首页 > 最新文献

Behavioral Ecology最新文献

英文 中文
Phenology and environmental determinants of explosive breeding in gliding treefrogs: diel timing of rainfall matters 滑翔树蛙爆炸性繁殖的物候学和环境决定因素:降雨的昼夜计时问题
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad072
Brandon A Güell, Karen M Warkentin
Abstract The influence of abiotic and biotic factors on the temporal pattern of calling and breeding for many temperate anurans is well understood. However, few studies have documented patterns of reproduction in explosive-breeding tropical frogs or incorporated multiple environmental factors in their analyses, especially across multiple breeding seasons. We combine long-term natural history observations and automated data collection methods with boosted regression tree (BRT) analysis to determine the phenology and determinants of explosive breeding in the gliding treefrog, Agalychnis spurrelli. We monitored breeding for a total of 418 days across three breeding seasons and determined the relative importance of several environmental factors on the probability of calling and breeding activity. Our study population of A. spurrelli on Costa Rica’s Osa Peninsula forms breeding aggregations up to 11 times per year during 1–2-day long explosive-breeding events, from late May to mid-September. Calling and breeding activity are strongly and positively related to accumulated rainfall during the previous 24 and 48–24 h before, particularly rainfall during the afternoon and evening. Day-of-year, days since breeding occurred, and lunar phase also influence reproductive activity. This study provides the first description and analysis of the phenology and factors that predict explosive breeding in A. spurrelli and illustrates the value of using automated data collection paired with BRTs for the analysis of complex ecological data. La influencia de factores abióticos y bióticos en el patrón temporal de cantos y reproducción de muchos anuros de zonas templadas es bien conocida. Sin embargo, pocos estudios han documentado patrones de reproducción en ranas tropicales, que se reproducen de manera explosiva, o han incorporado múltiples factores ambientales en sus análisis, especialmente a lo largo de múltiples temporadas de reproducción. Combinamos observaciones de historia natural a largo plazo y métodos automatizados de recolectar datos con análisis de árboles de regresión y técnicas de remuestreo (‘boosted regression trees’, BRT) para determinar la fenología y los determinantes de la reproducción explosiva en la rana planeadora, Agalychnis spurrelli. Monitoreamos la reproducción durante un total de 418 días a través de tres temporadas de reproducción y determinamos la importancia relativa de varios factores ambientales en la probabilidad de cantos y reproducción. Nuestra población de estudio de A. spurrelli en la Península de Osa en Costa Rica forma agregaciones reproductivas hasta 11 veces por año, los eventos de reproducción explosiva tienen 1 a 2 días de duración, desde finales de mayo hasta mediados de septiembre. Los cantos y la reproducción están fuerte y positivamente relacionados con la acumulación de lluvia en las 24 y 48 a 24 horas anteriores, particularmente durante la tarde y la noche. El día del año, los días desde la última reproducción, y la fase l
摘要:非生物和生物因素对温带无尾目动物鸣叫和繁殖时间格局的影响已经得到了很好的认识。然而,很少有研究记录了爆炸性繁殖的热带青蛙的繁殖模式,或者在分析中纳入了多种环境因素,特别是在多个繁殖季节。本研究结合长期自然历史观察和自动数据收集方法,结合增强回归树(BRT)分析,确定滑翔树蛙(Agalychnis spurrelli)的物候特征和爆炸性繁殖的决定因素。我们在三个繁殖季节共监测了418天的繁殖,并确定了几种环境因素对鸣叫概率和繁殖活动的相对重要性。我们在哥斯达黎加奥萨半岛的研究种群在每年5月下旬至9月中旬的1 - 2天的爆炸性繁殖事件中形成多达11次的繁殖聚集。鸣叫和繁殖活动与前24小时和前48 ~ 24 h的累积降雨量呈显著正相关,尤其是午后和傍晚的降雨量。一年中的哪一天,从繁殖开始的天数,以及月相也会影响生殖活动。本研究首次描述和分析了沙刺草的物候特征和预测爆炸性繁殖的因素,并说明了将自动化数据收集与brt相结合用于复杂生态数据分析的价值。工厂的流行性疾病abióticos y bióticos en el patrón时间的传染性疾病和reproducción许多疾病的流行性疾病和疾病的传染性疾病。在过去的一年里,电影工作室和纪录片赞助人一起制作了reproducción和热带电影,制作了爆炸性的电影,也制作了múltiples和环境电影制作商一起制作了análisis,特别是制作了大型的múltiples临时电影制作商reproducción。结合自然历史观测数据(大型广场),通过 (árboles)和 (regresión)通过 (fenología)和 (BRT)通过 (fenología)和 ()确定因素(reproducción)爆炸,planeadora, Agalychnis spurrelli。通过确定各种环境因素的重要性相对关系以及环境因素的可能性(reproducción),监测数据为reproducción持续时间和总时间(418)días。新发现población关于a . spurrelli和Península关于Osa和Costa Rica forma agregaciones reproductivas hasta 11 veces pte año,关于reproducción explosiva tienen 1 a 2 días de duración,关于mayo hasta mediados de september。Los cantos y la reproducción están futerte y positivtivente relados con la acumulación de lluvia en las 24 y 48和24小时前,特别是durante la tarde y la noche。El día del año, los días desde la última reproducción, y la fase lunar tamambien influyla reproducción。Este estudio es la primera descripción y análisis de la fenología y los factores que predicen la reproducción exploitva . A. spurrelli . el valor de utilizar la recolección de datos automatizada junto con BRT para el análisis de datos ecológicos complexos。
{"title":"Phenology and environmental determinants of explosive breeding in gliding treefrogs: diel timing of rainfall matters","authors":"Brandon A Güell, Karen M Warkentin","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad072","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad072","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The influence of abiotic and biotic factors on the temporal pattern of calling and breeding for many temperate anurans is well understood. However, few studies have documented patterns of reproduction in explosive-breeding tropical frogs or incorporated multiple environmental factors in their analyses, especially across multiple breeding seasons. We combine long-term natural history observations and automated data collection methods with boosted regression tree (BRT) analysis to determine the phenology and determinants of explosive breeding in the gliding treefrog, Agalychnis spurrelli. We monitored breeding for a total of 418 days across three breeding seasons and determined the relative importance of several environmental factors on the probability of calling and breeding activity. Our study population of A. spurrelli on Costa Rica’s Osa Peninsula forms breeding aggregations up to 11 times per year during 1–2-day long explosive-breeding events, from late May to mid-September. Calling and breeding activity are strongly and positively related to accumulated rainfall during the previous 24 and 48–24 h before, particularly rainfall during the afternoon and evening. Day-of-year, days since breeding occurred, and lunar phase also influence reproductive activity. This study provides the first description and analysis of the phenology and factors that