首页 > 最新文献

Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology最新文献

英文 中文
When Toughness Begets Respect: Dominant Individuals Gain Prestige and Leadership By Facilitating Intragroup Conflict Resolution 当坚韧开始受到尊重:强势个体通过促进团队内部冲突解决获得声望和领导力
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6
Joey T. Cheng, Nathan A. Dhaliwal, Miranda A. Too

Objective

Why do dominant leaders rise to power via the popular vote? This research tests whether when people feel threatened by intra-group disorder they desire stronger, more dominant leaders.

Methods

Participants (N = 1,026) read a vignette that depicts a within-group norm violation. We then used a between-subjects design to randomly assign participants to a specific version of the vignette in which (a) a focal target individual in the scenario varied in their dominance (punitiveness: from no to moderate to strong); and (b) the local group faced little or substantial intra-group conflict and disorder (threat: from low to high). Following this, participants reported how much they endorse the target individuals as leader and the individual’s perceived prestige.

Results

We find that intra-group conflict motivates a psychology that favors the rise of dominant leaders: Highly punitive individuals (seen as highly dominant) are endorsed as leaders when in-group threat is high, but comparably disfavored when threat is low. Under low threat, non-punitive individuals (who are seen as less dominant) are endorsed as leaders. Subsequent analyses reveal that these shifts in leader preferences are explained by corresponding changes in prestige. Under conditions of high threat, dominance confers prestige, whereas under low threat, dominance suppresses prestige. Tests of mediation further show that the effect of dominance on increased leader support under high threat is mediated by prestige.

Conclusions

In contexts of threat, such as internal disorder, dominant leaders are favored and gain prestige, owing to their perceived ability to supply benefits such as in mediating internal conflicts.

为什么占主导地位的领导人通过普选上台?这项研究测试了当人们感到受到群体内部障碍的威胁时,他们是否渴望更强大、更具统治力的领导者。方法参与者(N = 1026)读取描绘组内规范违反的小插曲。然后,我们使用受试者之间的设计,将参与者随机分配到一个特定版本的小插曲中,其中(a)场景中的焦点目标个体的主导地位各不相同(惩罚程度:从否到中等再到强烈);以及(b)当地群体几乎没有或实质上面临群体内部冲突和混乱(威胁:从低到高)。在此之后,参与者报告了他们对目标个人作为领导者的认可程度以及个人的声望。结果我们发现,群体内冲突激发了一种倾向于主导型领导者崛起的心理:当群体内威胁较高时,高度惩罚性的个人(被视为高度主导型)被认可为领导者,但当威胁较低时,则相对不受欢迎。在低威胁下,非惩罚性个人(被视为不那么占主导地位)被认可为领导者。随后的分析表明,领导者偏好的这些变化是由声望的相应变化来解释的。在高威胁条件下,支配地位赋予声望,而在低威胁条件下支配地位压制声望。调解测试进一步表明,在高威胁下,主导地位对增加领导人支持的影响是由声望调解的。结论在内部混乱等威胁环境中,占主导地位的领导人因其在调解内部冲突等方面提供利益的能力而受到青睐并获得声望。
{"title":"When Toughness Begets Respect: Dominant Individuals Gain Prestige and Leadership By Facilitating Intragroup Conflict Resolution","authors":"Joey T. Cheng,&nbsp;Nathan A. Dhaliwal,&nbsp;Miranda A. Too","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective\u0000</h3><p>Why do dominant leaders rise to power via the popular vote? This research tests whether when people feel threatened by intra-group disorder they desire stronger, more dominant leaders.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Participants (<i>N</i> = 1,026) read a vignette that depicts a within-group norm violation. We then used a between-subjects design to randomly assign participants to a specific version of the vignette in which (a) a focal target individual in the scenario varied in their dominance (punitiveness: from no to moderate to strong); and (b) the local group faced little or substantial intra-group conflict and disorder (threat: from low to high). Following this, participants reported how much they endorse the target individuals as leader and the individual’s perceived prestige.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We find that intra-group conflict motivates a psychology that favors the rise of dominant leaders: Highly punitive individuals (seen as highly dominant) are endorsed as leaders when in-group threat is high, but comparably disfavored when threat is low. Under low threat, non-punitive individuals (who are seen as less dominant) are endorsed as leaders. Subsequent analyses reveal that these shifts in leader preferences are explained by corresponding changes in prestige. Under conditions of high threat, dominance confers prestige, whereas under low threat, dominance suppresses prestige. Tests of mediation further show that the effect of dominance on increased leader support under high threat is mediated by prestige.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>In contexts of threat, such as internal disorder, dominant leaders are favored and gain prestige, owing to their perceived ability to supply benefits such as in mediating internal conflicts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42113154","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting 你所看到的可能就是你想要的:在自然环境中复制配偶
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-27 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9
Ryan C. Anderson, Ashleigh A. Armstrong
{"title":"What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting","authors":"Ryan C. Anderson, Ashleigh A. Armstrong","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"52893523","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting 你看到的可能是你想要的:在自然环境中复制伴侣
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-07-27 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9
Ryan C. Anderson, Ashleigh A. Armstrong

