首页 > 最新文献

Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology最新文献

英文 中文
The Cocksure Conundrum: How Evolution Created a Gendered Currency of Corporate Overconfidence 自信的难题:进化如何创造了一种企业过度自信的性别货币
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-08-19 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00197-5
Richard Ronay, William W. Maddux, William von Hippel

Biological differences between men and women mandate that women’s obligatory investment in reproduction is significantly greater than that of men. As a result, women have evolved to be the “choosier” of the two sexes and men have evolved to compete for female choice. To the degree that overconfidence is an effective tool for attracting mates and driving away competitors, greater competition among men suggests that they should express more overconfidence than women. Thus, sexual selection may be the primary reason why overconfidence is typically more pronounced in men than it is in women. Sexual selection may also be a distal, causal factor in what we describe as a cult of overconfidence pervading modern organizations and institutions. Whereas overconfidence was once regulated and constrained by features of ancestral life, levels of social mobility and accountability in contemporary society and modern organizations make it increasingly difficult to keep this gendered bias in check.

男女之间的生物学差异决定了妇女在生殖方面的强制性投资明显大于男子。因此,女性进化成了两性中的“挑剔者”,而男性进化成了争夺女性选择的竞争者。在某种程度上,过度自信是吸引伴侣和赶走竞争对手的有效工具,男性之间更大的竞争表明,他们应该比女性表现出更多的过度自信。因此,性选择可能是过度自信在男性中比在女性中更明显的主要原因。性选择也可能是一个遥远的因果因素,我们称之为现代组织和机构中普遍存在的过度自信崇拜。尽管过度自信曾经受到祖先生活特征的制约和约束,但当代社会和现代组织中的社会流动性和问责制水平使得控制这种性别偏见变得越来越困难。
{"title":"The Cocksure Conundrum: How Evolution Created a Gendered Currency of Corporate Overconfidence","authors":"Richard Ronay,&nbsp;William W. Maddux,&nbsp;William von Hippel","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00197-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00197-5","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Biological differences between men and women mandate that women’s obligatory investment in reproduction is significantly greater than that of men. As a result, women have evolved to be the “choosier” of the two sexes and men have evolved to compete for female choice. To the degree that overconfidence is an effective tool for attracting mates and driving away competitors, greater competition among men suggests that they should express more overconfidence than women. Thus, sexual selection may be the primary reason why overconfidence is typically more pronounced in men than it is in women. Sexual selection may also be a distal, causal factor in what we describe as a cult of overconfidence pervading modern organizations and institutions. Whereas overconfidence was once regulated and constrained by features of ancestral life, levels of social mobility and accountability in contemporary society and modern organizations make it increasingly difficult to keep this gendered bias in check.\u0000</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"557 - 578"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00197-5.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41774495","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Fertility predicts self-development-oriented competitiveness in naturally cycling women but not hormonal contraceptive users 生育率预测自然骑自行车的女性以自我发展为导向的竞争力,而不是激素避孕药使用者
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-08-16 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00198-4
Lindsie C. Arthur, Khandis R. Blake

Objective

A growing body of research has begun investigating the relationship between hormones and female competitiveness. Many researchers have focused on the effect of the menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptives. Despite many attempts at understanding hormone-behavior associations, contradictory findings have made it difficult to determine the existence of true effects. The aim of the current research was to use a robust methodological design to investigate the effect of fertility probability on four competitive orientations in naturally cycling women and hormonal contraceptive users.

Methods

Using a longitudinal diary study with over 3,900 observations from 21 countries, we explore the effect of fertility probability on four self-report competitive orientations after controlling for menstruation: self-developmental competition, hyper competitiveness, competition avoidance, and lack of interest toward competition.

Results

Using Bayesian estimation for ordinal mixed models, we found that fertility probability was associated with an increase in self-development competitiveness amongst naturally cycling women but not hormonal contraceptive users. We also found weak evidence that hormonal contraceptive users show reduced interest in competing compared to naturally cycling women. There were no other robust effects of fertility or hormonal contraceptive use.

Conclusions

These results suggest that fertility probability is associated with increased fluctuations in self-development competitive motivation and that hormonal contraceptives interfere with this effect. This research contributes to the growing body of literature suggesting that hormonal contraceptives may influence psychology and behavior by disrupting evolved hormonal mechanisms.

