首页 > 最新文献

The behavior analyst today最新文献

英文 中文
A decade later: Creating single-subject design graphs with Microsoft Excel 2007™. 十年后:用Microsoft Excel 2007™创建单主题设计图表。
Pub Date : 2008-06-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100655
Joshua K. Pritchard
In Carr and Burkholder's article (1998), they utilized the graphing guidelines recommended in the manuscript preparation checklist in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, however, the most recent recommendations (2000) do not include all of the guidelines mentioned in the first article. Those which are included: Data points do not connect across experimental phase and the y-axis zero value is raised off the abscissa. Absent from the newer recommendations is the criteria that data points do not quite touch the lines between them. Its removal as a criterion indicates that it is no longer a requirement, and as such will be addressed, but indicated as optional, according to the 2000 guidelines. As Carr and Burkholder indicated, the utilization of a spreadsheet to enter, gather, and analyze data will prevent redundant data entry and save time on the part of the behavior analyst. In addition, Excel[TM] appears to be a widely accepted spreadsheet program. In the tradition that Carr and Burkholder began, this tutorial will provide very detailed and easy to follow instructions on how to graph reversal, multi-element, and multiple baseline designs; however, as in the 1998 article, rudimentary understanding of spreadsheet programs is necessary prior to starting. In this tutorial, there will be no need to prepare the toolbar as all instructions will be utilizing default settings of Excel 2007[TM]. Each action step will be followed by a sequence of keyboard shortcuts to illustrate and simplify implementation of the instruction for that step. The keyboard shortcuts consist of the names of the keys in brackets []. Two keys connected by a plus sign within the same bracket indicate that the initial key should be depressed while pressing the second key (e.g., [ALT+T] indicates that one should hold down the Alt button and press the T button). Two bracketed buttons joined by a dash should be pressed in succession (e.g., [ALT+T]-[T]-[P] would indicate that you should hold down the ALT key and press the T key. After releasing these, you should press the T key again, and then press the P key. Anything in braces (e.g.,{Axis Title}) indicate that you should input text. Thus, if you encounter [ALT]-[T]-{Name}-[ENTER], you should press ALT, then press T, then type in the name, then press the ENTER key. When you encounter a word written in all capital letters, that indicates that this is the name of a button, text, or icon you should click or press. CREATING A REVERSAL DESIGN GRAPH Entering Data into the Spreadsheet Data will be staggered across columns by phases. This will provide a means to prevent data connection across phases (without having to manually do this phase by phase). In addition, it will allow the use of Excel Functions individually per phase to create trend lines. Once you have Excel 2007[TM] open, complete the following steps to enter your data. 1. Enter the data for the first phase (e.g., Baseline1) in the first column and click on the cell in the fol
在Carr和Burkholder的文章(1998)中,他们使用了《应用行为分析杂志》(Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis)的手稿准备清单中推荐的绘图指南,然而,最近的建议(2000)并没有包括第一篇文章中提到的所有指南。数据点不跨实验阶段连接,y轴零值偏离横坐标。新的建议中缺少的是数据点与它们之间的线不太接近的标准。根据2000年的指导方针,将其作为一项标准删除表明它不再是一项必要条件,因此将予以处理,但作为可选项。正如Carr和Burkholder指出的那样,使用电子表格来输入、收集和分析数据将防止冗余的数据输入,并节省行为分析师的时间。此外,Excel[TM]似乎是一个被广泛接受的电子表格程序。在Carr和Burkholder开始的传统中,本教程将提供非常详细和易于遵循的关于如何进行图形反转,多元素和多基线设计的说明;但是,与1998年的文章一样,在开始之前必须对电子表格程序有基本的了解。在本教程中,不需要准备工具栏,因为所有指令将使用Excel 2007[TM]的默认设置。每个操作步骤后面都有一系列键盘快捷键,以说明和简化该步骤的指令实现。键盘快捷键由括号[]中的键名组成。在同一括号内由加号连接的两个键表示在按第二个键时应该按下第一个键(例如,[ALT+T]表示应该按住ALT键并按下T键)。用破折号连接的两个括号按钮应该连续按下(例如,[ALT+T]-[T]-[P]表示你应该按住ALT键并按下T键。释放这些后,您应该再次按T键,然后按P键。大括号中的任何内容(例如,{Axis Title})都表明您应该输入文本。因此,如果遇到[ALT]-[T]-{Name}-[ENTER],您应该按ALT,然后按T,然后输入名称,然后按ENTER键。当您遇到一个全大写字母的单词时,这表明这是一个按钮、文本或图标的名称,您应该单击或按下它。将数据输入电子表格数据将按阶段跨栏错开。这将提供一种方法来防止跨阶段的数据连接(而不必逐个阶段手动执行此操作)。此外,它将允许在每个阶段单独使用Excel函数来创建趋势线。打开Excel 2007[TM]后,完成以下步骤输入数据。1. 在第一列中输入第一阶段的数据(例如,基线1),并单击下一行中的单元格。重复此操作,直到输入此阶段的所有数据。{数据}-[输入]2。对于每一个新阶段(如干预、干预2、回归基线等),在下一行和前一列右侧的一列中输入数据。重复每个阶段。[[右箭头]]-{数据}。创建图1。输入完所有数据后,将鼠标指针从左上角单元格拖动到刚刚输入的数据系列的最底部和最右侧。2. 单击Ribbon上的INSERT选项卡,然后单击LINE图形图标(右边第八个图标)。(ALT) -[我][H] 3。将出现不同类型图表的菜单。点击带有标记的线条(将鼠标悬停在每个图形的图片上,它将显示类型)。[L] -[输入]4。一个新的图表将出现在与数据相同的工作表上,同时在功能区中出现一个新部分,标记为“CHART TOOLS”。…