Abstract

Objectives

The phenomenon of mate copying (MC) suggests that an individual’s romantic desirability varies, to some degree, as a function of their romantic experiences. By manipulating relationship history, this research aimed to determine whether male desirability varied similarly for static stimuli (photographs) as it did for dynamic stimuli (short clips). Here we present 2 studies examining this idea.

Methods

In both studies heterosexual women aged 18–34 were recruited from social media and evaluated the desirability of men, before and after being given information about the level of relationship experience the men had previously had (either ‘none’, ‘some’, or ‘lots’).

Results

In Study 1 (N = 278), we employed static images of men and Study 2 (N = 66) was methodologically similar but employed dynamic stimuli. Study 1 indicated that a man’s desirability to heterosexual women increased when it was revealed that he had a moderate amount of relationship experience but decreased if he had none. Results from Study 2 suggested that desirability was not affected by an individual’s relationship experience.

Conclusions

MC was indicated when stimuli were static, but not when they were dynamic. These results suggest that MC may be a phenomenon that exists to a greater extent when stimuli are less informative.

摘要目的伴侣复制现象表明,一个人的浪漫欲望在一定程度上随着他们的浪漫经历而变化。通过操纵关系史,这项研究旨在确定男性对静态刺激(照片)的渴望是否与对动态刺激(短片)的渴望相似。在这里,我们提出了两项研究来检验这一观点。方法在这两项研究中,年龄在18-34岁的异性恋女性都是从社交媒体上招募的,并在获得关于男性以前有过的关系经历水平的信息(“没有”、“一些”或“很多”)之前和之后评估男性的可取性 = 278),我们采用了男性的静态图像和研究2(N = 66)在方法上相似,但采用了动态刺激。研究1表明,当男性有适度的恋爱经历时,他对异性恋女性的好感度会增加,但如果没有恋爱经历,好感度会降低。研究2的结果表明,可取性不受个人关系经历的影响。结论sMC在静态刺激时显示,而在动态刺激时不显示。这些结果表明,当刺激信息较少时,MC可能是一种更大程度上存在的现象。
{"title":"What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting","authors":"Ryan C. Anderson,&nbsp;Ashleigh A. Armstrong","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h2>Abstract\u0000</h2><div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>The phenomenon of mate copying (MC) suggests that an individual’s romantic desirability varies, to some degree, as a function of their romantic experiences. By manipulating relationship history, this research aimed to determine whether male desirability varied similarly for static stimuli (photographs) as it did for dynamic stimuli (short clips). Here we present 2 studies examining this idea.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>In both studies heterosexual women aged 18–34 were recruited from social media and evaluated the desirability of men, before and after being given information about the level of relationship experience the men had previously had (either ‘none’, ‘some’, or ‘lots’).</p><h3>Results</h3><p>In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 278), we employed static images of men and Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 66) was methodologically similar but employed dynamic stimuli. Study 1 indicated that a man’s desirability to heterosexual women increased when it was revealed that he had a moderate amount of relationship experience but decreased if he had none. Results from Study 2 suggested that desirability was not affected by an individual’s relationship experience.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>MC was indicated when stimuli were static, but not when they were dynamic. These results suggest that MC may be a phenomenon that exists to a greater extent when stimuli are less informative.</p></div></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50517652","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mu opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) moderates the influence of perceived parental attention on social support seeking Mu阿片受体基因(OPRM1)调节父母关注对社会支持寻求的影响
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-06-08 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w
Shaofeng Zheng, Keiko Ishii, Takahiko Masuda, Masahiro Matsunaga, Yasuki Noguchi, Hidenori Yamasue, Yohsuke Ohtsubo