predict explosive breeding in A. spurrelli and illustrates the value of using automated data collection paired with BRTs for the analysis of complex ecological data. La influencia de factores abióticos y bióticos en el patrón temporal de cantos y reproducción de muchos anuros de zonas templadas es bien conocida. Sin embargo, pocos estudios han documentado patrones de reproducción en ranas tropicales, que se reproducen de manera explosiva, o han incorporado múltiples factores ambientales en sus análisis, especialmente a lo largo de múltiples temporadas de reproducción. Combinamos observaciones de historia natural a largo plazo y métodos automatizados de recolectar datos con análisis de árboles de regresión y técnicas de remuestreo (‘boosted regression trees’, BRT) para determinar la fenología y los determinantes de la reproducción explosiva en la rana planeadora, Agalychnis spurrelli. Monitoreamos la reproducción durante un total de 418 días a través de tres temporadas de reproducción y determinamos la importancia relativa de varios factores ambientales en la probabilidad de cantos y reproducción. Nuestra población de estudio de A. spurrelli en la Península de Osa en Costa Rica forma agregaciones reproductivas hasta 11 veces por año, los eventos de reproducción explosiva tienen 1 a 2 días de duración, desde finales de mayo hasta mediados de septiembre. Los cantos y la reproducción están fuerte y positivamente relacionados con la acumulación de lluvia en las 24 y 48 a 24 horas anteriores, particularmente durante la tarde y la noche. El día del año, los días desde la última reproducción, y la fase l","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"144 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134946465","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Large fitness benefits of social nesting in a small carpenter bee 小木蜂群居筑巢的巨大健康效益
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-05 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad077
Michael Mikát, Sandra M Rehan
Abstract Facultatively social insects are an optimal model group for the study of the emergence of cooperation between individuals. Factors influencing the fitness benefits of social nesting are still debated. Nonreproductive subordinates can benefit from indirect fitness benefits due to increasing reproductive success of related individuals or direct fitness benefits due to direct future reproduction. Here, we studied the costs and benefits of social nesting in the small carpenter bee Ceratina albosticta. From demographic data and within-nest relatedness, we obtained key parameters for assessing the fitness of solitary females, social primaries, and social secondaries. C. albosticta were found to usually mate with one male and multiple mating is not common although exists at low frequencies. Social nests usually contain two females, which were found to be related (full sisters), but also some females were unrelated to each other. Patterns of parentage from microsatellite loci revealed that only one female reproduces in social nests. Our results show that relatedness, per capita brood productivity, and offspring survival strongly increase the fitness benefits of social nesting strategies. Social secondaries, when related to the social primaries, have higher inclusive fitness than solitary females, but unrelated social nesting females had no indirect fitness and much reduced inclusive fitness compared to solitary females. Interestingly, average fitness benefits of the social secondary were higher than solitary females. This study provides important empirical data on the costs and benefits of sociality in a facultatively social bee and sets the stage for future comparative studies.
兼性社会性昆虫是研究个体间合作出现的最佳模型群体。影响群居筑巢对健康益处的因素仍存在争议。无生殖能力的下属可以获得间接的适应性利益,因为相关个体的繁殖成功率增加,或者直接的适应性利益,因为直接的未来繁殖。在这里,我们研究了小木蜂(Ceratina albosticta)社会筑巢的成本和收益。从人口统计数据和巢内亲缘关系中,我们获得了评估独居雌性、社会初级和社会次级适合度的关键参数。白纹夜蛾通常与一只雄虫交配,多次交配的频率较低,但并不常见。社会巢穴通常包含两只雌性,它们被发现是有关系的(姐妹),但也有一些雌性彼此无关。来自微卫星位点的亲代模式显示,只有一只雌性在社会巢穴中繁殖。研究结果表明,亲缘关系、人均窝产率和后代存活率显著提高了群居筑巢策略的适应性效益。当与社会初级巢相关时,社会次级巢的整体适合度高于独居雌巢,但与独居雌巢无关的社会次级巢没有间接适合度,其整体适合度明显低于独居雌巢。有趣的是,群居配偶的平均健康收益高于独居配偶。本研究为兼性社会性蜜蜂的社会性成本和收益提供了重要的实证数据,并为未来的比较研究奠定了基础。
{"title":"Large fitness benefits of social nesting in a small carpenter bee","authors":"Michael Mikát, Sandra M Rehan","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad077","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad077","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Facultatively social insects are an optimal model group for the study of the emergence of cooperation between individuals. Factors influencing the fitness benefits of social nesting are still debated. Nonreproductive subordinates can benefit from indirect fitness benefits due to increasing reproductive success of related individuals or direct fitness benefits due to direct future reproduction. Here, we studied the costs and benefits of social nesting in the small carpenter bee Ceratina albosticta. From demographic data and within-nest relatedness, we obtained key parameters for assessing the fitness of solitary females, social primaries, and social secondaries. C. albosticta were found to usually mate with one male and multiple mating is not common although exists at low frequencies. Social nests usually contain two females, which were found to be related (full sisters), but also some females were unrelated to each other. Patterns of parentage from microsatellite loci revealed that only one female reproduces in social nests. Our results show that relatedness, per capita brood productivity, and offspring survival strongly increase the fitness benefits of social nesting strategies. Social secondaries, when related to the social primaries, have higher inclusive fitness than solitary females, but unrelated social nesting females had no indirect fitness and much reduced inclusive fitness compared to solitary females. Interestingly, average fitness benefits of the social secondary were higher than solitary females. This study provides important empirical data on the costs and benefits of sociality in a facultatively social bee and sets the stage for future comparative studies.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"3 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134946267","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Foraging is prioritized over nestmate rescue in desert ants and pupae are rescued more than adults 在沙漠蚂蚁中,觅食优先于拯救巢穴,蛹比成年蚂蚁更容易获救
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad083
