首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology最新文献

英文 中文
Avian navigation: the geomagnetic field provides compass cues but not a bicoordinate "map" plus a brief discussion of the alternative infrasound direction-finding hypothesis. 鸟类导航:地磁场提供指南针线索,但不提供双坐标 "地图",并简要讨论了另一种次声测向假说。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-18 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01627-9
Jonathan T Hagstrum

The geomagnetic field (GMF) is a worldwide source of compass cues used by animals and humans alike. The inclination of GMF flux lines also provides information on geomagnetic latitude. A long-disputed question, however, is whether horizontal gradients in GMF intensity, in combination with changes in inclination, provide bicoordinate "map" information. Multiple sources contribute to the total GMF, the largest of which is the core field. The ubiquitous crustal field is much less intense, but in both land and marine settings is strong enough at low altitudes (< 700 m; sea level) to mask the core field's weak N-S intensity gradient (~ 3-5 nT/km) over 10 s to 100 s of km. Non-orthogonal geomagnetic gradients, the lack of consistent E-W gradients, and the local masking of core-field intensity gradients by the crustal field, therefore, are grounds for rejection of the bicoordinate geomagnetic "map" hypothesis. In addition, the alternative infrasound direction-finding hypothesis is briefly reviewed. The GMF's diurnal variation has long been suggested as a possible Zeitgeber (timekeeper) for circadian rhythms and could explain the GMF's non-compass role in the avian navigational system. Requirements for detection of this weaker diurnal signal (~ 20-50 nT) might explain the magnetic alignment of resting and grazing animals.

地磁场(GMF)是全球范围内动物和人类使用的指南针线索来源。地磁场通量线的倾角也提供了地磁纬度的信息。然而,一个长期存在争议的问题是,地磁场强度的水平梯度与倾角的变化是否能提供双坐标 "地图 "信息。全球移动磁场有多种来源,其中最大的是地心磁场。无处不在的地壳场强度要低得多,但在陆地和海洋环境中,其强度在低海拔地区也足够大(例如,在海平面上)。
{"title":"Avian navigation: the geomagnetic field provides compass cues but not a bicoordinate \"map\" plus a brief discussion of the alternative infrasound direction-finding hypothesis.","authors":"Jonathan T Hagstrum","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01627-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01627-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The geomagnetic field (GMF) is a worldwide source of compass cues used by animals and humans alike. The inclination of GMF flux lines also provides information on geomagnetic latitude. A long-disputed question, however, is whether horizontal gradients in GMF intensity, in combination with changes in inclination, provide bicoordinate \"map\" information. Multiple sources contribute to the total GMF, the largest of which is the core field. The ubiquitous crustal field is much less intense, but in both land and marine settings is strong enough at low altitudes (< 700 m; sea level) to mask the core field's weak N-S intensity gradient (~ 3-5 nT/km) over 10 s to 100 s of km. Non-orthogonal geomagnetic gradients, the lack of consistent E-W gradients, and the local masking of core-field intensity gradients by the crustal field, therefore, are grounds for rejection of the bicoordinate geomagnetic \"map\" hypothesis. In addition, the alternative infrasound direction-finding hypothesis is briefly reviewed. The GMF's diurnal variation has long been suggested as a possible Zeitgeber (timekeeper) for circadian rhythms and could explain the GMF's non-compass role in the avian navigational system. Requirements for detection of this weaker diurnal signal (~ 20-50 nT) might explain the magnetic alignment of resting and grazing animals.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9673704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What does climbing mean exactly? Assessing spatiotemporal gait characteristics of inclined locomotion in parrots. 攀爬究竟意味着什么?评估鹦鹉倾斜运动的时空步态特征
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-04 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01630-0
Melody W Young, Clyde Webster, Daniel Tanis, Alissa F Schurr, Christopher S Hanna, Samantha K Lynch, Aleksandra S Ratkiewicz, Edwin Dickinson, Felix H Kong, Michael C Granatosky

At what inclination does climbing begin? In this paper, we investigate the transition from walking to climbing in two species of parrot (Agapornis roseicollis and Nymphicus hollandicus) that are known to incorporate both their tail and their craniocervical system into the gait cycle during vertical climbing. Locomotor behaviors ranging in inclination were observed at angles between 0° and 90° for A. roseicollis, and 45°-85° degrees for N. hollandicus. Use of the tail in both species was observed at 45° inclination, and was joined at higher inclinations (> 65°) by use of the craniocervical system. Additionally, as inclination approached (but remained below) 90°, locomotor speeds were reduced while gaits were characterized by higher duty factors and lower stride frequency. These gait changes are consistent with those thought to increase stability. At 90°, A. roseicollis significantly increased its stride length, resulting in higher overall locomotor speed. Collectively these data demonstrate that the transition between horizontal walking and vertical climbing is gradual, incrementally altering several components of gait as inclinations increase. Such data underscore the need for further investigation into how exactly "climbing" is defined and the specific locomotor characteristics that differentiate this behavior from level walking.