Objective

Caring, sensitive parenting is known to be associated with higher levels of engagement in support-seeking behaviors among children and young adolescents. However, no study has yet explored the role of perceived parental attention in social support seeking in early adulthood. Growing evidence suggests that the µ-opioid receptor gene polymorphism (OPRM1 A118G) moderates one’s responsiveness to social environments. Prompted by the differential susceptibility theory of gene–environment interaction, the present study examined whether the OPRM1 polymorphism would moderate the association between perceived parental attention and social support seeking in early adulthood.

Methods

Six hundred and twenty Japanese undergraduate students self-reported the amount of attention they subjectively perceived their parents to have given them during childhood and completed scales that assess support-seeking behaviors. Clippings of their fingernails were collected for genotyping.

Results

The results showed that the interactive effect of perceived parental attention and OPRM1 genotypes on social support seeking was significant. Specifically, perceived parental attention only significantly and positively predicted OPRM1 GG carriers’ social support seeking in response to stress.

Conclusions

The findings support the view that OPRM1 polymorphism moderates the association between early experiences and developmental outcomes.

众所周知,关爱、敏感的育儿方式与儿童和青少年更高程度的寻求支持行为有关。然而,目前还没有研究探讨父母关注在成年早期寻求社会支持中的作用。越来越多的证据表明,µ-阿片受体基因多态性(OPRM1 A118G)调节了一个人对社会环境的反应。在基因-环境相互作用的差异易感性理论的推动下,本研究检验了OPRM1多态性是否会调节成年早期感知到的父母关注和寻求社会支持之间的关联。方法620名日本大学生自我报告他们主观上认为父母在童年时期给予他们的关注程度,并完成评估寻求支持行为的量表。采集他们指甲的剪片进行基因分型。结果父母关注感与OPRM1基因型对社会支持寻求的交互作用显著。具体而言,感知到的父母关注仅显著且积极地预测了OPRM1 GG携带者对压力的社会支持寻求。结论研究结果支持了OPRM1多态性调节早期经历和发育结果之间关系的观点。
{"title":"Mu opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) moderates the influence of perceived parental attention on social support seeking","authors":"Shaofeng Zheng,&nbsp;Keiko Ishii,&nbsp;Takahiko Masuda,&nbsp;Masahiro Matsunaga,&nbsp;Yasuki Noguchi,&nbsp;Hidenori Yamasue,&nbsp;Yohsuke Ohtsubo","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Caring, sensitive parenting is known to be associated with higher levels of engagement in support-seeking behaviors among children and young adolescents. However, no study has yet explored the role of perceived parental attention in social support seeking in early adulthood. Growing evidence suggests that the µ-opioid receptor gene polymorphism (<i>OPRM1</i> A118G) moderates one’s responsiveness to social environments. Prompted by the differential susceptibility theory of gene–environment interaction, the present study examined whether the <i>OPRM1</i> polymorphism would moderate the association between perceived parental attention and social support seeking in early adulthood.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Six hundred and twenty Japanese undergraduate students self-reported the amount of attention they subjectively perceived their parents to have given them during childhood and completed scales that assess support-seeking behaviors. Clippings of their fingernails were collected for genotyping.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>The results showed that the interactive effect of perceived parental attention and <i>OPRM1</i> genotypes on social support seeking was significant. Specifically, perceived parental attention only significantly and positively predicted <i>OPRM1</i> GG carriers’ social support seeking in response to stress.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The findings support the view that <i>OPRM1</i> polymorphism moderates the association between early experiences and developmental outcomes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46689411","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Human Females as a Dispersal-Egalitarian Species: A Hypothesis about Women and Status 作为分散的平等主义物种的人类女性:关于女性和地位的假设
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-05-21 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00191-x
Joyce F. Benenson

Objectives

A paradox exists in research on girls and women. On the one hand, they behave in a more egalitarian fashion than their male counterparts. On the other hand, status increases their own and their children’s survival.

Methods

Evidence from non-human primates can help reconcile these findings. In species that do not reside with female kin for life, females are relatively egalitarian and individualistic. They typically do not cooperate or engage in direct competition and exhibit little tolerance for status differentials.

