首页 > 最新文献

Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology最新文献

英文 中文
Number of Close Kin but Not Gendered Fitness Interests Shapes Sex Role Attitudes 近亲但不分性别的健身兴趣塑造性角色态度
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2023-01-09 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00207-6
Khandis R. Blake, Alsa Wu, Hugh McGovern, Robert C. Brooks

Objective

Gendered Fitness Interests (GFI) Theory posits that the sex of one’s close kin is a source of variation in socio-political attitudes, especially those concerning sex roles. Drawing on the evolutionary theory of inclusive fitness gained through relatives, it suggests that individuals with greater expected future fitness returns from females will hold more liberal attitudes than those expecting greater future fitness returns from males.

Methods

We examine the effects of various GFI metrics on 11 sex role attitudes across respondents from 42 countries. GFI metrics were the number of kin of each sex, with these same effects discounted by relatedness, residual reproductive value, and both relatedness and residual reproductive value combined. We also examine differences in attitude prediction when examining effects among descendant-only versus all kin, and accounting for emotional closeness and proximity between kin.

Results

Accounting for relatedness and residual reproductive value improved attitude prediction more so than just number of kin. In direct contrast to previous work, individuals with female-biased future fitness interests demonstrated more conservative sex role attitudes. Attitudes did not correlate with an individual’s relative balance of fitness interests from males versus females. Effects were stronger when limiting kin to descendants only (but still not supportive of GFI Theory). Discounting by emotional closeness and proximity did not substantially alter the results, and there was little evidence of effect variation among world regions.

Conclusion

Overall, GFI Theory did not receive convincing support from our findings. We recommend more extensive modelling and empirical testing of GFI theory in order to understand the disparities in support for GFI between studies.

客观性别适合兴趣理论认为,近亲的性别是社会政治态度变化的来源,尤其是与性别角色有关的态度。根据通过亲属获得的包容性健康的进化理论,它表明,与那些期望男性未来健康回报更大的人相比,女性未来健康回报预期更大的个人将持有更自由的态度。方法我们调查了来自42个国家的受访者的各种GFI指标对11种性别角色态度的影响。GFI指标是每个性别的亲属数量,这些相同的影响通过相关度、剩余生殖价值以及相关度和剩余生殖价值的总和来折现。当我们考察仅后代与所有亲属之间的影响,并考虑亲属之间的情感亲密度和接近度时,我们还考察了态度预测的差异。结果考虑亲属关系和剩余生殖价值比仅亲属数量更能改善态度预测。与之前的工作直接相反,对女性未来健身兴趣有偏见的个体表现出更保守的性别角色态度。态度与男性与女性健身兴趣的相对平衡无关。当将亲属仅限于后代时,效果更强(但仍然不支持GFI理论)。通过情感亲密度和接近度进行贴现并没有实质性地改变结果,而且几乎没有证据表明世界各地区之间的影响存在差异。结论总的来说,GFI理论并没有从我们的研究结果中得到令人信服的支持。我们建议对GFI理论进行更广泛的建模和实证检验,以了解研究之间支持GFI的差异。
{"title":"Number of Close Kin but Not Gendered Fitness Interests Shapes Sex Role Attitudes","authors":"Khandis R. Blake,&nbsp;Alsa Wu,&nbsp;Hugh McGovern,&nbsp;Robert C. Brooks","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00207-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00207-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Gendered Fitness Interests (GFI) Theory posits that the sex of one’s close kin is a source of variation in socio-political attitudes, especially those concerning sex roles. Drawing on the evolutionary theory of inclusive fitness gained through relatives, it suggests that individuals with greater expected future fitness returns from females will hold more liberal attitudes than those expecting greater future fitness returns from males.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>We examine the effects of various GFI metrics on 11 sex role attitudes across respondents from 42 countries. GFI metrics were the number of kin of each sex, with these same effects discounted by relatedness, residual reproductive value, and both relatedness and residual reproductive value combined. We also examine differences in attitude prediction when examining effects among descendant-only versus all kin, and accounting for emotional closeness and proximity between kin.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Accounting for relatedness and residual reproductive value improved attitude prediction more so than just number of kin. In direct contrast to previous work, individuals with female-biased future fitness interests demonstrated more conservative sex role attitudes. Attitudes did not correlate with an individual’s relative balance of fitness interests from males versus females. Effects were stronger when limiting kin to descendants only (but still not supportive of GFI Theory). Discounting by emotional closeness and proximity did not substantially alter the results, and there was little evidence of effect variation among world regions.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Overall, GFI Theory did not receive convincing support from our findings. We recommend more extensive modelling and empirical testing of GFI theory in order to understand the disparities in support for GFI between studies.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"9 1","pages":"37 - 53"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2023-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00207-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42203769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Is Pathogen Disgust Increased on Days of the Menstrual Cycle when Progesterone is High? Evidence from a Between-Subjects Study Using Estimated Progesterone Levels 当孕酮水平高时,对病原体的厌恶感是否会增加?使用估计孕酮水平的受试者间研究证据
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-12-27 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00208-5
Yasaman Rafiee, Benedict C. Jones, Victor Shiramizu

Objective

The Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis proposes that women will show increased pathogen disgust at points in the menstrual cycle when progesterone is high, compensating for the immunosuppressive effects of progesterone. However, evidence for the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis from studies that used longitudinal designs to investigate whether pathogen disgust tracks changes in progesterone is mixed. It was recently proposed that longitudinal designs may be poorly suited to testing the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis because carry-over effects when women are tested repeatedly in within-subject designs might obscure the effects of progesterone and pathogen disgust. Consequently, we used a between-subjects design to test for a positive relationship between scores on the pathogen disgust subscale of the Three Domain Disgust Scale and progesterone levels imputed from menstrual cycle data using actuarial tables.