攀爬是从哪个倾角开始的?在本文中,我们研究了两种鹦鹉(Agapornis roseicollis和Nymphicus hollandicus)从行走到攀爬的过渡过程,已知这两种鹦鹉在垂直攀爬过程中将尾巴和颅颈系统纳入步态周期。在0°至90°的角度范围内,观察到A. roseicollis和N. hollandicus的运动行为倾角分别为45°至85°。两个物种在45°倾角时都能观察到尾部的使用,在更高倾角(> 65°)时,尾部的使用与颅颈系统的使用结合在一起。此外,当倾角接近(但仍低于)90°时,运动速度降低,同时步态具有较高的占空比和较低的步频。这些步态变化与那些被认为能提高稳定性的步态变化是一致的。在 90° 角时,A. roseicollis 明显增加了步长,从而提高了整体运动速度。这些数据共同表明,水平行走和垂直攀爬之间的过渡是渐进的,随着倾角的增加,步态的几个组成部分也会逐渐发生变化。这些数据突出表明,有必要进一步研究 "攀爬 "究竟是如何定义的,以及这种行为区别于水平行走的具体运动特征。
{"title":"What does climbing mean exactly? Assessing spatiotemporal gait characteristics of inclined locomotion in parrots.","authors":"Melody W Young, Clyde Webster, Daniel Tanis, Alissa F Schurr, Christopher S Hanna, Samantha K Lynch, Aleksandra S Ratkiewicz, Edwin Dickinson, Felix H Kong, Michael C Granatosky","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01630-0","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01630-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>At what inclination does climbing begin? In this paper, we investigate the transition from walking to climbing in two species of parrot (Agapornis roseicollis and Nymphicus hollandicus) that are known to incorporate both their tail and their craniocervical system into the gait cycle during vertical climbing. Locomotor behaviors ranging in inclination were observed at angles between 0° and 90° for A. roseicollis, and 45°-85° degrees for N. hollandicus. Use of the tail in both species was observed at 45° inclination, and was joined at higher inclinations (> 65°) by use of the craniocervical system. Additionally, as inclination approached (but remained below) 90°, locomotor speeds were reduced while gaits were characterized by higher duty factors and lower stride frequency. These gait changes are consistent with those thought to increase stability. At 90°, A. roseicollis significantly increased its stride length, resulting in higher overall locomotor speed. Collectively these data demonstrate that the transition between horizontal walking and vertical climbing is gradual, incrementally altering several components of gait as inclinations increase. Such data underscore the need for further investigation into how exactly \"climbing\" is defined and the specific locomotor characteristics that differentiate this behavior from level walking.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9411322","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Five-minute exposure to a novel appetitive food substance is sufficient time for a microRNA-dependent long-term memory to form. 暴露于新奇的食欲物质五分钟,就足以形成依赖于微RNA的长期记忆。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-29 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01650-w
Diana Kagan, Jasper Hollings, Anuradha Batabyal, Ken Lukowiak

The Garcia effect is a unique form of conditioned taste aversion which requires that a novel food stimulus be followed sometime later by a sickness state associated with the novel food stimulus. The long-lasting associative memory resulting from the Garcia effect ensures that organisms avoid toxic foods in their environment. Considering its ecological relevance, we sought to investigate whether a brief encounter (5 min) with a novel, appetitive food stimulus can cause a persisting long-term memory (LTM) to form that would in turn block the Garcia effect in Lymnaea stagnalis. Furthermore, we wanted to explore whether that persisting LTM could be modified by the alteration of microRNAs via an injection of poly-L-lysine (PLL), an inhibitor of Dicer-mediated microRNA biogenesis. The Garcia effect procedure involved two observations of feeding behavior in carrot separated by a heat stress (30 °C for 1 h). Exposing snails to carrot for 5 min caused a LTM to form and persist for 1 week, effectively preventing the Garcia effect in snails. In contrast, PLL injection following the 5-min carrot exposure impaired LTM formation, allowing the Garcia effect to occur. These results provide more insight into LTM formation and the Garcia effect, an important survival mechanism.