Results and Conclusions

Women follow this pattern. While a husband’s status and her female relatives’ support enhance a woman’s status and reproductive success, her own actions too influence her access to resources and allies. Evidence on girls’ and women’s same-sex competition and quests for status supports the hypothesis that human females inhabit dispersal-egalitarian communities in which competition is avoided, an egalitarian ethos prevails, competitive behavior is disguised, and status differentials are not tolerated.

目的女孩与妇女研究中存在着一个悖论。一方面,她们的行为比男性更加平等。另一方面,地位增加了他们自己和孩子的生存。方法来自非人类灵长类动物的证据可以帮助调和这些发现。在不与女性亲属终身居住的物种中,女性相对平等且个人主义。他们通常不合作或直接竞争,对地位差异几乎没有容忍度。结果与结论女性遵循这种模式。虽然丈夫的地位和女性亲属的支持提高了女性的地位和生育成功率,但她自己的行为也影响了她获得资源和盟友的机会。关于女孩和妇女的同性竞争和对地位的追求的证据支持这样一种假设,即人类女性生活在分散的平等主义社区中,在这个社区中,竞争被避免,平等主义精神盛行,竞争行为被掩盖,地位差异是不可容忍的。
{"title":"Human Females as a Dispersal-Egalitarian Species: A Hypothesis about Women and Status","authors":"Joyce F. Benenson","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00191-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00191-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>A paradox exists in research on girls and women. On the one hand, they behave in a more egalitarian fashion than their male counterparts. On the other hand, status increases their own and their children’s survival.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Evidence from non-human primates can help reconcile these findings. In species that do not reside with female kin for life, females are relatively egalitarian and individualistic. They typically do not cooperate or engage in direct competition and exhibit little tolerance for status differentials.</p><h3>Results and Conclusions</h3><p>Women follow this pattern. While a husband’s status and her female relatives’ support enhance a woman’s status and reproductive success, her own actions too influence her access to resources and allies. Evidence on girls’ and women’s same-sex competition and quests for status supports the hypothesis that human females inhabit dispersal-egalitarian communities in which competition is avoided, an egalitarian ethos prevails, competitive behavior is disguised, and status differentials are not tolerated.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48103980","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Associations Between Sexual Desire and Within-Individual Testosterone and Cortisol in Men and Women 性欲与男性和女性个体睾酮和皮质醇之间的关系
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-18 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00184-w
Kevin A Rosenfield, Heather Self, Talia Shirazi, Rodrigo Cardenes, Justin Carré, Triana Ortiz, Khytam Dawood, David A. Puts

Objective

The dual-hormone hypothesis (DHH) posits that some effects of testosterone on human behavior and psychology related to status-seeking are moderated by cortisol, such that they are stronger when cortisol levels are low. In support of the DHH, studies have found that cortisol negatively moderated the relationship between testosterone and such traits as status-seeking and interest in uncommitted sex. Others indicate a positive moderating influence of cortisol in some cases. Here, we test whether two psychosexual indices—sexual desire and sociosexuality—meet the expectations of the DHH in a large sample of men and women.

Method

646 women and 185 men attended lab sessions during which they provided saliva samples for hormonal analysis and responded to the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory-Revised and the Sexual Desire Inventory (180 women and 43 men returned for a second session approximately two months later). We quantified salivary hormone concentrations using ELISA and assessed within- and between-participant effects of hormones on psychosexual measures with mixed-effects models.

Results

We observed a positive interaction between within-subjects cortisol and testosterone in models of sexual desire in both men and women. For women, these effects emerged in models of general sexual desire and in models of the dyadic desire subscale and were robust to many analytical configurations. For men, the effects were limited to models of solitary desire, but were also robust to alternative analyses. We present data to quantify our risks of both type I and type II error.

Conclusions

Some of our results contrast with usual dual-hormone hypothesis predictions of negative interactions between testosterone and cortisol. We suggest several potential explanations for these results, including a positive feedback loop whereby elevated testosterone prompts increases in sexual desire and behavior, necessitating cortisol-induced mobilization of energy stores.