Methods

We employed a between-subject design on N = 1346 women using the Three Domains of Disgust Scale (TDDS).

Results

We found no evidence for a positive effect of progesterone on pathogen disgust, suggesting that null results for the hypothesized relationship between progesterone and pathogen disgust are not limited to studies using designs that may be subject to carry-over effects caused by repeated testing.

Conclusion

Our results add to a growing body of research that does not support the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis.

目的补偿性预防假说提出,女性在月经周期中孕酮水平高时,会表现出对病原体的厌恶感增加,从而补偿孕酮的免疫抑制作用。然而,使用纵向设计来调查病原体厌恶是否跟踪孕酮变化的研究中,补偿预防假说的证据是混合的。最近有人提出,纵向设计可能不适合测试补偿预防假说,因为在受试者内部设计中反复测试女性时的遗留效应可能会掩盖孕酮和病原体厌恶的影响。因此,我们使用受试者之间的设计来测试三域恶心量表的病原体厌恶分量表得分与使用精算表从月经周期数据估算的孕酮水平之间的正相关关系。方法采用N = 1346名女性使用三个厌恶领域量表(TDDS)。结果我们没有发现孕酮对病原体厌恶有积极影响的证据,这表明孕酮和病原体厌恶之间假设关系的无效结果并不局限于使用可能会因重复测试而产生结转影响的设计的研究。结论我们的研究结果增加了越来越多的不支持补偿预防假说的研究。
{"title":"Is Pathogen Disgust Increased on Days of the Menstrual Cycle when Progesterone is High? Evidence from a Between-Subjects Study Using Estimated Progesterone Levels","authors":"Yasaman Rafiee,&nbsp;Benedict C. Jones,&nbsp;Victor Shiramizu","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00208-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00208-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>The Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis proposes that women will show increased pathogen disgust at points in the menstrual cycle when progesterone is high, compensating for the immunosuppressive effects of progesterone. However, evidence for the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis from studies that used longitudinal designs to investigate whether pathogen disgust tracks changes in progesterone is mixed. It was recently proposed that longitudinal designs may be poorly suited to testing the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis because carry-over effects when women are tested repeatedly in within-subject designs might obscure the effects of progesterone and pathogen disgust. Consequently, we used a between-subjects design to test for a positive relationship between scores on the pathogen disgust subscale of the Three Domain Disgust Scale and progesterone levels imputed from menstrual cycle data using actuarial tables.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>We employed a between-subject design on <i>N = 1346</i> women using the Three Domains of Disgust Scale (TDDS).</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We found no evidence for a positive effect of progesterone on pathogen disgust, suggesting that null results for the hypothesized relationship between progesterone and pathogen disgust are not limited to studies using designs that may be subject to carry-over effects caused by repeated testing.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Our results add to a growing body of research that does not support the Compensatory Prophylaxis Hypothesis.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"9 1","pages":"26 - 36"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-12-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00208-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48926058","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Rank Reversal Aversion and Fairness in Hierarchies 等级反转厌恶与等级公平
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-10-28 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00206-7
Alexandre Foncelle, Elodie Barat, Jean-Claude Dreher, Jean-Baptiste Van der Henst

Objectives

Despite the aversion to inequality in humans, social hierarchies are a fundamental feature of their social life. Several mechanisms help explain the prevalence of hierarchies over egalitarianism. Recent work has suggested that while people tend to reduce resource inequalities when given the opportunity, they are reluctant to do so when it results in a reversal of social ranks (Xie et al., 2017). In this study, we explore how the way in which hierarchies are established influences this mechanism. We propose that aversion to rank reversal depends on whether rank asymmetry is fair or unfair.

Methods

In an online study, participants read 12 vignettes depicting six hypothetical hierarchies that varied in fairness. In each vignette, one individual was endowed with more resources than another individual, and participants could reduce that inequality by transferring resources from the higher-ranked individual to the lower-ranked one. In half of the vignettes, reducing the inequality led to a reversal of ranks, while in the other half it did not.

Results

We observed that participants were more likely to reverse ranks and reduce inequality when the hierarchy was perceived as unfair.

Conclusion

Overall, our results suggest that considerations of fairness guide participants’ in their decision to reverse ranks.