加西亚效应是一种独特的条件性味觉厌恶形式,它要求在受到新奇食物刺激后的一段时间内,出现与新奇食物刺激相关的疾病状态。加西亚效应产生的持久联想记忆确保生物避免食用环境中的有毒食物。考虑到与生态学的相关性,我们试图研究短暂接触(5 分钟)新奇、食欲旺盛的食物刺激是否会导致持久的长期记忆(LTM)形成,进而阻止滞游蛙的加西亚效应。此外,我们还想通过注射多聚-L-赖氨酸(PLL)(一种 Dicer 介导的 microRNA 生物生成抑制剂)来探索是否可以通过改变 microRNA 来改变持续的 LTM。加西亚效应程序包括两次胡萝卜摄食行为观察,中间间隔一次热应激(30 °C,1 小时)。将蜗牛暴露在胡萝卜中 5 分钟会导致 LTM 的形成并持续 1 周,从而有效地防止了蜗牛的加西亚效应。相反,在接触胡萝卜 5 分钟后注射 PLL 会阻碍 LTM 的形成,从而使加西亚效应发生。这些结果使人们对LTM的形成和加西亚效应这一重要的生存机制有了更深入的了解。
{"title":"Five-minute exposure to a novel appetitive food substance is sufficient time for a microRNA-dependent long-term memory to form.","authors":"Diana Kagan, Jasper Hollings, Anuradha Batabyal, Ken Lukowiak","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01650-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01650-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Garcia effect is a unique form of conditioned taste aversion which requires that a novel food stimulus be followed sometime later by a sickness state associated with the novel food stimulus. The long-lasting associative memory resulting from the Garcia effect ensures that organisms avoid toxic foods in their environment. Considering its ecological relevance, we sought to investigate whether a brief encounter (5 min) with a novel, appetitive food stimulus can cause a persisting long-term memory (LTM) to form that would in turn block the Garcia effect in Lymnaea stagnalis. Furthermore, we wanted to explore whether that persisting LTM could be modified by the alteration of microRNAs via an injection of poly-L-lysine (PLL), an inhibitor of Dicer-mediated microRNA biogenesis. The Garcia effect procedure involved two observations of feeding behavior in carrot separated by a heat stress (30 °C for 1 h). Exposing snails to carrot for 5 min caused a LTM to form and persist for 1 week, effectively preventing the Garcia effect in snails. In contrast, PLL injection following the 5-min carrot exposure impaired LTM formation, allowing the Garcia effect to occur. These results provide more insight into LTM formation and the Garcia effect, an important survival mechanism.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9693818","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Antlion larvae localize long distant preys by a mechanism based on time difference. 蚁狮幼虫通过一种基于时间差的机制来确定远距离猎物的位置。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-01 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01641-x
Vanessa Martinez, David Sillam-Dussès, Dušan Devetak, Vincent Lorent, Jan Podlesnik

Pit building antlions Euroleon nostras have been submitted to artificial cues in order to delineate their faculty to localize a prey. Series of propagating pulses in sand have been created from an extended source made of 10 piezoelectric transducers equally spaced on a line and located at a large distance from the pit. The envelope of each pulse encompasses six oscillations at a carrier frequency of 1250 Hz and up to eight oscillations at 1666 Hz. In one set of experiments, the first wave front is followed by similar wave fronts and the antlions respond to the cue by throwing sand in the opposite direction of the wave front propagation direction. In another set of experiments, the first wave front is randomly spatially structured while the propagation of the wave fronts inside the envelope of the pulse are not. In that case, the antlions respond less to the cue by throwing sand, and when they do, their sand throwing is more randomly distributed in direction. The finding shows that the localization of vibration signal by antlions are based on the equivalent for hearing animals of interaural time difference in which the onset has more significance than the interaural phase difference.

为了确定筑巢蚁定位猎物的能力,我们对筑巢蚁进行了人工提示。由 10 个压电换能器组成的扩展信号源在沙中产生了一系列传播脉冲,这些换能器的间距相等,并与蚁穴保持较大距离。每个脉冲的包络线包括载波频率为 1250 Hz 的六次振荡和高达 1666 Hz 的八次振荡。在一组实验中,第一个波阵面之后是类似的波阵面,蚁狮对提示做出反应,向波阵面传播方向的反方向扔沙子。在另一组实验中,第一个波阵面是随机的空间结构,而脉冲包络线内的波阵面传播则不是随机的空间结构。在这种情况下,蚁狮对抛沙提示的反应较小,而且当它们抛沙时,其抛沙方向的分布更加随机。这一发现表明,蚁狮对振动信号的定位是基于等效于听觉动物的耳间时差,其中起始时间比耳间相位差更重要。
{"title":"Antlion larvae localize long distant preys by a mechanism based on time difference.","authors":"Vanessa Martinez, David Sillam-Dussès, Dušan Devetak, Vincent Lorent, Jan Podlesnik","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01641-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01641-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pit building antlions Euroleon nostras have been submitted to artificial cues in order to delineate their faculty to localize a prey. Series of propagating pulses in sand have been created from an extended source made of 10 piezoelectric transducers equally spaced on a line and located at a large distance from the pit. The envelope of each pulse encompasses six oscillations at a carrier frequency of 1250 Hz and up to eight oscillations at 1666 Hz. In one set of experiments, the first wave front is followed by similar wave fronts and the antlions respond to the cue by throwing sand in the opposite direction of the wave front propagation direction. In another set of experiments, the first wave front is randomly spatially structured while the propagation of the wave fronts inside the envelope of the pulse are not. In that case, the antlions respond less to the cue by throwing sand, and when they do, their sand throwing is more randomly distributed in direction. The finding shows that the localization of vibration signal by antlions are based on the equivalent for hearing animals of interaural time difference in which the onset has more significance than the interaural phase difference.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9924818","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating the interactions between multiple memory stores in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis. 研究池塘蜗牛 Lymnaea stagnalis 中多种记忆存储之间的相互作用。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-03 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01649-3
Veronica Rivi, Anuradha Batabyal, Cristina Benatti, Johanna M C Blom, Fabio Tascedda, Ken Lukowiak

The pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis exhibits various forms of associative learning including (1) operant conditioning of aerial respiration where snails are trained not to open their pneumostome in a hypoxic pond water environment using a weak tactile stimulus to their pneumostome as they attempt to open it; and (2) a 24 h-lasting taste-specific learned avoidance known as the Garcia effect utilizing a lipopolysaccharide (LPS) injection just after snails eat a novel food substance (carrot). Typically, lab-inbred snails require two 0.5 h training sessions to form long-term memory (LTM) for operant conditioning of aerial respiration. However, some stressors (e.g., heat shock or predator scent) act as memory enhancers and thus a single 0.5 h training session is sufficient to enhance LTM formation lasting at least 24 h. Here, we found that snails forming a food-aversion LTM following Garcia-effect training exhibited enhanced LTM following operant condition of aerial respiration if trained in the presence of the food substance (carrot) they became averse to. Control experiments led us to conclude that carrot becomes a 'sickness' risk signal and acts as a stressor, sufficient to enhance LTM formation for another conditioning procedure.

池塘蜗牛(Lymnaea stagnalis)表现出多种形式的联想学习,包括:(1)空气呼吸的操作性条件反射,即在缺氧的池塘水环境中,当蜗牛试图打开气门时,对其气门进行微弱的触觉刺激,以训练蜗牛不打开气门;(2)持续 24 小时的味觉特异性习得性回避,即蜗牛刚吃下一种新的食物(胡萝卜)后,利用注射脂多糖(LPS)产生的加西亚效应。通常情况下,实验室饲养的蜗牛需要经过两次 0.5 小时的训练才能形成空中呼吸操作性条件反射的长期记忆(LTM)。然而,某些应激源(如热休克或捕食者气味)可以增强记忆,因此单次 0.5 小时的训练就足以增强至少 24 小时的长期记忆(LTM)形成。在这里,我们发现,如果在蜗牛变得厌恶的食物(胡萝卜)存在的情况下进行训练,那么经过加西亚效应训练形成食物厌恶长期记忆(LTM)的蜗牛在进行空中呼吸操作条件训练后,其长期记忆(LTM)会得到增强。对照实验使我们得出结论:胡萝卜成为一种 "疾病 "风险信号,并作为一种压力源,足以促进另一种条件程序的LTM形成。
{"title":"Investigating the interactions between multiple memory stores in the pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis.","authors":"Veronica Rivi, Anuradha Batabyal, Cristina Benatti, Johanna M C Blom, Fabio Tascedda, Ken Lukowiak","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01649-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01649-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The pond snail Lymnaea stagnalis exhibits various forms of associative learning including (1) operant conditioning of aerial respiration where snails are trained not to open their pneumostome in a hypoxic pond water environment using a weak tactile stimulus to their pneumostome as they attempt to open it; and (2) a 24 h-lasting taste-specific learned avoidance known as the Garcia effect utilizing a lipopolysaccharide (LPS) injection just after snails eat a novel food substance (carrot). Typically, lab-inbred snails require two 0.5 h training sessions to form long-term memory (LTM) for operant conditioning of aerial respiration. However, some stressors (e.g., heat shock or predator scent) act as memory enhancers and thus a single 0.5 h training session is sufficient to enhance LTM formation lasting at least 24 h. Here, we found that snails forming a food-aversion LTM following Garcia-effect training exhibited enhanced LTM following operant condition of aerial respiration if trained in the presence of the food substance (carrot) they became averse to. Control experiments led us to conclude that carrot becomes a 'sickness' risk signal and acts as a stressor, sufficient to enhance LTM formation for another conditioning procedure.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10099603","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Movement during the acquisition of a visual landmark may be necessary for rapid learning in ants. 蚂蚁在获取视觉地标的过程中的运动可能是快速学习的必要条件。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-28 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01652-8
Tomoko Sakiyama, Kenji Suda

We conducted laboratory experiments using Japanese carpenter ants (Camponotus japonicus) to investigate whether movement during visual learning can influence the learning performance of ant foragers. We performed three different experiments. In the first experiment, the ants could move freely in a straight maze during the visual learning. The ants in the experiments two and three were fixed to a certain position during the visual learning training. A distinct difference between these two experiments was that the ants in one experiment could perceive an approaching visual stimulus during the training, although they were fixed. After training phases, a Y-maze test was performed. One arm of the Y-maze had a visual stimulus presented to the ants during the training. We found that the ants in the first experiment showed rapid learning and correctly selected the landmark arm. However, the ants in the experiments two and three did not exhibit any preference for the chosen arm. Interestingly, we found differences in the time spent around a certain location in the Y-maze between the experiments two and three. These results suggest that movement during visual learning may influence the rapid learning of ant foragers.