Adi Bar, Tomer Gilad, Doaa Massad, Asaph Ferber, Dan Ben-Ezra, Daniela Segal, Susanne Foitzik, Inon Scharf
Abstract Social animals, and ants, in particular, exhibit a range of cooperative behaviors. One such behavior is the rescue of group members, which cannot return to the nest by themselves. However, if several group members need to be rescued, how do ants prioritize whom to save first? Furthermore, when food is offered in parallel, do ants prioritize feeding over rescuing? We studied the rescue behavior of the desert ant Cataglyphis niger. Workers invest more time in rescuing pupae than adult workers, perhaps because the value of brood is higher than that of older workers serving as foragers. Specific rescue behaviors, pulling the trapped individual or digging around it, differed when directed toward adults or pupae: rescuing workers more often pulled pupae whereas they dug more around trapped adults. Rescuing workers did not prioritize living individuals over dead ones or intact workers over injured ones indicating that trapped individuals were recognized chemically rather than by their morphology or behavior. Finally, workers prioritized foraging over rescuing, perhaps because fewer workers specialize in rescue behavior than in foraging. Our analysis indeed revealed that fewer workers both foraged and rescued trapped workers than expected by chance. In conclusion, ants that rescue others exhibit a complex set of behaviors, with varying attention and specific behaviors targeted at different individuals, perhaps according to the colony’s needs. Our study is important for emphasizing a relatively neglected aspect of sociality (rescue of group members) and demonstrates that the attentions of rescues differ based on the trapped nestmate’s life stage.
社会性动物,尤其是蚂蚁,表现出一系列的合作行为。其中一种行为是拯救不能自己返回巢穴的群体成员。然而,如果有几个群体成员需要救助,蚂蚁如何优先救助谁?此外,当同时提供食物时,蚂蚁会优先考虑进食而不是救援吗?我们研究了沙漠蚂蚁的救援行为。工蜂比成年工蜂花更多的时间抢救蛹,可能是因为孵出的幼虫比充当觅食者的老工蜂更有价值。具体的救援行为,拉动被困个体或在其周围挖掘,在针对成虫或蛹时有所不同:救援工人更多地拉动蛹,而他们更多地在被困成虫周围挖掘。救援人员并没有优先考虑活着的人而不是死去的人,也没有优先考虑完好无损的人而不是受伤的人,这表明被困的人是通过化学方式而不是通过形态或行为来识别的。最后,工蚁优先考虑觅食而不是救援,这可能是因为专门从事救援行为的工蚁比专门从事觅食的工蚁要少。我们的分析确实表明,觅食和营救被困工人的工人比偶然预期的要少。总之,拯救其他蚂蚁的蚂蚁表现出一套复杂的行为,可能根据群体的需要,对不同个体有不同的注意力和特定的行为。我们的研究强调了社会性的一个相对被忽视的方面(群体成员的救援),并证明了救援的关注是根据被困鸟巢的生命阶段而不同的。
{"title":"Foraging is prioritized over nestmate rescue in desert ants and pupae are rescued more than adults","authors":"Adi Bar, Tomer Gilad, Doaa Massad, Asaph Ferber, Dan Ben-Ezra, Daniela Segal, Susanne Foitzik, Inon Scharf","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad083","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad083","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Social animals, and ants, in particular, exhibit a range of cooperative behaviors. One such behavior is the rescue of group members, which cannot return to the nest by themselves. However, if several group members need to be rescued, how do ants prioritize whom to save first? Furthermore, when food is offered in parallel, do ants prioritize feeding over rescuing? We studied the rescue behavior of the desert ant Cataglyphis niger. Workers invest more time in rescuing pupae than adult workers, perhaps because the value of brood is higher than that of older workers serving as foragers. Specific rescue behaviors, pulling the trapped individual or digging around it, differed when directed toward adults or pupae: rescuing workers more often pulled pupae whereas they dug more around trapped adults. Rescuing workers did not prioritize living individuals over dead ones or intact workers over injured ones indicating that trapped individuals were recognized chemically rather than by their morphology or behavior. Finally, workers prioritized foraging over rescuing, perhaps because fewer workers specialize in rescue behavior than in foraging. Our analysis indeed revealed that fewer workers both foraged and rescued trapped workers than expected by chance. In conclusion, ants that rescue others exhibit a complex set of behaviors, with varying attention and specific behaviors targeted at different individuals, perhaps according to the colony’s needs. Our study is important for emphasizing a relatively neglected aspect of sociality (rescue of group members) and demonstrates that the attentions of rescues differ based on the trapped nestmate’s life stage.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"21 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135548353","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Sex-dependent integration of ornamentation, personality, morphology, and life history 纹饰、个性、形态和生活史的性别依赖性整合
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad085
David López-Idiáquez, Céline Teplitsky, Amélie Fargevieille, María Del Rey-Granado, Raphaëlle Mercier Gauthier, Christophe de Franceschi, Anne Charmantier, Claire Doutrelant, Denis Réale
Abstract Phenotypic integration can be defined as the patterns and strength of the covariances between traits in an organism. The pace of life syndrome (POLS) hypothesis provides a testable case of phenotypic integration as it predicts that traits that mediate the trade-off between current and future reproduction should have coevolved with the slow-fast life-history continuum and may thus covary across individuals of a population. Although the POLS hypothesis has received increasing attention over the last decade, there is a need for investigating whether POLS are sex-specific and whether ornamental traits can be included within the POLS framework. We used 9 years of data to describe the integration patterns of ornamental coloration, personality, morphology, and life history in blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) and whether they differed between males and females. With that aim, we fitted multivariate mixed models separating the among- from the within-individual covariances. We found that the overall integration between the studied traits was weak, but our results suggested some sex-specific covariances at the among- and within-individual levels. Finally, using structural equation models (SEM), we tested for the presence of trait modules (i.e., covariances between traits involved in the same biological function) within each sex. SEMs suggested the presence of a morphological module but no modules for coloration or behavioral traits. Also, results obtained from the SEM did not support the POLS hypothesis. Overall, this work highlights the importance of considering sex when studying phenotypic integration and the relevance of SEM to test POLS hypothesis.