目的双重激素假说(DHH)认为,睾酮对人类行为和心理的一些影响与地位寻求有关,它们受到皮质醇的调节,因此当皮质醇水平较低时,它们会更强。为了支持DHH,研究发现皮质醇对睾酮和寻求地位和对非承诺性行为感兴趣等特征之间的关系起到了负面调节作用。另一些则表明在某些情况下皮质醇具有积极的调节作用。在这里,我们在大量男性和女性样本中测试了两个性心理指数——性欲和社会性行为——是否符合DHH的预期。方法646名女性和185名男性参加了实验室会议,期间他们提供唾液样本进行激素分析,并对修订的社会性取向量表和性欲量表做出了回应(180名女性和43名男性在大约两个月后返回进行第二次会议)。我们使用ELISA定量唾液激素浓度,并使用混合效应模型评估激素对性心理测量的参与者内部和参与者之间的影响。结果在男性和女性的性欲模型中,我们观察到受试者体内皮质醇和睾酮之间存在积极的相互作用。对于女性来说,这些影响出现在一般性欲模型和二元欲望分量表模型中,并且在许多分析配置中都是稳健的。对于男性来说,这种影响仅限于孤独欲望的模型,但也适用于替代分析。我们提供数据来量化I型和II型错误的风险。结论我们的一些结果与通常的双激素假说预测睾酮和皮质醇之间的负相互作用形成对比。我们对这些结果提出了几个潜在的解释,包括一个正反馈回路,即睾酮升高会促使性欲和行为增加,从而需要皮质醇诱导的能量储备动员。
{"title":"Associations Between Sexual Desire and Within-Individual Testosterone and Cortisol in Men and Women","authors":"Kevin A Rosenfield,&nbsp;Heather Self,&nbsp;Talia Shirazi,&nbsp;Rodrigo Cardenes,&nbsp;Justin Carré,&nbsp;Triana Ortiz,&nbsp;Khytam Dawood,&nbsp;David A. Puts","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00184-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00184-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>The dual-hormone hypothesis (DHH) posits that some effects of testosterone on human behavior and psychology related to status-seeking are moderated by cortisol, such that they are stronger when cortisol levels are low. In support of the DHH, studies have found that cortisol negatively moderated the relationship between testosterone and such traits as status-seeking and interest in uncommitted sex. Others indicate a positive moderating influence of cortisol in some cases. Here, we test whether two psychosexual indices—sexual desire and sociosexuality—meet the expectations of the DHH in a large sample of men and women.</p><h3>Method</h3><p>646 women and 185 men attended lab sessions during which they provided saliva samples for hormonal analysis and responded to the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory-Revised and the Sexual Desire Inventory (180 women and 43 men returned for a second session approximately two months later). We quantified salivary hormone concentrations using ELISA and assessed within- and between-participant effects of hormones on psychosexual measures with mixed-effects models.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We observed a positive interaction between within-subjects cortisol and testosterone in models of sexual desire in both men and women. For women, these effects emerged in models of general sexual desire and in models of the dyadic desire subscale and were robust to many analytical configurations. For men, the effects were limited to models of solitary desire, but were also robust to alternative analyses. We present data to quantify our risks of both type I and type II error.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Some of our results contrast with usual dual-hormone hypothesis predictions of negative interactions between testosterone and cortisol. We suggest several potential explanations for these results, including a positive feedback loop whereby elevated testosterone prompts increases in sexual desire and behavior, necessitating cortisol-induced mobilization of energy stores.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41416908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Voice Changes Across the Menstrual Cycle in Response to Masculinized and Feminized Man and Woman 男性化和女性化男性和女性在月经周期中的声音变化
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-15 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00190-y
Irena Pavela Banai, Robert P. Burriss, Nataša Šimić

Objective

Research has demonstrated that men prefer women’s voices with higher pitch and that women’s voices recorded at high compared to low fertility phases of the menstrual cycle are rated as more attractive. These findings suggest that voice conveys information relevant to reproductive success. Because voice attractiveness is higher during the high fertility phase and voice pitch positively predicts attractiveness ratings, it has been hypothesized that cyclical changes in vocal attractiveness are driven by changes in voice pitch. However, attempts to detect acoustic changes have produced mixed results. With the higher degree of ecological validity achieved by including social context (simulated interactions with men and women) and by recording voice in the three phases of menstrual cycles, the present study addresses limitations of previous research.