我们利用日本木匠蚁(Camponotus japonicus)进行了实验室实验,研究视觉学习过程中的移动是否会影响蚂蚁觅食者的学习成绩。我们进行了三个不同的实验。在第一个实验中,蚂蚁在视觉学习过程中可以在直线迷宫中自由移动。实验二和实验三中的蚂蚁在视觉学习训练过程中被固定在某个位置上。这两个实验的一个明显区别是,一个实验中的蚂蚁虽然被固定,但在训练过程中可以感知到接近的视觉刺激。训练阶段结束后,进行了 Y 型迷宫测试。在训练过程中,Y 形迷宫的一只臂向蚂蚁展示了视觉刺激。我们发现,第一次实验中的蚂蚁学习速度很快,并能正确选择地标臂。然而,实验二和实验三中的蚂蚁并没有表现出对所选手臂的偏好。有趣的是,我们发现实验二和实验三的蚂蚁在 Y 型迷宫中围绕某一特定位置所花费的时间存在差异。这些结果表明,视觉学习过程中的运动可能会影响蚂蚁觅食者的快速学习。
{"title":"Movement during the acquisition of a visual landmark may be necessary for rapid learning in ants.","authors":"Tomoko Sakiyama, Kenji Suda","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01652-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01652-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We conducted laboratory experiments using Japanese carpenter ants (Camponotus japonicus) to investigate whether movement during visual learning can influence the learning performance of ant foragers. We performed three different experiments. In the first experiment, the ants could move freely in a straight maze during the visual learning. The ants in the experiments two and three were fixed to a certain position during the visual learning training. A distinct difference between these two experiments was that the ants in one experiment could perceive an approaching visual stimulus during the training, although they were fixed. After training phases, a Y-maze test was performed. One arm of the Y-maze had a visual stimulus presented to the ants during the training. We found that the ants in the first experiment showed rapid learning and correctly selected the landmark arm. However, the ants in the experiments two and three did not exhibit any preference for the chosen arm. Interestingly, we found differences in the time spent around a certain location in the Y-maze between the experiments two and three. These results suggest that movement during visual learning may influence the rapid learning of ant foragers.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9746583","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hearing in helmeted guineafowl (Numida meleagris): audiogram from 2 Hz to 10 kHz and localization acuity for brief noise bursts. 头盔鸠(Numida meleagris)的听力:2赫兹至10千赫兹的听力图和对短暂噪声脉冲的定位敏锐度。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-06 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01645-7
Henry E Heffner, Gimseong Koay, Rickye S Heffner

Behavioral hearing thresholds and noise localization acuity were determined using a conditioned avoidance/suppression procedure for three Helmeted guineafowl (Numida meleagris). The guineafowl responded to frequencies as low as 2 Hz at 82.5 dB SPL, and as high as 8 kHz at 84.5 dB SPL. At a level of 60 dB SPL, their hearing range spanned 8.12 octaves (24.6 Hz-6.86 kHz). Like most birds, they do not hear sounds above 8 kHz. However, the guineafowl demonstrated good low-frequency hearing (frequencies below 32 Hz), showing thresholds that are more sensitive than both the peafowl and pigeon, both of which hear infrasound. It thus appears that infrasound perception may be more common than previously thought and may have implications for species that inhabit areas with wind energy facilities. The guineafowls' minimum audible angle for a 100-ms broadband noise burst was 13.8 °, at the median for birds and near the mean for mammals. Unlike in mammals, the small sample of bird species and limited representation of lifestyles do not yet allow for meaningful interpretations of the selective pressures or mechanisms that underlie their abilities to locate sound sources.