目的尽管人类厌恶不平等,但社会等级制度是他们社会生活的一个基本特征。一些机制有助于解释等级制度相对于平等主义的普遍性。最近的研究表明,尽管人们在有机会时倾向于减少资源不平等,但当这导致社会地位逆转时,他们不愿意这样做(Xie et al.,2017)。在这项研究中,我们探讨了等级制度的建立方式如何影响这一机制。我们提出,对等级反转的厌恶取决于等级不对称是公平的还是不公平的。方法在一项在线研究中,参与者阅读了12个小插曲,描绘了六个公平性不同的假设等级。在每个小插曲中,一个人比另一个人拥有更多的资源,参与者可以通过将资源从排名较高的个人转移到排名较低的个人来减少这种不平等。在一半的小插曲中,减少不平等导致了排名的逆转,而在另一半则没有。结果我们观察到,当等级制度被认为不公平时,参与者更有可能颠倒排名,减少不平等。结论总体而言,我们的研究结果表明,公平的考虑指导了参与者的排名逆转决定。
{"title":"Rank Reversal Aversion and Fairness in Hierarchies","authors":"Alexandre Foncelle,&nbsp;Elodie Barat,&nbsp;Jean-Claude Dreher,&nbsp;Jean-Baptiste Van der Henst","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00206-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00206-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>Despite the aversion to inequality in humans, social hierarchies are a fundamental feature of their social life. Several mechanisms help explain the prevalence of hierarchies over egalitarianism. Recent work has suggested that while people tend to reduce resource inequalities when given the opportunity, they are reluctant to do so when it results in a reversal of social ranks (Xie et al., 2017). In this study, we explore how the way in which hierarchies are established influences this mechanism. We propose that aversion to rank reversal depends on whether rank asymmetry is fair or unfair.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>In an online study, participants read 12 vignettes depicting six hypothetical hierarchies that varied in fairness. In each vignette, one individual was endowed with more resources than another individual, and participants could reduce that inequality by transferring resources from the higher-ranked individual to the lower-ranked one. In half of the vignettes, reducing the inequality led to a reversal of ranks, while in the other half it did not.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We observed that participants were more likely to reverse ranks and reduce inequality when the hierarchy was perceived as unfair.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Overall, our results suggest that considerations of fairness guide participants’ in their decision to reverse ranks.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"520 - 537"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-10-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43529991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Does Men’s Facial Sexual Dimorphism Affect Male Observers’ Selective Attention? 男性面部性别差异是否影响男性观察者的选择性注意?
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-09-08 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00205-8
Graham Albert, Erika Wells, Steven Arnocky, Chang Hong Liu, Jessica K. Hlay, Carolyn R. Hodges-Simeon

Facial sexual dimorphism affects observers’ physical dominance ratings. Here, we test whether such perceived dominance influences selective attention. To minimize demand characteristics, we examined whether task-irrelevant masculinized men’s faces would show an attentional bias in several experimental paradigms. Experiment 1 employed a Posner Cueing Paradigm in which participants classified shapes after a masculinized or feminized man’s face was presented. We could not find a difference in participants’ classification speeds when either feminized or masculinized face cued target position. Experiment 2 employed a Flanker Task in which participants judged letter orientation, while ignoring flanking faces. There was no observed difference in participants’ reaction time (RT) when masculinized faces flanked the target. Experiment 3 employed a Dot Probe Task, where participants were presented with a masculinized face and a feminized face to the left and right of center screen, and a target shape was presented in the location of one face. Participants’ task was to classify shape orientation. We observe a small effect of facial sexual dimorphism on participants’ classification speed. In Experiment 4, we primed participants with images meant to induce fear or arousal before each trial of a Dot Probe Task. Following the presentation of a fear inducing picture, participants RT to classify shapes when a masculinized face cued target position did not differ from when a feminized face cued target position. The two different presentation times did not create different patterns of results, indicating that masculinized faces did not induce either a cueing or inhibitory affect. Overall, we failed to support the hypothesis that people selectively attend to masculinized faces when they are presented as irrelevant information.