越来越多的研究开始调查荷尔蒙与女性竞争力之间的关系。许多研究人员关注月经周期和激素避孕药的影响。尽管许多人试图理解激素行为的关联,但矛盾的发现使得很难确定是否存在真正的影响。目前研究的目的是使用一种稳健的方法设计来调查生育概率对自然循环女性和激素避孕使用者四种竞争取向的影响。方法采用一项来自21个国家的3900多项观察的纵向日记研究,探讨生育概率对月经控制后四种自我报告竞争取向的影响:自我发展竞争、过度竞争、回避竞争和对竞争缺乏兴趣。结果使用有序混合模型的贝叶斯估计,我们发现自然循环女性的生育概率与自我发展竞争力的提高有关,但与激素避孕药使用者无关。我们还发现,与自然骑自行车的女性相比,激素避孕药使用者对竞争的兴趣降低的证据不足。生育或使用激素避孕没有其他强有力的影响。结论这些结果表明,生育概率与自我发展竞争动机的波动增加有关,激素避孕药会干扰这种影响。这项研究有助于越来越多的文献表明,激素避孕药可能通过破坏进化的激素机制来影响心理和行为。
{"title":"Fertility predicts self-development-oriented competitiveness in naturally cycling women but not hormonal contraceptive users","authors":"Lindsie C. Arthur,&nbsp;Khandis R. Blake","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00198-4","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00198-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>\u0000Objective\u0000</h3><p>\u0000A growing body of research has begun investigating the relationship between hormones and female competitiveness. Many researchers have focused on the effect of the menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptives. Despite many attempts at understanding hormone-behavior associations, contradictory findings have made it difficult to determine the existence of true effects. The aim of the current research was to use a robust methodological design to investigate the effect of fertility probability on four competitive orientations in naturally cycling women and hormonal contraceptive users.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Using a longitudinal diary study with over 3,900 observations from 21 countries, we explore the effect of fertility probability on four self-report competitive orientations after controlling for menstruation: self-developmental competition, hyper competitiveness, competition avoidance, and lack of interest toward competition.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>Using Bayesian estimation for ordinal mixed models, we found that fertility probability was associated with an increase in self-development competitiveness amongst naturally cycling women but not hormonal contraceptive users. We also found weak evidence that hormonal contraceptive users show reduced interest in competing compared to naturally cycling women. There were no other robust effects of fertility or hormonal contraceptive use.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>These results suggest that fertility probability is associated with increased fluctuations in self-development competitive motivation and that hormonal contraceptives interfere with this effect. This research contributes to the growing body of literature suggesting that hormonal contraceptives may influence psychology and behavior by disrupting evolved hormonal mechanisms.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"489 - 519"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00198-4.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44029749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Gender and Context-Specific Effects of Vocal Dominance and Trustworthiness on Leadership Decisions 声音优势和可信度对领导决策的性别和情境特定影响
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-07-30 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00194-8
Christoph Schild, Elisa Braunsdorf, Katharina Steffens, Franka Pott, Julia Stern

Objective

The evolutionary-contingency hypothesis, which suggests that preferences for leaders are context-dependent, has found relatively consistent support from research investigating leadership decisions based on facial pictures. Here, we test whether these results transfer to leadership decisions based on voice recordings. We examined how dominance and trustworthiness perceptions relate to leadership decisions in wartime and peacetime contexts and whether effects differ by a speaker’s gender. Further, we investigate two cues that might be related to leadership decisions, as well as dominance and trustworthiness perceptions: voice pitch and strength of regional accent.

Methods

We conducted a preregistered online study with 125 raters and recordings of 120 speakers (61 men, 59 women) from different parts in Germany. Raters were randomly distributed into four rating conditions: dominance, trustworthiness, hypothetical vote (wartime) and hypothetical vote (peacetime).

Results

We find that dominant speakers were more likely to be voted for in a wartime context while trustworthy speakers were more likely to be voted for in a peacetime context. Voice pitch functions as a main cue for dominance perceptions, while strength of regional accent functions as a main cue for trustworthiness perceptions.

Conclusions

This study adds to a stream of research that suggests that (a) people’s voices contain important information based on which we form social impressions and (b) we prefer different types of leaders across different contexts. Future research should disentangle effects of gender bias in leadership decisions and investigate underlying mechanisms that influence how people’s voices contribute to achieving social status.