表型整合可以定义为生物体中性状之间协方差的模式和强度。生活节奏综合症(POLS)假说为表型整合提供了一个可测试的案例,因为它预测了调节当前和未来繁殖之间权衡的性状应该与慢速生活史连续体共同进化,因此可能在种群个体之间发生协变。虽然POLS假说在过去十年中受到越来越多的关注,但有必要研究POLS是否具有性别特异性以及观赏性状是否可以包括在POLS框架内。本文利用9年的数据,描述了蓝山雀(Cyanistes caeruleus)的观赏色彩、个性、形态和生活史的整合模式,以及它们在雌雄之间是否存在差异。为此,我们拟合了多变量混合模型,将个体间协方差与个体内协方差分开。我们发现所研究性状之间的整体整合较弱,但我们的结果表明在个体之间和个体内部水平上存在一些性别特异性的协方差。最后,使用结构方程模型(SEM),我们测试了每个性别中性状模块(即涉及相同生物功能的性状之间的协方差)的存在。sem表明存在形态模块,但没有颜色或行为特征模块。此外,从扫描电镜得到的结果不支持POLS假设。总的来说,这项工作强调了在研究表型整合时考虑性别的重要性,以及扫描电镜与测试POLS假设的相关性。
{"title":"Sex-dependent integration of ornamentation, personality, morphology, and life history","authors":"David López-Idiáquez, Céline Teplitsky, Amélie Fargevieille, María Del Rey-Granado, Raphaëlle Mercier Gauthier, Christophe de Franceschi, Anne Charmantier, Claire Doutrelant, Denis Réale","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad085","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad085","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Phenotypic integration can be defined as the patterns and strength of the covariances between traits in an organism. The pace of life syndrome (POLS) hypothesis provides a testable case of phenotypic integration as it predicts that traits that mediate the trade-off between current and future reproduction should have coevolved with the slow-fast life-history continuum and may thus covary across individuals of a population. Although the POLS hypothesis has received increasing attention over the last decade, there is a need for investigating whether POLS are sex-specific and whether ornamental traits can be included within the POLS framework. We used 9 years of data to describe the integration patterns of ornamental coloration, personality, morphology, and life history in blue tits (Cyanistes caeruleus) and whether they differed between males and females. With that aim, we fitted multivariate mixed models separating the among- from the within-individual covariances. We found that the overall integration between the studied traits was weak, but our results suggested some sex-specific covariances at the among- and within-individual levels. Finally, using structural equation models (SEM), we tested for the presence of trait modules (i.e., covariances between traits involved in the same biological function) within each sex. SEMs suggested the presence of a morphological module but no modules for coloration or behavioral traits. Also, results obtained from the SEM did not support the POLS hypothesis. Overall, this work highlights the importance of considering sex when studying phenotypic integration and the relevance of SEM to test POLS hypothesis.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"60 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135548345","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Cumulative experience influences contest investment in a social fish 累积经验影响社会鱼的竞争投资
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad078
Macie D Benincasa, Ryan L Earley, Ian M Hamilton
Abstract When animals live in long-term groups, the potential for conflict is high. Conflict is costly, so an individual’s decision to engage depends on the information it has about the costs and benefits of fighting. One source of information could be past contest experience, where previous winners/losers typically become more likely to win/lose in the future. However, repeated interactions can familiarize individuals with conflict and provide opportunities to learn to become better fighters, regardless of outcome. We explored how individuals integrate information from previous contests to inform future encounters in a group-living fish, Neolamprologus pulcher. We gave contestants single, reinforcing, and contradictory experiences and measured behavior and post-fight water-borne levels of androgenic steroids (testosterone, 11-ketotestosterone). Contradictory outcomes were associated with reduced investment in fighting. More fighting experience did not lead to greater investment in fighting, as consecutive losses resulted in reduced aggression. Also, there was no effect of fighting treatment on water-borne androgen concentrations. Interestingly, there were sex differences in which behaviors were influenced by experience, and in whether body mass was associated with androgen concentrations, which could indicate that males and females vary in how perceived fighting ability changes with contest experience. Our data reveal the complex ways in which repeated experiences can alter an individual’s propensity to invest in conflict. Repeated interactions associated with predictable changes in behavior can contribute to rank stability in groups and our results indicate that whether and how they do depend on the quality and quantity of interactions plus individual factors such as sex.