面部两性异形影响观察者的身体优势评级。在这里,我们测试这种被感知的支配地位是否会影响选择性注意力。为了最大限度地减少需求特征,我们在几个实验范式中研究了与任务无关的男性化男性的脸是否会表现出注意力偏差。实验1采用了Posner线索范式,在该范式中,参与者在呈现男性化或女性化的男性面部后对形状进行分类。当女性化或男性化的面部暗示目标位置时,我们没有发现参与者的分类速度有差异。实验2采用了侧脸任务,参与者判断字母的方向,而忽略侧脸。当男性化的面孔位于目标两侧时,参与者的反应时间(RT)没有观察到差异。实验3采用点探针任务,在中心屏幕的左右两侧向参与者呈现男性化的脸和女性化的脸,并在一张脸的位置呈现目标形状。参与者的任务是对形状方向进行分类。我们观察到面部两性异形对参与者分类速度的影响很小。在实验4中,我们在每次测试点探针任务之前,为参与者准备了旨在诱导恐惧或觉醒的图像。在呈现一张引发恐惧的图片后,当男性化的面部提示的目标位置与女性化的面部暗示的目标位置没有差异时,参与者RT对形状进行分类。两种不同的呈现时间并没有产生不同的结果模式,这表明男性化的面孔既没有引起提示作用,也没有引起抑制作用。总的来说,我们未能支持这样一种假设,即当人们将男性化的面孔作为无关信息呈现时,他们会选择性地关注这些面孔。
{"title":"Does Men’s Facial Sexual Dimorphism Affect Male Observers’ Selective Attention?","authors":"Graham Albert,&nbsp;Erika Wells,&nbsp;Steven Arnocky,&nbsp;Chang Hong Liu,&nbsp;Jessica K. Hlay,&nbsp;Carolyn R. Hodges-Simeon","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00205-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00205-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Facial sexual dimorphism affects observers’ physical dominance ratings. Here, we test whether such perceived dominance influences selective attention. To minimize demand characteristics, we examined whether task-irrelevant masculinized men’s faces would show an attentional bias in several experimental paradigms. Experiment 1 employed a Posner Cueing Paradigm in which participants classified shapes after a masculinized or feminized man’s face was presented. We could not find a difference in participants’ classification speeds when either feminized or masculinized face cued target position. Experiment 2 employed a Flanker Task in which participants judged letter orientation, while ignoring flanking faces. There was no observed difference in participants’ reaction time (RT) when masculinized faces flanked the target. Experiment 3 employed a Dot Probe Task, where participants were presented with a masculinized face and a feminized face to the left and right of center screen, and a target shape was presented in the location of one face. Participants’ task was to classify shape orientation. We observe a small effect of facial sexual dimorphism on participants’ classification speed. In Experiment 4, we primed participants with images meant to induce fear or arousal before each trial of a Dot Probe Task. Following the presentation of a fear inducing picture, participants RT to classify shapes when a masculinized face cued target position did not differ from when a feminized face cued target position. The two different presentation times did not create different patterns of results, indicating that masculinized faces did not induce either a cueing or inhibitory affect. Overall, we failed to support the hypothesis that people selectively attend to masculinized faces when they are presented as irrelevant information.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"9 1","pages":"1 - 25"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-09-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00205-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45108147","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Does Jealousy Protect People from Infidelity? Investigating the Interplay Between Romantic Jealousy, Personality and the Probability of Detecting Infidelity 嫉妒能防止人们不忠吗?浪漫嫉妒、个性与发现不忠概率的相互作用研究
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-09-08 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00203-w
Menelaos Apostolou, Adamantia Antonopoulou

Objective

Extra-pair mating has potentially severe costs, which favor the evolution of mechanisms that would enable people to reduce them by detecting their partners’ infidelity. Such a mechanism is romantic jealousy, and the current research attempted to examine the interplay between romantic jealousy, personality and the probability of detecting infidelity.

Method

We employed quantitative research methods on a sample of 916 Greek-speaking participants.

Results

we found that higher scorers in romantic jealousy were more likely to detect infidelity than lower scorers. The effect was independent of one’s own infidelity, sex and age. We also found that neuroticism and openness predicted the probability to detect infidelity indirectly through jealousy. More specifically, high scorers in neuroticism experienced stronger jealousy, which in turn, was associated with increased probability to detect infidelity. On the other hand, high scorers in openness experienced lower jealousy that was associated with a decreased probability of detecting infidelity.

Conclusions

Our results were consistent with the hypothesis that the jealousy mechanism has evolved to enable individuals to detect infidelity.

额外配对可能会带来严重的成本,这有利于机制的演变,使人们能够通过检测伴侣的不忠来减少成本。这种机制就是浪漫嫉妒,目前的研究试图检验浪漫嫉妒、个性和发现不忠的可能性之间的相互作用。方法采用定量研究方法对916名讲希腊语的参与者进行调查。结果我们发现,在浪漫嫉妒中得分较高的人比得分较低的人更有可能发现不忠。这种影响与自己的不忠、性别和年龄无关。我们还发现,神经质和开放性预测了通过嫉妒间接发现不忠的可能性。更具体地说,神经质的高分者经历了更强的嫉妒,这反过来又与发现不忠的可能性增加有关。另一方面,开放性得分高的人嫉妒心较低,这与发现不忠的概率降低有关。结论我们的研究结果与嫉妒机制已经进化为使个体能够检测不忠的假设一致。
{"title":"Does Jealousy Protect People from Infidelity? Investigating the Interplay Between Romantic Jealousy, Personality and the Probability of Detecting Infidelity","authors":"Menelaos Apostolou,&nbsp;Adamantia Antonopoulou","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00203-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00203-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Extra-pair mating has potentially severe costs, which favor the evolution of mechanisms that would enable people to reduce them by detecting their partners’ infidelity. Such a mechanism is romantic jealousy, and the current research attempted to examine the interplay between romantic jealousy, personality and the probability of detecting infidelity.</p><h3>Method</h3><p>We employed quantitative research methods on a sample of 916 Greek-speaking participants. </p><h3>Results</h3><p>we found that higher scorers in romantic jealousy were more likely to detect infidelity than lower scorers. The effect was independent of one’s own infidelity, sex and age. We also found that neuroticism and openness predicted the probability to detect infidelity indirectly through jealousy. More specifically, high scorers in neuroticism experienced stronger jealousy, which in turn, was associated with increased probability to detect infidelity. On the other hand, high scorers in openness experienced lower jealousy that was associated with a decreased probability of detecting infidelity.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Our results were consistent with the hypothesis that the jealousy mechanism has evolved to enable individuals to detect infidelity.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 3","pages":"370 - 381"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-09-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00203-w.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44129410","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Relationships of the Ulna-to-fibula Ratio to Baseline and Reactive Steroid Hormone Levels: An Exploratory Study 尺骨与腓骨之比与基线和反应性类固醇激素水平的关系:一项探索性研究
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-08-31 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00204-9
Martin G. Köllner, Sinja Braun, Hanna Schöttner, Gelena Dlugash, Marlene Bettac, Simon Steib