使用条件性回避/抑制程序测定了三只盔斑鸠(Numida meleagris)的行为听阈和噪声定位敏锐度。红腹角雉对低至 82.5 dB SPL 的 2 Hz 频率和高至 84.5 dB SPL 的 8 kHz 频率均有反应。当声压级为 60 dB SPL 时,它们的听力范围为 8.12 个八度音程(24.6 Hz-6.86 kHz)。与大多数鸟类一样,它们听不到 8 千赫以上的声音。不过,豚鼠的低频听力(频率低于 32 赫兹)很好,其阈值比能听到次声波的鹦鹉和鸽子都要灵敏。由此看来,对次声波的感知可能比以前认为的更为普遍,这可能会对栖息在风能设施地区的物种产生影响。豚鼠对 100 毫秒宽带噪声脉冲的最小可听角度为 13.8°,处于鸟类的中位数,接近哺乳动物的平均值。与哺乳动物不同,鸟类物种样本较少,生活方式的代表性有限,因此还无法对鸟类定位声源能力的选择性压力或机制做出有意义的解释。
{"title":"Hearing in helmeted guineafowl (Numida meleagris): audiogram from 2 Hz to 10 kHz and localization acuity for brief noise bursts.","authors":"Henry E Heffner, Gimseong Koay, Rickye S Heffner","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01645-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01645-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Behavioral hearing thresholds and noise localization acuity were determined using a conditioned avoidance/suppression procedure for three Helmeted guineafowl (Numida meleagris). The guineafowl responded to frequencies as low as 2 Hz at 82.5 dB SPL, and as high as 8 kHz at 84.5 dB SPL. At a level of 60 dB SPL, their hearing range spanned 8.12 octaves (24.6 Hz-6.86 kHz). Like most birds, they do not hear sounds above 8 kHz. However, the guineafowl demonstrated good low-frequency hearing (frequencies below 32 Hz), showing thresholds that are more sensitive than both the peafowl and pigeon, both of which hear infrasound. It thus appears that infrasound perception may be more common than previously thought and may have implications for species that inhabit areas with wind energy facilities. The guineafowls' minimum audible angle for a 100-ms broadband noise burst was 13.8 °, at the median for birds and near the mean for mammals. Unlike in mammals, the small sample of bird species and limited representation of lifestyles do not yet allow for meaningful interpretations of the selective pressures or mechanisms that underlie their abilities to locate sound sources.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9638360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding daily rhythms in weakly electric fish: the role of melatonin on the electric behavior of Brachyhypopomus gauderio. 了解弱电鱼类的日节律:褪黑激素对 Brachyhypopomus gauderio 电行为的作用。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01626-w
Juan I Vazquez, Valentina Gascue, Laura Quintana, Adriana Migliaro

Living organisms display molecular, physiological and behavioral rhythms synchronized with natural environmental cycles. Understanding the interaction between environment, physiology and behavior requires taking into account the complexity of natural habitats and the diversity of behavioral and physiological adaptations. Brachyhypopomus gauderio is characterized by the emission of electric organ discharges (EOD), with a very stable rate modulated by social and environmental cues. The nocturnal arousal in B. gauderio coincides with a melatonin-dependent EOD rate increase. Here, we first show a daily cycle in both the EOD basal rate (EOD-BR) and EOD-BR variability of B. gauderio in nature. We approached the understanding of the role of melatonin in this natural behavior through both behavioral pharmacology and in vitro assays. We report, for the first time in gymnotiformes, a direct effect of melatonin on the pacemaker nucleus (PN) in in vitro preparation. Melatonin treatment lowered EOD-BR in freely moving fish and PN basal rate, while increasing the variability of both. These results show that melatonin plays a key role in modulating the electric behavior of B. gauderio through its effect on rate and variability, both of which must be under a tight temporal regulation to prepare the animal for the challenging nocturnal environment.

生物体的分子、生理和行为节律与自然环境周期同步。要理解环境、生理和行为之间的相互作用,就必须考虑到自然栖息地的复杂性以及行为和生理适应的多样性。Brachyhypopomus gauderio 的特点是发射电器官放电(EOD),其速率非常稳定,受社会和环境线索的调节。B. gauderio的夜间唤醒与依赖褪黑激素的电器官放电率增加相吻合。在这里,我们首次展示了自然界中 B. gauderio 的 EOD 基础率(EOD-BR)和 EOD-BR 变化的日周期。我们通过行为药理学和体外实验来了解褪黑激素在这种自然行为中的作用。我们首次报告了褪黑激素对体外制备的起搏器核(PN)的直接影响。褪黑激素处理降低了自由活动鱼类的EOD-BR和PN基础率,同时增加了两者的变异性。这些结果表明,褪黑激素通过对速率和变异性的影响,在调节高德鱼的电行为中发挥着关键作用。
{"title":"Understanding daily rhythms in weakly electric fish: the role of melatonin on the electric behavior of Brachyhypopomus gauderio.","authors":"Juan I Vazquez, Valentina Gascue, Laura Quintana, Adriana Migliaro","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01626-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01626-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Living organisms display molecular, physiological and behavioral rhythms synchronized with natural environmental cycles. Understanding the interaction between environment, physiology and behavior requires taking into account the complexity of natural habitats and the diversity of behavioral and physiological adaptations. Brachyhypopomus gauderio is characterized by the emission of electric organ discharges (EOD), with a very stable rate modulated by social and environmental cues. The nocturnal arousal in B. gauderio coincides with a melatonin-dependent EOD rate increase. Here, we first show a daily cycle in both the EOD basal rate (EOD-BR) and EOD-BR variability of B. gauderio in nature. We approached the understanding of the role of melatonin in this natural behavior through both behavioral pharmacology and in vitro assays. We report, for the first time in gymnotiformes, a direct effect of melatonin on the pacemaker nucleus (PN) in in vitro preparation. Melatonin treatment lowered EOD-BR in freely moving fish and PN basal rate, while increasing the variability of both. These results show that melatonin plays a key role in modulating the electric behavior of B. gauderio through its effect on rate and variability, both of which must be under a tight temporal regulation to prepare the animal for the challenging nocturnal environment.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9593970","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Learning the native pond odor as one of the mechanisms of olfactory orientation in juvenile smooth newt Lissotriton vulgaris. 学习原生池塘气味是幼滑蝾螈的嗅觉定向机制之一。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-02 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01640-y
Yaroslav Aleksandrovich Vyatkin, Vladimir Vladimirovich Shakhparonov