当动物长期群居时,发生冲突的可能性很高。冲突是代价高昂的,所以一个人参与冲突的决定取决于他对战斗成本和收益的了解。一个信息来源可能是过去的比赛经验,以前的赢家/输家通常更有可能在未来赢/输。然而,反复的互动可以使个体熟悉冲突,并提供学习成为更好的战士的机会,而不管结果如何。我们探索了个体如何整合以前竞争的信息,以便为群居鱼(Neolamprologus pulcher)的未来相遇提供信息。我们给参赛者单一的、强化的和矛盾的经历,并测量了他们的行为和赛后水中携带的雄激素水平(睾酮,11-酮睾酮)。矛盾的结果与战斗投入的减少有关。更多的战斗经验并没有导致更多的战斗投入,因为连续的损失导致侵略性的减少。此外,战斗处理对水中雄激素浓度没有影响。有趣的是,在行为受经验影响方面存在性别差异,在体重是否与雄激素浓度相关方面也存在性别差异,这可能表明男性和女性在感知战斗能力随比赛经验变化的方式上存在差异。我们的数据揭示了重复的经历可以改变个人投入冲突的倾向的复杂方式。与可预测的行为变化相关的重复互动有助于群体中的等级稳定性,我们的研究结果表明,它们是否以及如何依赖于互动的质量和数量以及性别等个人因素。
{"title":"Cumulative experience influences contest investment in a social fish","authors":"Macie D Benincasa, Ryan L Earley, Ian M Hamilton","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad078","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad078","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract When animals live in long-term groups, the potential for conflict is high. Conflict is costly, so an individual’s decision to engage depends on the information it has about the costs and benefits of fighting. One source of information could be past contest experience, where previous winners/losers typically become more likely to win/lose in the future. However, repeated interactions can familiarize individuals with conflict and provide opportunities to learn to become better fighters, regardless of outcome. We explored how individuals integrate information from previous contests to inform future encounters in a group-living fish, Neolamprologus pulcher. We gave contestants single, reinforcing, and contradictory experiences and measured behavior and post-fight water-borne levels of androgenic steroids (testosterone, 11-ketotestosterone). Contradictory outcomes were associated with reduced investment in fighting. More fighting experience did not lead to greater investment in fighting, as consecutive losses resulted in reduced aggression. Also, there was no effect of fighting treatment on water-borne androgen concentrations. Interestingly, there were sex differences in which behaviors were influenced by experience, and in whether body mass was associated with androgen concentrations, which could indicate that males and females vary in how perceived fighting ability changes with contest experience. Our data reveal the complex ways in which repeated experiences can alter an individual’s propensity to invest in conflict. Repeated interactions associated with predictable changes in behavior can contribute to rank stability in groups and our results indicate that whether and how they do depend on the quality and quantity of interactions plus individual factors such as sex.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"20 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135770996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Golden mimics use multiple defenses to counter generalist and specialist predators 金色模仿者使用多种防御来对抗通才和专精掠食者
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-23 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad076
Stano Pekár, Martin J Whiting, Marie E Herberstein
Abstract Many prey species employ multiple defenses during interactions with predators. Multiple defenses can provide a selective advantage against a single predator at different stages of the interaction or attack, as well as against multiple predator types. However, the efficacy of multiple defenses both during different sequences of an attack and against multiple predator types, remains poorly understood. We measured and classified defensive traits used by five mimics (Müllerian and Batesian) of the myrmecomorphic golden mimicry complex and one non-mimetic species. We then performed predatory trials using two different predators that differed markedly in their body size, trophic specialization, and how they handle prey—one being an ant specialist (spider) and the other a generalist which avoided ants (skink). We identified 12 defensive traits and classified them into four groups (primary, chemical, mechanical, and behavioral), which were strongly correlated. Skinks were much less likely to attack and capture mimics than the ant-eating spider predators. Our results show that multiple defenses (five or six) were used against each predator. The defensive behaviors and features that were most effective against skinks included appendage waving and large body size, whereas the golden “shine” warning signal, large body size, cuticle thickness, and defensive gland size were most effective against spiders. Most defenses appeared to be predator-specific. We conclude that potential prey in the golden mimicry complex have been selected for multiple defenses because of their vulnerability to different predator types and consequently, the efficacy of some of these defenses likely represents a trade-off.
许多被捕食物种在与捕食者的相互作用中采用多种防御手段。多重防御可以在相互作用或攻击的不同阶段提供针对单一捕食者的选择性优势,也可以针对多种捕食者类型。然而,在不同的攻击顺序和对抗多种捕食者类型时,多重防御的功效仍然知之甚少。我们测量并分类了拟态金拟态复合体的5个拟态种(m lererian和Batesian)和1个非拟态种的防御性状。然后,我们用两种不同的捕食者进行了捕食试验,这两种捕食者在体型、营养专业化和处理猎物的方式上都有明显的不同——一种是蚂蚁专家(蜘蛛),另一种是避免蚂蚁的通才(石龙子)。我们确定了12种防御特征,并将它们分为四类(初级、化学、机械和行为),它们之间存在很强的相关性。与吃蚂蚁的蜘蛛掠食者相比,石龙子攻击和捕获模仿物的可能性要小得多。我们的研究结果表明,对每个捕食者都使用了多种防御措施(五到六种)。对石龙子最有效的防御行为和特征是挥舞附属物和大体型,而对蜘蛛最有效的防御行为和特征是金色的“闪光”警告信号、大体型、角质层厚度和防御腺体大小。大多数防御似乎是针对捕食者的。我们得出的结论是,黄金拟态复合体中的潜在猎物被选择用于多种防御,因为它们对不同类型的捕食者的脆弱性,因此,这些防御的有效性可能代表了一种权衡。
{"title":"Golden mimics use multiple defenses to counter generalist and specialist predators","authors":"Stano Pekár, Martin J Whiting, Marie E Herberstein","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad076","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad076","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Many prey species employ multiple defenses during interactions with predators. Multiple defenses can provide a selective advantage against a single predator at different stages of the interaction or attack, as well as against multiple predator types. However, the efficacy of multiple defenses both during different sequences of an attack and against multiple predator types, remains poorly understood. We measured and classified defensive traits used by five mimics (Müllerian and Batesian) of the myrmecomorphic golden mimicry complex and one non-mimetic species. We then performed predatory trials using two different predators that differed markedly in their body size, trophic specialization, and how they handle prey—one being an ant specialist (spider) and the other a generalist which avoided ants (skink). We identified 12 defensive traits and classified them into four groups (primary, chemical, mechanical, and behavioral), which were strongly correlated. Skinks were much less likely to attack and capture mimics than the ant-eating spider predators. Our results show that multiple defenses (five or six) were used against each predator. The defensive behaviors and features that were most effective against skinks included appendage waving and large body size, whereas the golden “shine” warning signal, large body size, cuticle thickness, and defensive gland size were most effective against spiders. Most defenses appeared to be predator-specific. We conclude that potential prey in the golden mimicry complex have been selected for multiple defenses because of their vulnerability to different predator types and consequently, the efficacy of some of these defenses likely represents a trade-off.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"61 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135957734","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The impact of food availability on risk-induced trait responses in prey 食物可得性对猎物风险诱导性状反应的影响