Objective

Organizational hormone effects on the human brain and behavior are often retrospectively assessed via morphological markers of prenatal (e.g., 2D:4D digit ratio) or pubertal (e.g., facial width-to-height ratio, fWHR) hormone exposure. It has been argued that markers should relate to circulating hormones particularly in challenging, dominance/status-relevant situations. However, meta-analytic research indicates that fWHR, a frequently used pubertal marker, is neither reliably sex-dimorphic nor related to steroid hormones. This casts doubt on fWHR’s validity for reflecting hormone levels. Ulna-to-fibula ratio (UFR), an alternative, long-bone-length-based pubertal marker, is sex-dimorphic and associated with dominance motivation. However, its hormonal associations were never tested before. We therefore explored UFR’s relationships to baseline and reactive hormone levels.

Methods

We measured ulna and fibula length as well as shoulder/waist/hip circumference of 81 participants (49 women; after exclusions) via anthropometry. Salivary hormone levels (estradiol, testosterone) at baseline and after a gross-motor one-on-one balancing contest were measured via radioimmunoassay.

Results

We replicated UFR’s dimorphism, unrelatedness to height, and correlations to other putative markers of organizational hormone effects. On an exploratory basis, we found UFR to be related to overall baseline testosterone and to competition-induced reactive surges in steroid hormones (estradiol, testosterone) overall and in women.

Conclusions

Our results hint at UFR’s relationship to baseline testosterone and may indicate functional connections between outcomes of pubertal organizational hormone effects and contest-induced steroid reactivity. Pubertal organizational hormone effects may prepare the endocrine system for dominance and status contests. However, the small sample and the exploratory nature of our research demands replication.

组织激素对人脑和行为的影响通常通过产前(如2D:4D数字比)或青春期(如面部宽高比,fWHR)激素暴露的形态学标志物进行回顾性评估。有人认为,标记物应该与循环激素有关,特别是在具有挑战性的、与支配地位/地位相关的情况下。然而,荟萃分析研究表明,fWHR,一种常用的青春期标志物,既不是可靠的性别二态性,也与类固醇激素无关。这让人们对fWHR反映激素水平的有效性产生了怀疑。尺骨与腓骨比率(UFR)是一种替代性的、基于长骨长度的青春期标志物,是性别二型的,与支配动机有关。然而,它与荷尔蒙的关系以前从未被测试过。因此,我们探讨了UFR与基线和反应性激素水平的关系。方法我们通过人体测量法测量了81名参与者(49名女性;排除在外)的尺骨和腓骨长度以及肩/腰/臀围。通过放射免疫分析法测量基线和毛运动一对一平衡竞赛后的唾液激素水平(雌二醇、睾酮)。结果我们复制了UFR的二态性、与身高的不迟发性以及与其他公认的组织激素效应标志物的相关性。在探索的基础上,我们发现UFR与总体基线睾酮以及竞争诱导的类固醇激素(雌二醇、睾酮)反应性激增有关。结论我们的研究结果提示了UFR与基线睾酮的关系,并可能表明青春期组织激素效应的结果与竞争诱导的类固醇反应性之间的功能联系。青春期组织激素的作用可能使内分泌系统为支配地位和地位的竞争做好准备。然而,我们研究的小样本和探索性质需要复制。
{"title":"Relationships of the Ulna-to-fibula Ratio to Baseline and Reactive Steroid Hormone Levels: An Exploratory Study","authors":"Martin G. Köllner,&nbsp;Sinja Braun,&nbsp;Hanna Schöttner,&nbsp;Gelena Dlugash,&nbsp;Marlene Bettac,&nbsp;Simon Steib","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00204-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00204-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Organizational hormone effects on the human brain and behavior are often retrospectively assessed via morphological markers of prenatal (e.g., 2D:4D digit ratio) or pubertal (e.g., facial width-to-height ratio, fWHR) hormone exposure. It has been argued that markers should relate to circulating hormones particularly in challenging, dominance/status-relevant situations. However, meta-analytic research indicates that fWHR, a frequently used pubertal marker, is neither reliably sex-dimorphic nor related to steroid hormones. This casts doubt on fWHR’s validity for reflecting hormone levels. Ulna-to-fibula ratio (UFR), an alternative, long-bone-length-based pubertal marker, is sex-dimorphic and associated with dominance motivation. However, its hormonal associations were never tested before. We therefore explored UFR’s relationships to baseline and reactive hormone levels.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>We measured ulna and fibula length as well as shoulder/waist/hip circumference of 81 participants (49 women; after exclusions) via anthropometry. Salivary hormone levels (estradiol, testosterone) at baseline and after a gross-motor one-on-one balancing contest were measured via radioimmunoassay.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We replicated UFR’s dimorphism, unrelatedness to height, and correlations to other putative markers of organizational hormone effects. On an exploratory basis, we found UFR to be related to overall baseline testosterone and to competition-induced reactive surges in steroid hormones (estradiol, testosterone) overall and in women.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Our results hint at UFR’s relationship to baseline testosterone and may indicate functional connections between outcomes of pubertal organizational hormone effects and contest-induced steroid reactivity. Pubertal organizational hormone effects may prepare the endocrine system for dominance and status contests. However, the small sample and the exploratory nature of our research demands replication.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"407 - 432"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00204-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48543462","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychoneuroimmunology and Tattooing 精神神经免疫学和纹身
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-08-23 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00202-x
Christopher D. Lynn, Michaela E. Howells, Michael P. Muehlenbein, Holly Wood, Grey W. Caballero, Tomasz J. Nowak, Jeffrey Gassen