目的进化偶然性假说认为,对领导者的偏好取决于背景,该假说从调查基于面部照片的领导决策的研究中得到了相对一致的支持。在这里,我们测试这些结果是否会转化为基于语音记录的领导决策。我们研究了在战时和和平时期,主导地位和可信度认知与领导决策的关系,以及说话者的性别是否会产生不同的影响。此外,我们还调查了两个可能与领导决策以及支配地位和可信度认知有关的线索:音高和地区口音的强度。方法我们对125名评分者进行了一项预先注册的在线研究,并对来自德国不同地区的120名演讲者(61名男性,59名女性)进行了录音。评分者被随机分为四个评分条件:支配地位、可信度、假设投票(战时)和假设投票(和平时期)。结果我们发现,在战争背景下,占主导地位的演讲者更有可能被投票支持,而在和平时期,值得信赖的演讲者更容易被投票支持。音高是优势感知的主要线索,而地区口音的强度是可信度感知的主要提示。结论这项研究补充了一系列研究,这些研究表明:(a)人们的声音包含重要信息,我们据此形成社会印象;(b)我们更喜欢不同背景下不同类型的领导者。未来的研究应该理清性别偏见在领导决策中的影响,并调查影响人们的声音如何有助于实现社会地位的潜在机制。
{"title":"Gender and Context-Specific Effects of Vocal Dominance and Trustworthiness on Leadership Decisions","authors":"Christoph Schild,&nbsp;Elisa Braunsdorf,&nbsp;Katharina Steffens,&nbsp;Franka Pott,&nbsp;Julia Stern","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00194-8","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00194-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>The evolutionary-contingency hypothesis, which suggests that preferences for leaders are context-dependent, has found relatively consistent support from research investigating leadership decisions based on facial pictures. Here, we test whether these results transfer to leadership decisions based on voice recordings. We examined how dominance and trustworthiness perceptions relate to leadership decisions in wartime and peacetime contexts and whether effects differ by a speaker’s gender. Further, we investigate two cues that might be related to leadership decisions, as well as dominance and trustworthiness perceptions: voice pitch and strength of regional accent.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>We conducted a preregistered online study with 125 raters and recordings of 120 speakers (61 men, 59 women) from different parts in Germany. Raters were randomly distributed into four rating conditions: dominance, trustworthiness, hypothetical vote (wartime) and hypothetical vote (peacetime).</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We find that dominant speakers were more likely to be voted for in a wartime context while trustworthy speakers were more likely to be voted for in a peacetime context. Voice pitch functions as a main cue for dominance perceptions, while strength of regional accent functions as a main cue for trustworthiness perceptions.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>This study adds to a stream of research that suggests that (a) people’s voices contain important information based on which we form social impressions and (b) we prefer different types of leaders across different contexts. Future research should disentangle effects of gender bias in leadership decisions and investigate underlying mechanisms that influence how people’s voices contribute to achieving social status.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"538 - 556"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00194-8.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48252888","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
When Toughness Begets Respect: Dominant Individuals Gain Prestige and Leadership By Facilitating Intragroup Conflict Resolution 当坚韧开始受到尊重:强势个体通过促进团队内部冲突解决获得声望和领导力
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-07-29 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6
Joey T. Cheng, Nathan A. Dhaliwal, Miranda A. Too

Objective

Why do dominant leaders rise to power via the popular vote? This research tests whether when people feel threatened by intra-group disorder they desire stronger, more dominant leaders.

Methods

Participants (N = 1,026) read a vignette that depicts a within-group norm violation. We then used a between-subjects design to randomly assign participants to a specific version of the vignette in which (a) a focal target individual in the scenario varied in their dominance (punitiveness: from no to moderate to strong); and (b) the local group faced little or substantial intra-group conflict and disorder (threat: from low to high). Following this, participants reported how much they endorse the target individuals as leader and the individual’s perceived prestige.

Results

We find that intra-group conflict motivates a psychology that favors the rise of dominant leaders: Highly punitive individuals (seen as highly dominant) are endorsed as leaders when in-group threat is high, but comparably disfavored when threat is low. Under low threat, non-punitive individuals (who are seen as less dominant) are endorsed as leaders. Subsequent analyses reveal that these shifts in leader preferences are explained by corresponding changes in prestige. Under conditions of high threat, dominance confers prestige, whereas under low threat, dominance suppresses prestige. Tests of mediation further show that the effect of dominance on increased leader support under high threat is mediated by prestige.

Conclusions

In contexts of threat, such as internal disorder, dominant leaders are favored and gain prestige, owing to their perceived ability to supply benefits such as in mediating internal conflicts.