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-23 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad074
Michael J Sheriff, Isabella Mancini, Olivia K Aguiar, Eleanor R DiNuzzo, Sophia Maloney-Buckley, Sam Sonnega, Sarah C Donelan
Abstract Prey respond to predation risk by altering their morphology, physiology, and behavior, responses that may come at a cost to prey foraging and growth. However, their perception of risk may depend upon the environmental context in which the interaction occurs. Here, we examined how food availability influenced prey’s responses to a nonlethal but free-ranging predator. We used an experimental mesocosm set-up of an intertidal system of green crab (Carcinus maenas) predators and dogwhelk snail (Nucella lapillus) prey, with blue mussels as the basal resource. We measured individual Nucella risk-aversion behavior (summed value of their habitat use) and their growth throughout the 28-day experiment. We found that when Nucella were exposed to predation risk, they had greater risk-aversion behavior when food was available as compared to when food was not. We also found that risk-aversion behavior increased over time in all treatments but that individuals exposed to predation risk with food always had greater risk-aversion behavior. Nucella had significantly more growth when provided with food as compared to without. In treatments with food, snails exposed to predation risk had significantly reduced growth compared to those not exposed to risk. In treatments without food, predation risk had no effect on growth. Our results support the hypothesis that greater resource availability significantly increases individual risk responses and the costs of risk avoidance, thus food magnifies risk-induced non-consumptive effects. They provide insights into context-based predation risk effects and how individuals may prioritize safety versus foraging depending upon perceived risk, food availability, and the costs of responding.
猎物通过改变其形态、生理和行为来应对捕食风险,这些反应可能以猎物的觅食和生长为代价。然而,他们对风险的感知可能取决于相互作用发生的环境背景。在这里,我们研究了食物供应如何影响猎物对非致命但自由放养的捕食者的反应。以蓝贻贝为基础资源,建立了绿蟹(Carcinus maenas)捕食者和狗螺(Nucella lapillus)捕食者的潮间带系统的实验中生态系统。在28天的实验中,我们测量了个体Nucella的风险厌恶行为(其栖息地利用的总价值)和它们的生长。我们发现,当Nucella暴露在被捕食的风险中时,它们在有食物时比没有食物时有更大的风险厌恶行为。我们还发现,在所有治疗中,风险厌恶行为随着时间的推移而增加,但暴露于食物捕食风险的个体总是有更大的风险厌恶行为。在提供食物时,Nucella的生长明显高于没有提供食物的情况。在食物处理中,与没有暴露于风险的蜗牛相比,暴露于捕食风险的蜗牛的生长明显降低。在没有食物的处理中,被捕食的风险对生长没有影响。我们的研究结果支持这样的假设,即更大的资源可获得性显著增加了个体的风险反应和风险规避成本,因此食物放大了风险引起的非消费效应。它们提供了基于情境的捕食风险效应的见解,以及个体如何根据感知风险、食物可用性和响应成本优先考虑安全与觅食。
{"title":"The impact of food availability on risk-induced trait responses in prey","authors":"Michael J Sheriff, Isabella Mancini, Olivia K Aguiar, Eleanor R DiNuzzo, Sophia Maloney-Buckley, Sam Sonnega, Sarah C Donelan","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad074","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad074","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Prey respond to predation risk by altering their morphology, physiology, and behavior, responses that may come at a cost to prey foraging and growth. However, their perception of risk may depend upon the environmental context in which the interaction occurs. Here, we examined how food availability influenced prey’s responses to a nonlethal but free-ranging predator. We used an experimental mesocosm set-up of an intertidal system of green crab (Carcinus maenas) predators and dogwhelk snail (Nucella lapillus) prey, with blue mussels as the basal resource. We measured individual Nucella risk-aversion behavior (summed value of their habitat use) and their growth throughout the 28-day experiment. We found that when Nucella were exposed to predation risk, they had greater risk-aversion behavior when food was available as compared to when food was not. We also found that risk-aversion behavior increased over time in all treatments but that individuals exposed to predation risk with food always had greater risk-aversion behavior. Nucella had significantly more growth when provided with food as compared to without. In treatments with food, snails exposed to predation risk had significantly reduced growth compared to those not exposed to risk. In treatments without food, predation risk had no effect on growth. Our results support the hypothesis that greater resource availability significantly increases individual risk responses and the costs of risk avoidance, thus food magnifies risk-induced non-consumptive effects. They provide insights into context-based predation risk effects and how individuals may prioritize safety versus foraging depending upon perceived risk, food availability, and the costs of responding.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"43 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135957733","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Competition for acoustic space in a temperate-forest bird community. 温带森林鸟类群落对声学空间的竞争。
IF 2.5 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-23 eCollection Date: 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad075
Agata Staniewicz, Emilia Sokołowska, Adrianna Muszyńska, Michał Budka

Animals that communicate by acoustic signaling share a common acoustic environment. Birds are particularly vocal examples, using a wide repertoire of broadcast signals for mate attraction and territorial defense. However, interference caused by sounds that overlap in frequency and time can disrupt signal detection and reduce reproductive success. Here, we investigated competition avoidance mechanisms used by the bird community inhabiting a primeval lowland temperate forest in Białowieża, Eastern Poland. We recorded the dawn chorus at 84 locations in early and late spring and calculated dissimilarity indices of the broadcast signals to examine how species with greater song similarities use spatial and temporal partitioning to avoid competition for acoustic space throughout the breeding season. The bird community changed its use of acoustic space throughout the day and season. Birds did not use spatial partitioning of signal space when we looked at recording locations over the whole study period, but they did in a seasonal context, with species more acoustically different than expected by chance recorded at the same point in the same part of the season. Our results also indicate that daily temporal niche partitioning may only occur at certain times before sunrise, with no evidence of large-scale temporal partitioning between species vocalizing during the same 1-min recordings in daytime. These results contribute toward our understanding of the evolution of bird communication and highlight the strategies employed by different species to improve their signal transmission.