Objective

Though it injures the body in many ways, tattooing may also prepare it for later dermal stress through psychoneuroimmunological means.

Methods

To test this, we examined salivary endocrine (cortisol), immune (secretory immunoglobulin A), and inflammatory (C-reactive protein) responses to receiving a new tattoo relative to previous tattoo experience among 48 adults attending a tattoo festival.

Results

We found no effect of previous tattoo experience on pre-posttest cortisol but a significant main effect of extent of previous tattoo experience on pre-posttest cortisol and secretory immunoglobulin A and significant extent of body-by-hour tattooed interaction effect on C-reactive protein.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that the positive psychological evaluation of tattooing as eustress may contribute to biochemical adaptation through tattooing.

尽管纹身对身体有多种伤害,但它也可能通过心理神经免疫手段为以后的皮肤压力做好准备。方法为了测试这一点,我们在48名参加纹身节的成年人中,检测了接受新纹身后唾液内分泌(皮质醇)、免疫(分泌性免疫球蛋白A)和炎症(C反应蛋白)的反应,并与之前的纹身经历进行了比较。结果既往纹身经历对测试前后皮质醇无影响,但既往纹身经历程度对测试前后的皮质醇和分泌性免疫球蛋白a有显著的主要影响,且纹身对C反应蛋白有显著的逐小时交互作用。结论对纹身的积极心理评价可能有助于纹身后的生物化学适应。
{"title":"Psychoneuroimmunology and Tattooing","authors":"Christopher D. Lynn,&nbsp;Michaela E. Howells,&nbsp;Michael P. Muehlenbein,&nbsp;Holly Wood,&nbsp;Grey W. Caballero,&nbsp;Tomasz J. Nowak,&nbsp;Jeffrey Gassen","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00202-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00202-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Though it injures the body in many ways, tattooing may also prepare it for later dermal stress through psychoneuroimmunological means.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>To test this, we examined salivary endocrine (cortisol), immune (secretory immunoglobulin A), and inflammatory (C-reactive protein) responses to receiving a new tattoo relative to previous tattoo experience among 48 adults attending a tattoo festival.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We found no effect of previous tattoo experience on pre-posttest cortisol but a significant main effect of extent of previous tattoo experience on pre-posttest cortisol and secretory immunoglobulin A and significant extent of body-by-hour tattooed interaction effect on C-reactive protein.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These findings suggest that the positive psychological evaluation of tattooing as eustress may contribute to biochemical adaptation through tattooing.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 3","pages":"355 - 369"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43824997","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Two Routes to Status, One Route to Health: Trait Dominance and Prestige Differentially Associate with Self-reported Stress and Health in Two US University Populations 两条通往地位的途径,一条通往健康的途径:两个美国大学群体的特质优势和声望与自我报告的压力和健康的差异关联
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-08-23 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00199-3
Erik L. Knight

Objective

Social status has been extensively linked to stress and health outcomes. However, two routes by which status can be earned – dominance and prestige – may not uniformly relate to lower stress and better health because of inherent behavioral and stress-exposure differences in these two routes.

Methods

In one exploratory and two preregistered studies, participants (total N = 978) self-reported their trait dominance and prestige and self-reported several stress and health outcomes.

Results

The meta-effects evident across the three studies indicate that higher trait dominance was associated with worse outcomes – higher stress, poorer physical and mental health, poorer behavioral health, poorer life satisfaction, higher negative affect (range of absolute values of non-zero correlations, |r| = [0.074, 0.315], ps < 0.021) – and higher trait prestige was associated with better outcomes – lower stress, better physical and mental health, better behavioral health, better life satisfaction, higher positive and lower negative mood (|r| = [0.134, 0.478], ps < 0.001). These effects remained evident (with few exceptions) after controlling for socioeconomic status, other status-relevant traits, or self-enhancing motives; associations with behavior relevant to the COVID19 pandemic generally were not robust.

Conclusions

This work indicates that evolved traits related to the preferred route by which status is earned likely impact self-reported stress and health outcomes. Future research is necessary to examine physiological and other objective indicators of stress and health in more diverse populations.