为什么占主导地位的领导人通过普选上台?这项研究测试了当人们感到受到群体内部障碍的威胁时,他们是否渴望更强大、更具统治力的领导者。方法参与者(N = 1026)读取描绘组内规范违反的小插曲。然后,我们使用受试者之间的设计,将参与者随机分配到一个特定版本的小插曲中,其中(a)场景中的焦点目标个体的主导地位各不相同(惩罚程度:从否到中等再到强烈);以及(b)当地群体几乎没有或实质上面临群体内部冲突和混乱(威胁:从低到高)。在此之后,参与者报告了他们对目标个人作为领导者的认可程度以及个人的声望。结果我们发现,群体内冲突激发了一种倾向于主导型领导者崛起的心理:当群体内威胁较高时,高度惩罚性的个人(被视为高度主导型)被认可为领导者,但当威胁较低时,则相对不受欢迎。在低威胁下,非惩罚性个人(被视为不那么占主导地位)被认可为领导者。随后的分析表明,领导者偏好的这些变化是由声望的相应变化来解释的。在高威胁条件下,支配地位赋予声望,而在低威胁条件下支配地位压制声望。调解测试进一步表明,在高威胁下,主导地位对增加领导人支持的影响是由声望调解的。结论在内部混乱等威胁环境中,占主导地位的领导人因其在调解内部冲突等方面提供利益的能力而受到青睐并获得声望。
{"title":"When Toughness Begets Respect: Dominant Individuals Gain Prestige and Leadership By Facilitating Intragroup Conflict Resolution","authors":"Joey T. Cheng,&nbsp;Nathan A. Dhaliwal,&nbsp;Miranda A. Too","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective\u0000</h3><p>Why do dominant leaders rise to power via the popular vote? This research tests whether when people feel threatened by intra-group disorder they desire stronger, more dominant leaders.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Participants (<i>N</i> = 1,026) read a vignette that depicts a within-group norm violation. We then used a between-subjects design to randomly assign participants to a specific version of the vignette in which (a) a focal target individual in the scenario varied in their dominance (punitiveness: from no to moderate to strong); and (b) the local group faced little or substantial intra-group conflict and disorder (threat: from low to high). Following this, participants reported how much they endorse the target individuals as leader and the individual’s perceived prestige.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We find that intra-group conflict motivates a psychology that favors the rise of dominant leaders: Highly punitive individuals (seen as highly dominant) are endorsed as leaders when in-group threat is high, but comparably disfavored when threat is low. Under low threat, non-punitive individuals (who are seen as less dominant) are endorsed as leaders. Subsequent analyses reveal that these shifts in leader preferences are explained by corresponding changes in prestige. Under conditions of high threat, dominance confers prestige, whereas under low threat, dominance suppresses prestige. Tests of mediation further show that the effect of dominance on increased leader support under high threat is mediated by prestige.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>In contexts of threat, such as internal disorder, dominant leaders are favored and gain prestige, owing to their perceived ability to supply benefits such as in mediating internal conflicts.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"383 - 406"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00196-6.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42113154","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting 你所看到的可能就是你想要的:在自然环境中复制配偶
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-07-27 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9
Ryan C. Anderson, Ashleigh A. Armstrong
{"title":"What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting","authors":"Ryan C. Anderson, Ashleigh A. Armstrong","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 1","pages":"296 - 312"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"52893523","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting 你看到的可能是你想要的:在自然环境中复制伴侣
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-07-27 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9
Ryan C. Anderson, Ashleigh A. Armstrong

Abstract

Objectives

The phenomenon of mate copying (MC) suggests that an individual’s romantic desirability varies, to some degree, as a function of their romantic experiences. By manipulating relationship history, this research aimed to determine whether male desirability varied similarly for static stimuli (photographs) as it did for dynamic stimuli (short clips). Here we present 2 studies examining this idea.

Methods

In both studies heterosexual women aged 18–34 were recruited from social media and evaluated the desirability of men, before and after being given information about the level of relationship experience the men had previously had (either ‘none’, ‘some’, or ‘lots’).

Results

In Study 1 (N = 278), we employed static images of men and Study 2 (N = 66) was methodologically similar but employed dynamic stimuli. Study 1 indicated that a man’s desirability to heterosexual women increased when it was revealed that he had a moderate amount of relationship experience but decreased if he had none. Results from Study 2 suggested that desirability was not affected by an individual’s relationship experience.

Conclusions

MC was indicated when stimuli were static, but not when they were dynamic. These results suggest that MC may be a phenomenon that exists to a greater extent when stimuli are less informative.