通过声音信号进行交流的动物共享一个共同的声音环境。鸟类就是一个特别的声音例子,它们使用广泛的广播信号来吸引配偶和保卫领土。然而,由频率和时间重叠的声音引起的干扰会破坏信号检测,降低繁殖成功率。在此,我们研究了居住在波兰东部Białowieża的原始低地温带森林中的鸟类群落所使用的竞争规避机制。我们在早春和晚春的84个地点记录了黎明合唱,并计算了广播信号的不相似性指数,以研究歌声相似性较大的物种如何利用空间和时间划分来避免整个繁殖季节对声音空间的竞争。鸟类群落在白天和季节改变了对声学空间的利用。当我们在整个研究期间观察记录位置时,鸟类没有使用信号空间的空间划分,但它们在季节背景下使用了信号空间,在季节的同一时间同一地点记录的物种在声音上的差异比预期的要大。我们的研究结果还表明,每天的时间生态位划分可能只发生在日出前的特定时间,没有证据表明在白天相同的1分钟录音中物种之间存在大规模的时间划分。这些结果有助于我们理解鸟类通信的进化,并突出了不同物种改善信号传输的策略。
{"title":"Competition for acoustic space in a temperate-forest bird community.","authors":"Agata Staniewicz, Emilia Sokołowska, Adrianna Muszyńska, Michał Budka","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad075","DOIUrl":"10.1093/beheco/arad075","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Animals that communicate by acoustic signaling share a common acoustic environment. Birds are particularly vocal examples, using a wide repertoire of broadcast signals for mate attraction and territorial defense. However, interference caused by sounds that overlap in frequency and time can disrupt signal detection and reduce reproductive success. Here, we investigated competition avoidance mechanisms used by the bird community inhabiting a primeval lowland temperate forest in Białowieża, Eastern Poland. We recorded the dawn chorus at 84 locations in early and late spring and calculated dissimilarity indices of the broadcast signals to examine how species with greater song similarities use spatial and temporal partitioning to avoid competition for acoustic space throughout the breeding season. The bird community changed its use of acoustic space throughout the day and season. Birds did not use spatial partitioning of signal space when we looked at recording locations over the whole study period, but they did in a seasonal context, with species more acoustically different than expected by chance recorded at the same point in the same part of the season. Our results also indicate that daily temporal niche partitioning may only occur at certain times before sunrise, with no evidence of large-scale temporal partitioning between species vocalizing during the same 1-min recordings in daytime. These results contribute toward our understanding of the evolution of bird communication and highlight the strategies employed by different species to improve their signal transmission.</p>","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"34 6","pages":"1043-1054"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10636732/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134648406","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early-life behavior, survival, and maternal personality in a wild marsupial. 野生有袋动物的早期生活行为、生存和母性性格。
IF 2.5 3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-14 eCollection Date: 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad070
Weliton Menário Costa, Wendy J King, Timothée Bonnet, Marco Festa-Bianchet, Loeske E B Kruuk

Individual behavior varies for many reasons, but how early in life are such differences apparent, and are they under selection? We investigated variation in early-life behavior in a wild eastern gray kangaroo (Macropus giganteus) population, and quantified associations of behavior with early survival. Behavior of young was measured while still in the pouch and as subadults, and survival to weaning was monitored. We found consistent variation between offspring of different mothers in levels of activity at the pouch stage, in flight initiation distance (FID) as subadults, and in subadult survival, indicating similarity between siblings. There was no evidence of covariance between the measures of behavior at the pouch young versus subadult stages, nor of covariance of the early-life behavioral traits with subadult survival. However, there was a strong covariance between FIDs of mothers and those of their offspring tested at different times. Further, of the total repeatability of subadult FID (51.5%), more than half could be attributed to differences between offspring of different mothers. Our results indicate that 1) behavioral variation is apparent at a very early stage of development (still in the pouch in the case of this marsupial); 2) between-mother differences can explain much of the repeatability (or "personality") of juvenile behavior; and 3) mothers and offspring exhibit similar behavioral responses to stimuli. However, 4) we found no evidence of selection via covariance between early-life or maternal behavioral traits and juvenile survival in this wild marsupial.