Methods

Forty-eight naturally cycling women were recorded during the menstrual, late follicular (high fertility), and luteal phases while leaving voice messages to masculinized and femininized men and women.

Results

No cycle-related changes in pitch and pitch variability for the recordings directed to masculinized and femininized men and women were detected. By including relationship status as predictor in additional models, higher-order interaction effects showed that single and partnered women displayed opposite cycle-related pitch changes directed only to women, but not men.

Conclusion

The cycle-related voice changes found in the present study do not support the hypothesis that cyclic pitch variations represent an adaptive mechanism for attracting partners. We discuss cyclic changes in voice pitch in relation to intrasexual competition by taking into an account that the present study is likely underpowered for adequate testing of the complex higher-order interactions.

客观研究表明,男性更喜欢女性音调更高的声音,而在月经周期的高生育期与低生育期相比,女性的声音被认为更具吸引力。这些发现表明,声音传达了与生殖成功相关的信息。因为在高生育率阶段,声音吸引力更高,而音高正预测吸引力评级,因此有人假设,声音吸引力的周期性变化是由音高的变化驱动的。然而,检测声学变化的尝试产生了喜忧参半的结果。通过包括社会背景(模拟与男性和女性的互动)和记录月经周期三个阶段的声音,本研究实现了更高程度的生态有效性,解决了以往研究的局限性。方法记录48名自然周期女性在月经期、卵泡晚期(高生育率)和黄体期的情况,同时给男性化和女性化的男性和女性留下语音信息。结果在针对男性化和女性化男性和女性的录音中,没有检测到音高和音高变异性的周期相关变化。通过在其他模型中纳入关系状态作为预测因素,高阶交互效应表明,单身和伴侣女性表现出与周期相反的音调变化,这些变化只针对女性,而不针对男性。结论本研究中发现的与周期相关的语音变化不支持周期音高变化代表吸引伴侣的适应机制的假设。我们讨论了音调的周期性变化与性内竞争的关系,考虑到本研究可能缺乏对复杂的高阶相互作用进行充分测试的能力。
{"title":"Voice Changes Across the Menstrual Cycle in Response to Masculinized and Feminized Man and Woman","authors":"Irena Pavela Banai,&nbsp;Robert P. Burriss,&nbsp;Nataša Šimić","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00190-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00190-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Research has demonstrated that men prefer women’s voices with higher pitch and that women’s voices recorded at high compared to low fertility phases of the menstrual cycle are rated as more attractive. These findings suggest that voice conveys information relevant to reproductive success. Because voice attractiveness is higher during the high fertility phase and voice pitch positively predicts attractiveness ratings, it has been hypothesized that cyclical changes in vocal attractiveness are driven by changes in voice pitch. However, attempts to detect acoustic changes have produced mixed results. With the higher degree of ecological validity achieved by including social context (simulated interactions with men and women) and by recording voice in the three phases of menstrual cycles, the present study addresses limitations of previous research.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Forty-eight naturally cycling women were recorded during the menstrual, late follicular (high fertility), and luteal phases while leaving voice messages to masculinized and femininized men and women.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>No cycle-related changes in pitch and pitch variability for the recordings directed to masculinized and femininized men and women were detected. By including relationship status as predictor in additional models, higher-order interaction effects showed that single and partnered women displayed opposite cycle-related pitch changes directed only to women, but not men.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>The cycle-related voice changes found in the present study do not support the hypothesis that cyclic pitch variations represent an adaptive mechanism for attracting partners. We discuss cyclic changes in voice pitch in relation to intrasexual competition by taking into an account that the present study is likely underpowered for adequate testing of the complex higher-order interactions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45687908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Genetically-predicted trait-BMI, everyday discrimination and life satisfaction among older U.S. adults 美国老年人的遗传预测特征BMI、日常歧视和生活满意度
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-04-01 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00189-5
Aniruddha Das Ph.D.

Objectives

This study tested whether genetically predicted trait-body mass index (trait-BMI) was linked to more general daily discrimination among older adults, and consequently to decline in their life satisfaction.

Methods

Data were from the Health and Retirement Study, nationally representative of U.S. adults over 50. Genetic prediction models were used to extract the trait component of BMI, which was then deployed in regression models for discrimination. A recently developed “regression with residuals” approach was used to test associations with subsequent change in life satisfaction.