摘要目的伴侣复制现象表明,一个人的浪漫欲望在一定程度上随着他们的浪漫经历而变化。通过操纵关系史,这项研究旨在确定男性对静态刺激(照片)的渴望是否与对动态刺激(短片)的渴望相似。在这里,我们提出了两项研究来检验这一观点。方法在这两项研究中,年龄在18-34岁的异性恋女性都是从社交媒体上招募的,并在获得关于男性以前有过的关系经历水平的信息(“没有”、“一些”或“很多”)之前和之后评估男性的可取性 = 278),我们采用了男性的静态图像和研究2(N = 66)在方法上相似,但采用了动态刺激。研究1表明,当男性有适度的恋爱经历时,他对异性恋女性的好感度会增加,但如果没有恋爱经历,好感度会降低。研究2的结果表明,可取性不受个人关系经历的影响。结论sMC在静态刺激时显示,而在动态刺激时不显示。这些结果表明,当刺激信息较少时,MC可能是一种更大程度上存在的现象。
{"title":"What You See May Be What You Want: Mate Copying in a Natural Setting","authors":"Ryan C. Anderson,&nbsp;Ashleigh A. Armstrong","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h2>Abstract\u0000</h2><div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>The phenomenon of mate copying (MC) suggests that an individual’s romantic desirability varies, to some degree, as a function of their romantic experiences. By manipulating relationship history, this research aimed to determine whether male desirability varied similarly for static stimuli (photographs) as it did for dynamic stimuli (short clips). Here we present 2 studies examining this idea.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>In both studies heterosexual women aged 18–34 were recruited from social media and evaluated the desirability of men, before and after being given information about the level of relationship experience the men had previously had (either ‘none’, ‘some’, or ‘lots’).</p><h3>Results</h3><p>In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 278), we employed static images of men and Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 66) was methodologically similar but employed dynamic stimuli. Study 1 indicated that a man’s desirability to heterosexual women increased when it was revealed that he had a moderate amount of relationship experience but decreased if he had none. Results from Study 2 suggested that desirability was not affected by an individual’s relationship experience.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>MC was indicated when stimuli were static, but not when they were dynamic. These results suggest that MC may be a phenomenon that exists to a greater extent when stimuli are less informative.</p></div></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 3","pages":"296 - 312"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-07-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00193-9.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50517652","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mu opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) moderates the influence of perceived parental attention on social support seeking Mu阿片受体基因(OPRM1)调节父母关注对社会支持寻求的影响
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-06-08 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w
Shaofeng Zheng, Keiko Ishii, Takahiko Masuda, Masahiro Matsunaga, Yasuki Noguchi, Hidenori Yamasue, Yohsuke Ohtsubo

Objective

Caring, sensitive parenting is known to be associated with higher levels of engagement in support-seeking behaviors among children and young adolescents. However, no study has yet explored the role of perceived parental attention in social support seeking in early adulthood. Growing evidence suggests that the µ-opioid receptor gene polymorphism (OPRM1 A118G) moderates one’s responsiveness to social environments. Prompted by the differential susceptibility theory of gene–environment interaction, the present study examined whether the OPRM1 polymorphism would moderate the association between perceived parental attention and social support seeking in early adulthood.

Methods

Six hundred and twenty Japanese undergraduate students self-reported the amount of attention they subjectively perceived their parents to have given them during childhood and completed scales that assess support-seeking behaviors. Clippings of their fingernails were collected for genotyping.

Results

The results showed that the interactive effect of perceived parental attention and OPRM1 genotypes on social support seeking was significant. Specifically, perceived parental attention only significantly and positively predicted OPRM1 GG carriers’ social support seeking in response to stress.

Conclusions

The findings support the view that OPRM1 polymorphism moderates the association between early experiences and developmental outcomes.

众所周知,关爱、敏感的育儿方式与儿童和青少年更高程度的寻求支持行为有关。然而,目前还没有研究探讨父母关注在成年早期寻求社会支持中的作用。越来越多的证据表明,µ-阿片受体基因多态性(OPRM1 A118G)调节了一个人对社会环境的反应。在基因-环境相互作用的差异易感性理论的推动下,本研究检验了OPRM1多态性是否会调节成年早期感知到的父母关注和寻求社会支持之间的关联。方法620名日本大学生自我报告他们主观上认为父母在童年时期给予他们的关注程度,并完成评估寻求支持行为的量表。采集他们指甲的剪片进行基因分型。结果父母关注感与OPRM1基因型对社会支持寻求的交互作用显著。具体而言,感知到的父母关注仅显著且积极地预测了OPRM1 GG携带者对压力的社会支持寻求。结论研究结果支持了OPRM1多态性调节早期经历和发育结果之间关系的观点。
{"title":"Mu opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) moderates the influence of perceived parental attention on social support seeking","authors":"Shaofeng Zheng,&nbsp;Keiko Ishii,&nbsp;Takahiko Masuda,&nbsp;Masahiro Matsunaga,&nbsp;Yasuki Noguchi,&nbsp;Hidenori Yamasue,&nbsp;Yohsuke Ohtsubo","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Caring, sensitive parenting is known to be associated with higher levels of engagement in support-seeking behaviors among children and young adolescents. However, no study has yet explored the role of perceived parental attention in social support seeking in early adulthood. Growing evidence suggests that the µ-opioid receptor gene polymorphism (<i>OPRM1</i> A118G) moderates one’s responsiveness to social environments. Prompted by the differential susceptibility theory of gene–environment interaction, the present study examined whether the <i>OPRM1</i> polymorphism would moderate the association between perceived parental attention and social support seeking in early adulthood.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Six hundred and twenty Japanese undergraduate students self-reported the amount of attention they subjectively perceived their parents to have given them during childhood and completed scales that assess support-seeking behaviors. Clippings of their fingernails were collected for genotyping.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>The results showed that the interactive effect of perceived parental attention and <i>OPRM1</i> genotypes on social support seeking was significant. Specifically, perceived parental attention only significantly and positively predicted <i>OPRM1</i> GG carriers’ social support seeking in response to stress.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>The findings support the view that <i>OPRM1</i> polymorphism moderates the association between early experiences and developmental outcomes.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 3","pages":"281 - 295"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-06-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.1007/s40750-022-00192-w.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46689411","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Human Females as a Dispersal-Egalitarian Species: A Hypothesis about Women and Status 作为分散的平等主义物种的人类女性:关于女性和地位的假设
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-05-21 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00191-x
Joyce F. Benenson