社会地位与压力和健康结果有着广泛的联系。然而,由于这两种途径内在的行为和压力暴露差异,获得地位的两种途径——支配地位和声望——可能与降低压力和改善健康状况并不一致。方法在一项探索性研究和两项预先登记的研究中,参与者(总共N = 978)自我报告了他们的特质优势和声望,并自我报告了一些压力和健康结果。结果三项研究中明显的元效应表明,特质优势度越高,结果越差——压力越大,身心健康越差,行为健康越差、生活满意度越差,负面影响越高(非零相关性的绝对值范围,|r|=[0.074,0.315],ps <; 0.021),并且更高的特质声望与更好的结果相关——更低的压力、更好的身心健康、更好的行为健康、更好地生活满意度、更高的积极情绪和更低的消极情绪(|r|=[0.1340.478],ps <; 0.001)。在控制了社会经济地位、其他与地位相关的特征或自我提升动机后,这些影响仍然很明显(只有少数例外);与新冠肺炎疫情相关行为的关联性通常不强。结论这项研究表明,与获得地位的首选途径相关的进化特征可能会影响自我报告的压力和健康结果。未来的研究有必要在更多样化的人群中检查压力和健康的生理和其他客观指标。
{"title":"Two Routes to Status, One Route to Health: Trait Dominance and Prestige Differentially Associate with Self-reported Stress and Health in Two US University Populations","authors":"Erik L. Knight","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00199-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00199-3","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Social status has been extensively linked to stress and health outcomes. However, two routes by which status can be earned – dominance and prestige – may not uniformly relate to lower stress and better health because of inherent behavioral and stress-exposure differences in these two routes.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>In one exploratory and two preregistered studies, participants (total N = 978) self-reported their trait dominance and prestige and self-reported several stress and health outcomes.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>The meta-effects evident across the three studies indicate that higher trait dominance was associated with worse outcomes – higher stress, poorer physical and mental health, poorer behavioral health, poorer life satisfaction, higher negative affect (range of absolute values of non-zero correlations, |<i>r</i>| = [0.074, 0.315], <i>p</i>s &lt; 0.021) – and higher trait prestige was associated with better outcomes – lower stress, better physical and mental health, better behavioral health, better life satisfaction, higher positive and lower negative mood (|<i>r</i>| = [0.134, 0.478], <i>p</i>s &lt; 0.001). These effects remained evident (with few exceptions) after controlling for socioeconomic status, other status-relevant traits, or self-enhancing motives; associations with behavior relevant to the COVID19 pandemic generally were not robust.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>This work indicates that evolved traits related to the preferred route by which status is earned likely impact self-reported stress and health outcomes. Future research is necessary to examine physiological and other objective indicators of stress and health in more diverse populations.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"461 - 488"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00199-3.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33443845","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adaptive Calibration in Early Development: Brief Measures of Perceived Childhood Harshness and Unpredictability 早期发展中的适应性校准:儿童时期感知的刺耳和不可预测性的简要测量
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-08-22 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00200-z
Heather M. Maranges, Connor R. Hasty, Jose L. Martinez, Jon K. Maner

Objective

A burgeoning literature inspired by life history theory suggests that psychological and behavioral processes become adaptively calibrated to the levels of harshness and unpredictability encountered in early developmental environments. The current research develops and validates brief scales intended to measure perceptions of childhood harshness (resource scarcity) and unpredictability.

Methods

Data were collected from adults in the U.S. (total N = 3252). Study 1 was used to design the measures and confirm reliability. Study 2 provided evidence of convergent and discriminant validity. Study 3 assessed associations between the perceived harshness and unpredictability scales and indicators of life history strategies.

Results

The scales showed good convergent validity (e.g., moderate-to-strong associations with adverse childhood experiences, impulsivity, and a lack of self-control) and discriminant validity (e.g., null-to-low associations with social desirability, sex, and age), as well as associations with biometric (e.g., age of menarche and sexual debut), behavioral (e.g., number of sexual partners, age of first offspring, number of offspring), and psychometric (e.g., scores on the K-SF-42 and Mini-K) indicators of life history strategies.

Conclusions

These scales provide easy-to-administer retrospective measures of perceived childhood harshness and unpredictability and facilitate research testing hypotheses related to adaptive calibration.