Olfaction is an important mechanism of orientation in amphibians toward the breeding site. It is known that anurans can memorize the odor of the native pond during larval development and prefer this odor prior to the beginning of dispersion. However, such a mechanism in urodeles has not been studied yet. We conducted experiments on recognition of the odor of a native water body in juveniles of the smooth newt Lissotriton vulgaris. One group of larvae were reared in pure water (control), the other group in water with morpholine (10-7 mol/L). A few days after metamorphosis, the newts were tested under paired-choice conditions in a T-maze. A total of 73 newts from the experimental group and 47 newts from the control group were tested. The results of the experiment show that the newts in the experimental group preferred the morpholine solution, whereas the individuals of the control group made the choice randomly. We conclude that newts can learn the odor of the environment in which they developed and use this memory for orientation in later stages of life.

嗅觉是两栖动物向繁殖地定向的重要机制。众所周知,无尾两栖动物能在幼虫发育过程中记住原生池塘的气味,并在开始散布之前偏好这种气味。然而,这种机制在urodeles中还没有被研究过。我们进行了平滑蝾螈幼体识别原生水体气味的实验。一组幼体在纯水(对照组)中饲养,另一组在含有吗啉(10-7 mol/L)的水中饲养。蝾螈变态几天后,在 T 型迷宫中进行配对选择测试。共有 73 只实验组蝾螈和 47 只对照组蝾螈接受了测试。实验结果表明,实验组的蝾螈更喜欢吗啉溶液,而对照组的个体则随机做出选择。我们的结论是,蝾螈可以学习其生长环境的气味,并在以后的生活中利用这种记忆进行定向。
{"title":"Learning the native pond odor as one of the mechanisms of olfactory orientation in juvenile smooth newt Lissotriton vulgaris.","authors":"Yaroslav Aleksandrovich Vyatkin, Vladimir Vladimirovich Shakhparonov","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01640-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01640-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Olfaction is an important mechanism of orientation in amphibians toward the breeding site. It is known that anurans can memorize the odor of the native pond during larval development and prefer this odor prior to the beginning of dispersion. However, such a mechanism in urodeles has not been studied yet. We conducted experiments on recognition of the odor of a native water body in juveniles of the smooth newt Lissotriton vulgaris. One group of larvae were reared in pure water (control), the other group in water with morpholine (10<sup>-7</sup> mol/L). A few days after metamorphosis, the newts were tested under paired-choice conditions in a T-maze. A total of 73 newts from the experimental group and 47 newts from the control group were tested. The results of the experiment show that the newts in the experimental group preferred the morpholine solution, whereas the individuals of the control group made the choice randomly. We conclude that newts can learn the odor of the environment in which they developed and use this memory for orientation in later stages of life.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9917464","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chromatic discrimination in fixed saturation levels from tufted capuchin monkeys with different color vision genotypes. 具有不同色觉基因型的簇毛卷尾猴在固定饱和度水平下的色觉分辨能力。
IF 2.1 4区 心理学 Q1 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-06-02 DOI: 10.1007/s00359-023-01644-8
Luiza Karina Gonçalves Meireles, Leticia Miquilini, Felipe André Costa Brito, Anderson Raiol Rodrigues, Leonardo Dutra Henriques, Einat Hauzman, Daniela Maria Oliveira Bonci, Marcelo Fernandes Costa, Olavo de Faria Galvão, Dora Fix Ventura, Paulo Roney Kilpp Goulart, Givago Silva Souza