Objectives

A paradox exists in research on girls and women. On the one hand, they behave in a more egalitarian fashion than their male counterparts. On the other hand, status increases their own and their children’s survival.

Methods

Evidence from non-human primates can help reconcile these findings. In species that do not reside with female kin for life, females are relatively egalitarian and individualistic. They typically do not cooperate or engage in direct competition and exhibit little tolerance for status differentials.

Results and Conclusions

Women follow this pattern. While a husband’s status and her female relatives’ support enhance a woman’s status and reproductive success, her own actions too influence her access to resources and allies. Evidence on girls’ and women’s same-sex competition and quests for status supports the hypothesis that human females inhabit dispersal-egalitarian communities in which competition is avoided, an egalitarian ethos prevails, competitive behavior is disguised, and status differentials are not tolerated.

目的女孩与妇女研究中存在着一个悖论。一方面,她们的行为比男性更加平等。另一方面,地位增加了他们自己和孩子的生存。方法来自非人类灵长类动物的证据可以帮助调和这些发现。在不与女性亲属终身居住的物种中,女性相对平等且个人主义。他们通常不合作或直接竞争,对地位差异几乎没有容忍度。结果与结论女性遵循这种模式。虽然丈夫的地位和女性亲属的支持提高了女性的地位和生育成功率,但她自己的行为也影响了她获得资源和盟友的机会。关于女孩和妇女的同性竞争和对地位的追求的证据支持这样一种假设,即人类女性生活在分散的平等主义社区中,在这个社区中,竞争被避免,平等主义精神盛行,竞争行为被掩盖,地位差异是不可容忍的。
{"title":"Human Females as a Dispersal-Egalitarian Species: A Hypothesis about Women and Status","authors":"Joyce F. Benenson","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00191-x","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00191-x","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objectives</h3><p>A paradox exists in research on girls and women. On the one hand, they behave in a more egalitarian fashion than their male counterparts. On the other hand, status increases their own and their children’s survival.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Evidence from non-human primates can help reconcile these findings. In species that do not reside with female kin for life, females are relatively egalitarian and individualistic. They typically do not cooperate or engage in direct competition and exhibit little tolerance for status differentials.</p><h3>Results and Conclusions</h3><p>Women follow this pattern. While a husband’s status and her female relatives’ support enhance a woman’s status and reproductive success, her own actions too influence her access to resources and allies. Evidence on girls’ and women’s same-sex competition and quests for status supports the hypothesis that human females inhabit dispersal-egalitarian communities in which competition is avoided, an egalitarian ethos prevails, competitive behavior is disguised, and status differentials are not tolerated.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 4","pages":"433 - 460"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-05-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48103980","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Associations Between Sexual Desire and Within-Individual Testosterone and Cortisol in Men and Women 性欲与男性和女性个体睾酮和皮质醇之间的关系
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-04-18 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00184-w
Kevin A Rosenfield, Heather Self, Talia Shirazi, Rodrigo Cardenes, Justin Carré, Triana Ortiz, Khytam Dawood, David A. Puts

Objective

The dual-hormone hypothesis (DHH) posits that some effects of testosterone on human behavior and psychology related to status-seeking are moderated by cortisol, such that they are stronger when cortisol levels are low. In support of the DHH, studies have found that cortisol negatively moderated the relationship between testosterone and such traits as status-seeking and interest in uncommitted sex. Others indicate a positive moderating influence of cortisol in some cases. Here, we test whether two psychosexual indices—sexual desire and sociosexuality—meet the expectations of the DHH in a large sample of men and women.

Method

646 women and 185 men attended lab sessions during which they provided saliva samples for hormonal analysis and responded to the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory-Revised and the Sexual Desire Inventory (180 women and 43 men returned for a second session approximately two months later). We quantified salivary hormone concentrations using ELISA and assessed within- and between-participant effects of hormones on psychosexual measures with mixed-effects models.