Results

Genetically predicted trait-BMI was linked to more general discrimination reports. It also had negative associations with change in life satisfaction—linkages not consistently or strongly mediated by discrimination.

Conclusions

Trait-BMI—arguably resistant to sustained alteration through individual efforts—seems linked to decline in older adults’ life satisfaction. General daily discrimination, however, may not be an important mechanism.

目的本研究测试了遗传预测的特征体重指数(trait BMI)是否与老年人更普遍的日常歧视有关,从而与他们的生活满意度下降有关。方法数据来自健康与退休研究,该研究在全国范围内代表了50岁以上的美国成年人。使用遗传预测模型提取BMI的特征成分,然后将其应用于回归模型中进行判别。最近开发的一种“残差回归”方法被用来测试与随后生活满意度变化的相关性。结果遗传预测的特征BMI与更普遍的歧视报告有关。它也与生活满意度的变化有负面联系——这种联系并不总是或强烈地受到歧视的影响。结论高BMI——可以说是抵抗个人努力的持续改变——似乎与老年人生活满意度的下降有关。然而,一般的日常歧视可能不是一个重要的机制。
{"title":"Genetically-predicted trait-BMI, everyday discrimination and life satisfaction among older U.S. adults","authors":"Aniruddha Das Ph.D.","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00189-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00189-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>This study tested whether genetically predicted trait-body mass index (trait-BMI) was linked to more general daily discrimination among older adults, and consequently to decline in their life satisfaction.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Data were from the Health and Retirement Study, nationally representative of U.S. adults over 50. Genetic prediction models were used to extract the trait component of BMI, which was then deployed in regression models for discrimination. A recently developed “regression with residuals” approach was used to test associations with subsequent change in life satisfaction.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Genetically predicted trait-BMI was linked to more general discrimination reports. It also had negative associations with change in life satisfaction—linkages not consistently or strongly mediated by discrimination.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Trait-BMI—arguably resistant to sustained alteration through individual efforts—seems linked to decline in older adults’ life satisfaction. General daily discrimination, however, may not be an important mechanism.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48053165","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Changes in Positive Affect Due to Popularity in an Experimental Dating Context Influence Some of Men’s, but Not Women’s, Socio-Political Attitudes 在实验约会环境中,由于受欢迎而产生的积极影响的变化会影响一些男性的社会政治态度,但不会影响女性
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-03-14 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00188-6
Francesca R. Luberti, Khandis R. Blake, Robert C. Brooks

Objective

Trait mate value covaries with several socio-political attitudes. One’s dating popularity in a mating market can, however, shift one’s self-perceived mate value in that market. We tested whether dating popularity could therefore also shift socio-political attitudes, and whether trait mate value could moderate this effect.

Method

Heterosexual participants (N = 237) reported their trait mate value. Participants then recorded a video of themselves and received video responses from five opposite-sex peers, each consisting of either positive or negative romantic feedback—forming the manipulation (popularity: from low to high). Afterwards, we measured participants’ attitudes to traditional gender roles, casual sex, minimum wage and healthcare, and implicit sexual and political attitudes.

Results

Unpopular men reported less support for casual sex than popular men. There was no main effect on women. Unpopular men had lower positive affect than popular men, and in turn men with lower positive affect reported less support for casual sex and for increasing the minimum wage and access to healthcare than men with higher positive affect. Unpopular low mate-value women reported more support for casual sex than popular low mate-value women. Unpopular men of low and average mate value reported less support for casual sex than popular men of low and average mate value. There was no effect on average mate-value women and high mate-value women and men.

Conclusions

Changes in positive affect due to dating popularity influence some of men’s, but not women’s, socio-political attitudes, and trait mate value moderates the effects of popularity on attitudes to casual sex.

