (b) Associations among self-esteem, frequency of self-reflection, and intention for self-change are mediated by concern about self-change. Participants (N = 997; age range, 15 to 69 yrs) completed an internet survey. ANOVA results suggested that intention for self-change, concern about self-change, and frequency of self-reflection decreased with age, and that self-esteem-scores increased with age. Simultaneous analysis of multiple age groups showed that for all groups of low self-esteem and frequent self-reflection promoted intention for self-change and that there were significant mediating effects for concern about self-change. Therefore, these findings supported out research hypotheses.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"34329156","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
[Effectiveness of an executive function task with a touch panel system for young children]. [幼儿触摸面板系统执行功能任务的有效性]。
Q4 Psychology Pub Date : 2016-06-01 DOI: 10.4992/jjpsy.87.15203
Yoko Sakata, Yusuke Moriguchi

The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task is a widely used measure for the development of executive function during early childhood. In this task, children are asked to sort cards according to one rule (e.g., color) during preswitch phases, after which they are asked to sort cards according to another rule (e.g.; shape) during the postswitch phases. A computer version of the DCCS was needed to standardize the test material, but a previous study showed that children showed more difficulty in a computer version with a mouse device than the standard card version. In this study, we assessed the effectiveness of a computer version with a touch panel and compared performance with the standard card version. Three- and 4-year-old children were given the card version and computer version of the DCCS tasks. The results revealed that children showed similar performance during the preswitch and postswitch phases both in the computer version and in the card version. The results suggest that both versions of the task assessed the same underlying cognitive processes.

维度变化卡片分类(DCCS)任务是儿童早期执行功能发展的一种广泛使用的测量方法。在这个任务中,孩子们被要求在预转换阶段根据一个规则(例如,颜色)对卡片进行分类,之后他们被要求根据另一个规则(例如;形状)在转换后阶段。为了使测试内容标准化,需要计算机版的DCCS,但之前的一项研究表明,孩子们在使用鼠标设备的计算机版中表现出比标准卡片版更大的难度。在这项研究中,我们评估了带有触摸屏的电脑版本的有效性,并将其与标准卡片版本的性能进行了比较。三岁和四岁的孩子分别得到了卡片版和电脑版的DCCS任务。结果显示,在电脑版本和卡片版本中,儿童在转换前和转换后阶段的表现相似。结果表明,这两个版本的任务评估的是相同的潜在认知过程。
{"title":"[Effectiveness of an executive function task with a touch panel system for young children].","authors":"Yoko Sakata,&nbsp;Yusuke Moriguchi","doi":"10.4992/jjpsy.87.15203","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15203","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task is a widely used measure for the development of executive function during early childhood. In this task, children are asked to sort cards according to one rule (e.g., color) during preswitch phases, after which they are asked to sort cards according to another rule (e.g.; shape) during the postswitch phases. A computer version of the DCCS was needed to standardize the test material, but a previous study showed that children showed more difficulty in a computer version with a mouse device than the standard card version. In this study, we assessed the effectiveness of a computer version with a touch panel and compared performance with the standard card version. Three- and 4-year-old children were given the card version and computer version of the DCCS tasks. The results revealed that children showed similar performance during the preswitch and postswitch phases both in the computer version and in the card version. The results suggest that both versions of the task assessed the same underlying cognitive processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":53680,"journal":{"name":"Shinrigaku Kenkyu","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.4992/jjpsy.87.15203","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"34329157","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Shinrigaku Kenkyu
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1