Results

We observed a positive interaction between within-subjects cortisol and testosterone in models of sexual desire in both men and women. For women, these effects emerged in models of general sexual desire and in models of the dyadic desire subscale and were robust to many analytical configurations. For men, the effects were limited to models of solitary desire, but were also robust to alternative analyses. We present data to quantify our risks of both type I and type II error.

Conclusions

Some of our results contrast with usual dual-hormone hypothesis predictions of negative interactions between testosterone and cortisol. We suggest several potential explanations for these results, including a positive feedback loop whereby elevated testosterone prompts increases in sexual desire and behavior, necessitating cortisol-induced mobilization of energy stores.

目的双重激素假说(DHH)认为,睾酮对人类行为和心理的一些影响与地位寻求有关,它们受到皮质醇的调节,因此当皮质醇水平较低时,它们会更强。为了支持DHH,研究发现皮质醇对睾酮和寻求地位和对非承诺性行为感兴趣等特征之间的关系起到了负面调节作用。另一些则表明在某些情况下皮质醇具有积极的调节作用。在这里,我们在大量男性和女性样本中测试了两个性心理指数——性欲和社会性行为——是否符合DHH的预期。方法646名女性和185名男性参加了实验室会议,期间他们提供唾液样本进行激素分析,并对修订的社会性取向量表和性欲量表做出了回应(180名女性和43名男性在大约两个月后返回进行第二次会议)。我们使用ELISA定量唾液激素浓度,并使用混合效应模型评估激素对性心理测量的参与者内部和参与者之间的影响。结果在男性和女性的性欲模型中,我们观察到受试者体内皮质醇和睾酮之间存在积极的相互作用。对于女性来说,这些影响出现在一般性欲模型和二元欲望分量表模型中,并且在许多分析配置中都是稳健的。对于男性来说,这种影响仅限于孤独欲望的模型,但也适用于替代分析。我们提供数据来量化I型和II型错误的风险。结论我们的一些结果与通常的双激素假说预测睾酮和皮质醇之间的负相互作用形成对比。我们对这些结果提出了几个潜在的解释,包括一个正反馈回路,即睾酮升高会促使性欲和行为增加,从而需要皮质醇诱导的能量储备动员。
{"title":"Associations Between Sexual Desire and Within-Individual Testosterone and Cortisol in Men and Women","authors":"Kevin A Rosenfield,&nbsp;Heather Self,&nbsp;Talia Shirazi,&nbsp;Rodrigo Cardenes,&nbsp;Justin Carré,&nbsp;Triana Ortiz,&nbsp;Khytam Dawood,&nbsp;David A. Puts","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00184-w","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00184-w","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>The dual-hormone hypothesis (DHH) posits that some effects of testosterone on human behavior and psychology related to status-seeking are moderated by cortisol, such that they are stronger when cortisol levels are low. In support of the DHH, studies have found that cortisol negatively moderated the relationship between testosterone and such traits as status-seeking and interest in uncommitted sex. Others indicate a positive moderating influence of cortisol in some cases. Here, we test whether two psychosexual indices—sexual desire and sociosexuality—meet the expectations of the DHH in a large sample of men and women.</p><h3>Method</h3><p>646 women and 185 men attended lab sessions during which they provided saliva samples for hormonal analysis and responded to the Sociosexual Orientation Inventory-Revised and the Sexual Desire Inventory (180 women and 43 men returned for a second session approximately two months later). We quantified salivary hormone concentrations using ELISA and assessed within- and between-participant effects of hormones on psychosexual measures with mixed-effects models.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>We observed a positive interaction between within-subjects cortisol and testosterone in models of sexual desire in both men and women. For women, these effects emerged in models of general sexual desire and in models of the dyadic desire subscale and were robust to many analytical configurations. For men, the effects were limited to models of solitary desire, but were also robust to alternative analyses. We present data to quantify our risks of both type I and type II error.</p><h3>Conclusions</h3><p>Some of our results contrast with usual dual-hormone hypothesis predictions of negative interactions between testosterone and cortisol. We suggest several potential explanations for these results, including a positive feedback loop whereby elevated testosterone prompts increases in sexual desire and behavior, necessitating cortisol-induced mobilization of energy stores.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 2","pages":"156 - 178"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41416908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Voice Changes Across the Menstrual Cycle in Response to Masculinized and Feminized Man and Woman 男性化和女性化男性和女性在月经周期中的声音变化
IF 1.6 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, BIOLOGICAL Pub Date : 2022-04-15 DOI: 10.1007/s40750-022-00190-y
Irena Pavela Banai, Robert P. Burriss, Nataša Šimić

Objective

Research has demonstrated that men prefer women’s voices with higher pitch and that women’s voices recorded at high compared to low fertility phases of the menstrual cycle are rated as more attractive. These findings suggest that voice conveys information relevant to reproductive success. Because voice attractiveness is higher during the high fertility phase and voice pitch positively predicts attractiveness ratings, it has been hypothesized that cyclical changes in vocal attractiveness are driven by changes in voice pitch. However, attempts to detect acoustic changes have produced mixed results. With the higher degree of ecological validity achieved by including social context (simulated interactions with men and women) and by recording voice in the three phases of menstrual cycles, the present study addresses limitations of previous research.