目的将价值观与几种社会政治态度相结合。然而,一个人在交配市场上的约会受欢迎程度会改变一个人在该市场上自我感知的伴侣价值。我们测试了约会的流行是否也会因此改变社会政治态度,以及特质伴侣价值是否会调节这种影响。方法异性参与者(N = 237)报告了它们的性状配偶值。然后,参与者录制了一段自己的视频,并收到了五名异性同伴的视频回应,每个人都有积极或消极的浪漫反馈——形成了操纵(受欢迎程度:从低到高)。之后,我们测量了参与者对传统性别角色、随意性行为、最低工资和医疗保健的态度,以及隐含的性和政治态度。结果不受欢迎的男性对随意性行为的支持率低于受欢迎的男人。对妇女没有主要影响。不受欢迎的男性的积极影响低于受欢迎的女性,而积极影响较低的男性对随意性行为、提高最低工资和获得医疗保健的支持也低于积极影响较高的男性。不受欢迎的低伴侣价值女性比受欢迎的高伴侣价值女性更支持随意性行为。据报道,与伴侣价值低且平均的受欢迎男性相比,伴侣价值低而平均的不受欢迎男性对随意性行为的支持更少。对平均配偶价值女性、高配偶价值女性和男性没有影响。结论约会受欢迎程度导致的积极影响的变化影响了一些男性(而不是女性)的社会政治态度,而特质伴侣价值观调节了受欢迎程度对随意性行为态度的影响。
{"title":"Changes in Positive Affect Due to Popularity in an Experimental Dating Context Influence Some of Men’s, but Not Women’s, Socio-Political Attitudes","authors":"Francesca R. Luberti,&nbsp;Khandis R. Blake,&nbsp;Robert C. Brooks","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00188-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00188-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Trait mate value covaries with several socio-political attitudes. One’s dating popularity in a mating market can, however, shift one’s self-perceived mate value in that market. We tested whether dating popularity could therefore also shift socio-political attitudes, and whether trait mate value could moderate this effect.</p><h3>Method</h3><p>Heterosexual participants (<i>N</i> = 237) reported their trait mate value. Participants then recorded a video of themselves and received video responses from five opposite-sex peers, each consisting of either positive or negative romantic feedback—forming the manipulation (popularity: from low to high). Afterwards, we measured participants’ attitudes to traditional gender roles, casual sex, minimum wage and healthcare, and implicit sexual and political attitudes.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Unpopular men reported less support for casual sex than popular men. There was no main effect on women. Unpopular men had lower positive affect than popular men, and in turn men with lower positive affect reported less support for casual sex and for increasing the minimum wage and access to healthcare than men with higher positive affect. Unpopular low mate-value women reported more support for casual sex than popular low mate-value women. Unpopular men of low and average mate value reported less support for casual sex than popular men of low and average mate value. There was no effect on average mate-value women and high mate-value women and men.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Changes in positive affect due to dating popularity influence some of men’s, but not women’s, socio-political attitudes, and trait mate value moderates the effects of popularity on attitudes to casual sex.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44529449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
An introduction to the Special Issue on “Sports Science: Evolutionary Perspectives and Biological Mechanisms” “体育科学:进化视角与生物学机制”特刊简介
IF 1.6 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2022-02-17 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00187-7
Davide Ponzi

The goal of the special issue on “Sports science: evolutionary perspectives and biological mechanisms” was to build a bridge to help the development of a coherent and unifying approach to the study of sport science within an evolutionary framework. By focusing specifically on the biological and psychological dynamics of sport performance and competition, we asked if sports can be used to study the evolution of human behavior, biology and psychology. Likewise, we asked whether this evolutionary approach could improve our understandings of the physical and psychological limits of human athletic performance and health.

“体育科学:进化视角和生物学机制”特刊的目标是搭建一座桥梁,帮助在进化框架内制定连贯统一的体育科学研究方法。通过特别关注体育表现和比赛的生物学和心理学动态,我们询问体育是否可以用于研究人类行为、生物学和心理学的进化。同样,我们询问这种进化方法是否可以提高我们对人类运动表现和健康的生理和心理极限的理解。
{"title":"An introduction to the Special Issue on “Sports Science: Evolutionary Perspectives and Biological Mechanisms”","authors":"Davide Ponzi","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00187-7","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00187-7","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The goal of the special issue on “Sports science: evolutionary perspectives and biological mechanisms” was to build a bridge to help the development of a coherent and unifying approach to the study of sport science within an evolutionary framework. By focusing specifically on the biological and psychological dynamics of sport performance and competition, we asked if sports can be used to study the evolution of human behavior, biology and psychology. Likewise, we asked whether this evolutionary approach could improve our understandings of the physical and psychological limits of human athletic performance and health.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-02-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00187-7.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48471960","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1