个体行为的不同有很多原因,但这种差异在生命的早期是明显的,它们是在自然选择下出现的吗?我们研究了野生东部灰袋鼠(Macropus giganteus)种群早期生活行为的变化,并量化了行为与早期生存的关系。幼崽在育儿袋和亚成虫时期的行为被测量,并监测断奶前的存活情况。我们发现不同母亲的后代在育儿袋期的活动水平、亚成虫的飞行起始距离(FID)和亚成虫存活率方面存在一致的差异,这表明兄弟姐妹之间存在相似性。在育儿袋幼体和亚成虫阶段的行为测量之间没有协方差的证据,也没有协方差的早期生活行为特征与亚成虫存活率。然而,在不同时间测试的母亲及其后代的FIDs之间存在很强的协方差。此外,在亚成体FID的总可重复性中(51.5%),超过一半的可归因于不同母亲的后代之间的差异。我们的研究结果表明,1)行为变异在发育的早期阶段就很明显(在这种有袋动物的情况下,还在育儿袋中);2)母亲之间的差异可以解释青少年行为的可重复性(或“个性”);母亲和后代对刺激表现出相似的行为反应。然而,我们没有发现这种野生有袋动物的早期生活或母亲行为特征与幼崽生存之间通过协方差进行选择的证据。
{"title":"Early-life behavior, survival, and maternal personality in a wild marsupial.","authors":"Weliton Menário Costa, Wendy J King, Timothée Bonnet, Marco Festa-Bianchet, Loeske E B Kruuk","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad070","DOIUrl":"10.1093/beheco/arad070","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individual behavior varies for many reasons, but how early in life are such differences apparent, and are they under selection? We investigated variation in early-life behavior in a wild eastern gray kangaroo (<i>Macropus giganteus</i>) population, and quantified associations of behavior with early survival. Behavior of young was measured while still in the pouch and as subadults, and survival to weaning was monitored. We found consistent variation between offspring of different mothers in levels of activity at the pouch stage, in flight initiation distance (FID) as subadults, and in subadult survival, indicating similarity between siblings. There was no evidence of covariance between the measures of behavior at the pouch young versus subadult stages, nor of covariance of the early-life behavioral traits with subadult survival. However, there was a strong covariance between FIDs of mothers and those of their offspring tested at different times. Further, of the total repeatability of subadult FID (51.5%), more than half could be attributed to differences between offspring of different mothers. Our results indicate that 1) behavioral variation is apparent at a very early stage of development (still in the pouch in the case of this marsupial); 2) between-mother differences can explain much of the repeatability (or \"personality\") of juvenile behavior; and 3) mothers and offspring exhibit similar behavioral responses to stimuli. However, 4) we found no evidence of selection via covariance between early-life or maternal behavioral traits and juvenile survival in this wild marsupial.</p>","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"34 6","pages":"1002-1012"},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2023-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10636729/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134648407","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
White-tailed deer responses to acoustic predator cues are contingent upon past land use and contemporary fire regime 白尾鹿对捕食者声音线索的反应取决于过去的土地利用和当代的火灾制度
3区 环境科学与生态学 Q2 BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1093/beheco/arad071
Savannah L Bartel, John C Kilgo, John L Orrock
Abstract Prey can assess the immediate risk of predation by detecting cues of predator presence, and it is expected that prey should invest in costly antipredator behaviors when a cue of predator presence is detected. Features of the habitat in which the cue is detected, such as vegetative concealment, serve as indirect cues of risk and can mediate how prey respond to direct cues of predator presence. Past agricultural land use and contemporary fire regimes are common disturbances that may modify prey perceptions of risk and could, therefore, alter prey responses to direct cues of predator presence. We examined whether the overlap of these two disturbances affected white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus) responses to cues of predator presence by measuring deer vigilance and foraging bout duration in response to coyote (Canis latrans) vocalizations across 20 woodlands that varied in past land use and contemporary fire regime. Frequent fire regimes consistently increased deer visibility to predators across both land-use history contexts. Deer exhibited no behavioral response to the predator cue in habitats containing infrequent fire regimes or agricultural legacies. Deer responded to the cue in frequently burned woodlands without agricultural legacies through increased vigilance and time spent at a foraging location. These findings reveal that land-use legacies and contemporary fire regimes can mediate how prey respond to direct cues of risk. They also suggest that prey may balance the uncertainty associated with cues of predation risk with the urgency of responding to a potential attack by being vigilant and remaining in place.
摘要猎物可以通过发现捕食者存在的线索来评估捕食的即时风险,当发现捕食者存在的线索时,猎物应该投入代价高昂的反捕食行为。发现线索的栖息地特征,如植物隐藏,可以作为危险的间接线索,并可以调节猎物对捕食者存在的直接线索的反应。过去的农业用地和当代的火灾制度是常见的干扰,可能会改变猎物对风险的感知,因此可能会改变猎物对捕食者存在的直接线索的反应。我们研究了这两种干扰的重叠是否会影响白尾鹿(Odocoileus virginianus)对捕食者存在线索的反应,通过测量鹿对土狼(Canis latrans)叫声的警觉性和觅食持续时间,这些林地在过去的土地利用和当代的火灾制度中有所不同。在两种土地使用历史背景下,频繁的火灾持续增加鹿对捕食者的可见度。在不经常发生火灾或农业遗产的栖息地,鹿对捕食者的暗示没有表现出行为反应。在没有农业遗迹的经常被烧毁的林地中,鹿通过提高警惕和在觅食地点花费的时间来对线索做出反应。这些发现表明,土地利用遗产和当代火灾制度可以调节猎物对直接风险线索的反应。他们还认为,猎物可能会平衡与捕食风险线索相关的不确定性,以及通过保持警惕和留在原地来应对潜在攻击的紧迫性。
{"title":"White-tailed deer responses to acoustic predator cues are contingent upon past land use and contemporary fire regime","authors":"Savannah L Bartel, John C Kilgo, John L Orrock","doi":"10.1093/beheco/arad071","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arad071","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Prey can assess the immediate risk of predation by detecting cues of predator presence, and it is expected that prey should invest in costly antipredator behaviors when a cue of predator presence is detected. Features of the habitat in which the cue is detected, such as vegetative concealment, serve as indirect cues of risk and can mediate how prey respond to direct cues of predator presence. Past agricultural land use and contemporary fire regimes are common disturbances that may modify prey perceptions of risk and could, therefore, alter prey responses to direct cues of predator presence. We examined whether the overlap of these two disturbances affected white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus) responses to cues of predator presence by measuring deer vigilance and foraging bout duration in response to coyote (Canis latrans) vocalizations across 20 woodlands that varied in past land use and contemporary fire regime. Frequent fire regimes consistently increased deer visibility to predators across both land-use history contexts. Deer exhibited no behavioral response to the predator cue in habitats containing infrequent fire regimes or agricultural legacies. Deer responded to the cue in frequently burned woodlands without agricultural legacies through increased vigilance and time spent at a foraging location. These findings reveal that land-use legacies and contemporary fire regimes can mediate how prey respond to direct cues of risk. They also suggest that prey may balance the uncertainty associated with cues of predation risk with the urgency of responding to a potential attack by being vigilant and remaining in place.","PeriodicalId":8840,"journal":{"name":"Behavioral Ecology","volume":"35 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135488621","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"环境科学与生态学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Behavioral Ecology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1