受生命史理论启发,一篇新兴的文献表明,心理和行为过程会根据早期发展环境中遇到的严酷和不可预测性水平进行适应性校准。目前的研究开发并验证了旨在衡量儿童对严酷(资源稀缺)和不可预测性的看法的简短量表。方法收集美国成年人的数据(总N = 3252)。研究1用于设计措施并确认可靠性。研究2提供了收敛和判别有效性的证据。研究3评估了感知的严酷性和不可预测性量表以及生活史策略指标之间的关联。结果量表显示出良好的收敛有效性(例如,与不良童年经历、冲动和缺乏自我控制有中度到强烈的关联)和判别有效性(如,与社会愿望、性别和年龄的零到低关联),以及与生物特征的关联(例如,初潮年龄和性首次亮相),生活史策略的行为指标(例如,性伴侣的数量、第一个后代的年龄、后代的数量)和心理指标(例如K-SF-42和Mini-K的得分)。结论这些量表提供了易于管理的儿童时期感知的严酷性和不可预测性的回顾性测量,并有助于研究测试与适应性校准相关的假设。
{"title":"Adaptive Calibration in Early Development: Brief Measures of Perceived Childhood Harshness and Unpredictability","authors":"Heather M. Maranges,&nbsp;Connor R. Hasty,&nbsp;Jose L. Martinez,&nbsp;Jon K. Maner","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00200-z","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00200-z","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>A burgeoning literature inspired by life history theory suggests that psychological and behavioral processes become adaptively calibrated to the levels of harshness and unpredictability encountered in early developmental environments. The current research develops and validates brief scales intended to measure perceptions of childhood harshness (resource scarcity) and unpredictability.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Data were collected from adults in the U.S. (total <i>N</i> = 3252). Study 1 was used to design the measures and confirm reliability. Study 2 provided evidence of convergent and discriminant validity. Study 3 assessed associations between the perceived harshness and unpredictability scales and indicators of life history strategies.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>The scales showed good convergent validity (e.g., moderate-to-strong associations with adverse childhood experiences, impulsivity, and a lack of self-control) and discriminant validity (e.g., null-to-low associations with social desirability, sex, and age), as well as associations with biometric (e.g., age of menarche and sexual debut), behavioral (e.g., number of sexual partners, age of first offspring, number of offspring), and psychometric (e.g., scores on the K-SF-42 and Mini-K) indicators of life history strategies.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These scales provide easy-to-administer retrospective measures of perceived childhood harshness and unpredictability and facilitate research testing hypotheses related to adaptive calibration.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 3","pages":"313 - 343"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47801838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Testosterone, estradiol, and immune response in women 女性的睾酮、雌二醇和免疫反应
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-08-19 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00201-y
Javier I. Borráz-León, Severi Luoto, Indrikis A. Krams, Markus J. Rantala, Giedrius Trakimas, Sanita Kecko, Tatjana Krama

Objective

Findings on the associations between sex hormones and immune function are scarce and mixed, especially in women. To contribute to the understanding on how sex hormones and immune function interact, we analyzed relationships between testosterone, estradiol, and immune responses in women.

Methods

Two doses of hepatitis B vaccine were administered to a group of 55 healthy women. Venous blood samples were collected at three time points: before the first vaccination (time I), one month after the first vaccination (time II), and one month after the second vaccination (time III), to quantify sex hormone levels (i.e., testosterone and estradiol) and the production of antibodies in response to the hepatitis B vaccinations.

Results

Women’s immune response (i.e., the production of hepatitis B antibodies) was negatively associated with testosterone levels one month after the first vaccination and positively associated with estradiol levels one month after the second vaccination. A decrease in testosterone levels between time II and time III was also observed. No differences in estradiol levels over time were found.

Conclusion

Our results demonstrate negative associations between testosterone and immune responses in women as previously described for males of several animal species, including humans. There were also positive associations between estradiol and immune responses, highlighting the immunomodulatory role of sex hormones in women. Potential bidirectional effects between immune markers and sex hormones are discussed.

关于性激素与免疫功能之间关系的研究很少,而且参差不齐,尤其是在女性中。为了有助于理解性激素和免疫功能是如何相互作用的,我们分析了女性睾酮、雌二醇和免疫反应之间的关系。方法对55名健康女性接种两剂乙肝疫苗。在三个时间点采集静脉血样:第一次接种疫苗前(时间I)、第一次接种后一个月(时间II)和第二次接种疫苗后一个小时(时间III),以量化性激素水平(即睾酮和雌二醇)和对乙肝疫苗的抗体产生。结果女性的免疫反应(即乙肝抗体的产生)与第一次接种后1个月的睾酮水平呈负相关,与第二次接种后一个月的雌二醇水平呈正相关。还观察到在时间II和时间III之间睾酮水平的降低。没有发现雌二醇水平随时间变化的差异。结论我们的研究结果表明,女性的睾酮和免疫反应之间存在负相关,正如之前对包括人类在内的几个动物物种的雄性所描述的那样。雌二醇与免疫反应之间也存在正相关,这突出了性激素在女性中的免疫调节作用。讨论了免疫标志物和性激素之间潜在的双向作用。
{"title":"Testosterone, estradiol, and immune response in women","authors":"Javier I. Borráz-León,&nbsp;Severi Luoto,&nbsp;Indrikis A. Krams,&nbsp;Markus J. Rantala,&nbsp;Giedrius Trakimas,&nbsp;Sanita Kecko,&nbsp;Tatjana Krama","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00201-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00201-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Findings on the associations between sex hormones and immune function are scarce and mixed, especially in women. To contribute to the understanding on how sex hormones and immune function interact, we analyzed relationships between testosterone, estradiol, and immune responses in women.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Two doses of hepatitis B vaccine were administered to a group of 55 healthy women. Venous blood samples were collected at three time points: before the first vaccination (time I), one month after the first vaccination (time II), and one month after the second vaccination (time III), to quantify sex hormone levels (i.e., testosterone and estradiol) and the production of antibodies in response to the hepatitis B vaccinations.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Women’s immune response (i.e., the production of hepatitis B antibodies) was negatively associated with testosterone levels one month after the first vaccination and positively associated with estradiol levels one month after the second vaccination. A decrease in testosterone levels between time II and time III was also observed. No differences in estradiol levels over time were found.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>Our results demonstrate negative associations between testosterone and immune responses in women as previously described for males of several animal species, including humans. There were also positive associations between estradiol and immune responses, highlighting the immunomodulatory role of sex hormones in women. Potential bidirectional effects between immune markers and sex hormones are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 3","pages":"344 - 354"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49290726","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1