Methods

Forty-eight naturally cycling women were recorded during the menstrual, late follicular (high fertility), and luteal phases while leaving voice messages to masculinized and femininized men and women.

Results

No cycle-related changes in pitch and pitch variability for the recordings directed to masculinized and femininized men and women were detected. By including relationship status as predictor in additional models, higher-order interaction effects showed that single and partnered women displayed opposite cycle-related pitch changes directed only to women, but not men.

Conclusion

The cycle-related voice changes found in the present study do not support the hypothesis that cyclic pitch variations represent an adaptive mechanism for attracting partners. We discuss cyclic changes in voice pitch in relation to intrasexual competition by taking into an account that the present study is likely underpowered for adequate testing of the complex higher-order interactions.

客观研究表明,男性更喜欢女性音调更高的声音,而在月经周期的高生育期与低生育期相比,女性的声音被认为更具吸引力。这些发现表明,声音传达了与生殖成功相关的信息。因为在高生育率阶段,声音吸引力更高,而音高正预测吸引力评级,因此有人假设,声音吸引力的周期性变化是由音高的变化驱动的。然而,检测声学变化的尝试产生了喜忧参半的结果。通过包括社会背景(模拟与男性和女性的互动)和记录月经周期三个阶段的声音,本研究实现了更高程度的生态有效性,解决了以往研究的局限性。方法记录48名自然周期女性在月经期、卵泡晚期(高生育率)和黄体期的情况,同时给男性化和女性化的男性和女性留下语音信息。结果在针对男性化和女性化男性和女性的录音中,没有检测到音高和音高变异性的周期相关变化。通过在其他模型中纳入关系状态作为预测因素,高阶交互效应表明,单身和伴侣女性表现出与周期相反的音调变化,这些变化只针对女性,而不针对男性。结论本研究中发现的与周期相关的语音变化不支持周期音高变化代表吸引伴侣的适应机制的假设。我们讨论了音调的周期性变化与性内竞争的关系,考虑到本研究可能缺乏对复杂的高阶相互作用进行充分测试的能力。
{"title":"Voice Changes Across the Menstrual Cycle in Response to Masculinized and Feminized Man and Woman","authors":"Irena Pavela Banai,&nbsp;Robert P. Burriss,&nbsp;Nataša Šimić","doi":"10.1007/s40750-022-00190-y","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s40750-022-00190-y","url":null,"abstract":"<div><h3>Objective</h3><p>Research has demonstrated that men prefer women’s voices with higher pitch and that women’s voices recorded at high compared to low fertility phases of the menstrual cycle are rated as more attractive. These findings suggest that voice conveys information relevant to reproductive success. Because voice attractiveness is higher during the high fertility phase and voice pitch positively predicts attractiveness ratings, it has been hypothesized that cyclical changes in vocal attractiveness are driven by changes in voice pitch. However, attempts to detect acoustic changes have produced mixed results. With the higher degree of ecological validity achieved by including social context (simulated interactions with men and women) and by recording voice in the three phases of menstrual cycles, the present study addresses limitations of previous research.</p><h3>Methods</h3><p>Forty-eight naturally cycling women were recorded during the menstrual, late follicular (high fertility), and luteal phases while leaving voice messages to masculinized and femininized men and women.</p><h3>Results</h3><p>No cycle-related changes in pitch and pitch variability for the recordings directed to masculinized and femininized men and women were detected. By including relationship status as predictor in additional models, higher-order interaction effects showed that single and partnered women displayed opposite cycle-related pitch changes directed only to women, but not men.</p><h3>Conclusion</h3><p>The cycle-related voice changes found in the present study do not support the hypothesis that cyclic pitch variations represent an adaptive mechanism for attracting partners. We discuss cyclic changes in voice pitch in relation to intrasexual competition by taking into an account that the present study is likely underpowered for adequate testing of the complex higher-order interactions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":7178,"journal":{"name":"Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology","volume":"8 2","pages":"238 - 262"},"PeriodicalIF":1.6,"publicationDate":"2022-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45687908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1