Recent research has proposed new approaches to investigate color vision in Old World Monkeys by measuring suprathreshold chromatic discrimination. In this study, we aimed to extend this approach to New World Monkeys with different color vision genotypes by examining their performance in chromatic discrimination tasks along different fixed chromatic saturation axes. Four tufted capuchin monkeys were included in the study, and their color vision genotypes were one classical protanope, one classical deuteranope, one non-classical protanope, and a normal trichromat. During the experiments, the monkeys were required to perform a chromatic discrimination task using pseudoisochromatic stimuli with varying target saturations of 0.06, 0.04, 0.03, and 0.02 u'v' units. The number of errors made by the monkeys along different chromatic axes was recorded, and their performance was quantified using the binomial probability of their hits during the tests. Our results showed that dichromatic monkeys made more errors near the color confusion lines associated with their specific color vision genotypes, while the trichromatic monkey did not demonstrate any systematic errors. At high chromatic saturation, the trichromatic monkey had significant hits in the chromatic axes around the 180° chromatic axis, whereas the dichromatic monkeys had errors in colors around the color confusion lines. At lower saturation, the performance of the dichromatic monkeys became more challenging to differentiate among the three types, but it was still distinct from that of the trichromatic monkey. In conclusion, our findings suggest that high saturation conditions can be used to identify the color vision dichromatic phenotype of capuchin monkeys, while low chromatic saturation conditions enable the distinction between trichromats and dichromats. These results extend the understanding of color vision in New World Monkeys and highlight the usefulness of suprathreshold chromatic discrimination measures in exploring color vision in non-human primates.

最近的研究提出了新的方法,通过测量阈上色度分辨来研究旧世界猴的色觉。在这项研究中,我们旨在将这种方法推广到具有不同色觉基因型的新世界猴身上,沿着不同的固定色饱和度轴考察它们在色辨别任务中的表现。研究中包括四只簇毛猴,它们的色觉基因型分别是经典原色猴、经典去色猴、非经典原色猴和正常三色猴。在实验过程中,猴子需要使用目标饱和度分别为 0.06、0.04、0.03 和 0.02 u'v' 单位的假异色刺激物来完成色觉辨别任务。我们记录了猴子在不同色度轴上出错的次数,并用它们在测试中命中的二项式概率来量化它们的表现。结果表明,二色猴在与其特定色觉基因型相关的颜色混淆线附近出错较多,而三色猴没有表现出任何系统性错误。在高色饱和度时,三色猴在 180° 色轴附近的色轴上有明显的命中率,而二色猴在颜色混淆线附近的颜色上有错误。饱和度较低时,二色猴的表现更难区分三种类型,但仍与三色猴的表现截然不同。总之,我们的研究结果表明,高饱和度条件可用于识别卷尾猴的色觉二色性表型,而低色度饱和度条件则可区分三色性和二色性。这些结果拓展了人们对新世界猴色觉的认识,并强调了阈上色度分辨测量在探索非人灵长类动物色觉方面的实用性。
{"title":"Chromatic discrimination in fixed saturation levels from tufted capuchin monkeys with different color vision genotypes.","authors":"Luiza Karina Gonçalves Meireles, Leticia Miquilini, Felipe André Costa Brito, Anderson Raiol Rodrigues, Leonardo Dutra Henriques, Einat Hauzman, Daniela Maria Oliveira Bonci, Marcelo Fernandes Costa, Olavo de Faria Galvão, Dora Fix Ventura, Paulo Roney Kilpp Goulart, Givago Silva Souza","doi":"10.1007/s00359-023-01644-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s00359-023-01644-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent research has proposed new approaches to investigate color vision in Old World Monkeys by measuring suprathreshold chromatic discrimination. In this study, we aimed to extend this approach to New World Monkeys with different color vision genotypes by examining their performance in chromatic discrimination tasks along different fixed chromatic saturation axes. Four tufted capuchin monkeys were included in the study, and their color vision genotypes were one classical protanope, one classical deuteranope, one non-classical protanope, and a normal trichromat. During the experiments, the monkeys were required to perform a chromatic discrimination task using pseudoisochromatic stimuli with varying target saturations of 0.06, 0.04, 0.03, and 0.02 u'v' units. The number of errors made by the monkeys along different chromatic axes was recorded, and their performance was quantified using the binomial probability of their hits during the tests. Our results showed that dichromatic monkeys made more errors near the color confusion lines associated with their specific color vision genotypes, while the trichromatic monkey did not demonstrate any systematic errors. At high chromatic saturation, the trichromatic monkey had significant hits in the chromatic axes around the 180° chromatic axis, whereas the dichromatic monkeys had errors in colors around the color confusion lines. At lower saturation, the performance of the dichromatic monkeys became more challenging to differentiate among the three types, but it was still distinct from that of the trichromatic monkey. In conclusion, our findings suggest that high saturation conditions can be used to identify the color vision dichromatic phenotype of capuchin monkeys, while low chromatic saturation conditions enable the distinction between trichromats and dichromats. These results extend the understanding of color vision in New World Monkeys and highlight the usefulness of suprathreshold chromatic discrimination measures in exploring color vision in non-human primates.</p>","PeriodicalId":54862,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9937241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Comparative Physiology A-Neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1