{"title":"A decade later: Creating single-subject design graphs with Microsoft Excel 2007™.","authors":"Joshua K. Pritchard","doi":"10.1037/H0100655","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100655","url":null,"abstract":"In Carr and Burkholder's article (1998), they utilized the graphing guidelines recommended in the manuscript preparation checklist in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, however, the most recent recommendations (2000) do not include all of the guidelines mentioned in the first article. Those which are included: Data points do not connect across experimental phase and the y-axis zero value is raised off the abscissa. Absent from the newer recommendations is the criteria that data points do not quite touch the lines between them. Its removal as a criterion indicates that it is no longer a requirement, and as such will be addressed, but indicated as optional, according to the 2000 guidelines. As Carr and Burkholder indicated, the utilization of a spreadsheet to enter, gather, and analyze data will prevent redundant data entry and save time on the part of the behavior analyst. In addition, Excel[TM] appears to be a widely accepted spreadsheet program. In the tradition that Carr and Burkholder began, this tutorial will provide very detailed and easy to follow instructions on how to graph reversal, multi-element, and multiple baseline designs; however, as in the 1998 article, rudimentary understanding of spreadsheet programs is necessary prior to starting. In this tutorial, there will be no need to prepare the toolbar as all instructions will be utilizing default settings of Excel 2007[TM]. Each action step will be followed by a sequence of keyboard shortcuts to illustrate and simplify implementation of the instruction for that step. The keyboard shortcuts consist of the names of the keys in brackets []. Two keys connected by a plus sign within the same bracket indicate that the initial key should be depressed while pressing the second key (e.g., [ALT+T] indicates that one should hold down the Alt button and press the T button). Two bracketed buttons joined by a dash should be pressed in succession (e.g., [ALT+T]-[T]-[P] would indicate that you should hold down the ALT key and press the T key. After releasing these, you should press the T key again, and then press the P key. Anything in braces (e.g.,{Axis Title}) indicate that you should input text. Thus, if you encounter [ALT]-[T]-{Name}-[ENTER], you should press ALT, then press T, then type in the name, then press the ENTER key. When you encounter a word written in all capital letters, that indicates that this is the name of a button, text, or icon you should click or press. CREATING A REVERSAL DESIGN GRAPH Entering Data into the Spreadsheet Data will be staggered across columns by phases. This will provide a means to prevent data connection across phases (without having to manually do this phase by phase). In addition, it will allow the use of Excel Functions individually per phase to create trend lines. Once you have Excel 2007[TM] open, complete the following steps to enter your data. 1. Enter the data for the first phase (e.g., Baseline1) in the first column and click on the cell in the fol","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"153-161"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472130","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
The Determination of the Conflict Resolution Strategies of University Students that They Use when They Have Conflicts with People. 大学生在与人发生冲突时使用的冲突解决策略的测定。
Pub Date : 2008-06-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100660
Sibel Dincyurek, A. Civelek
According to Cuceloglu (2002:195), general opinion is that; "if individuals have good intentions and know how to speak to each other, there will be no arguments or conflicts". If we look at the same understanding from a different point of view, it is seen that the outcome will be "people in conflict, are people who have bad intentions and who do not know manners". These expressions, might be pronouncing certain good intentions, yet they do not stand for reality. Because, as long as people live together, no matter how good intentions they have or how understanding they are, conflicts and infighting are inevitable. According to Oner (1996:191) people think that "conflict occurs when one ore more people cannot agree on a subject". In some conflict cases, the discourses and behaviours of the individuals in conflict are different from each other, or the individuals perceive them in a different way. Conflicts generally happen when needs, instincts and desires of people counteract. These different needs, different instincts, different points of view and different perceptions cause conflicts. We all live through occasional conflicts for thinking in a different way. This is a part of being human." According to Tastan (2002:1) it will be best to mention two factors causing interpersonal conflicts in people's lives. The reason for young individuals having interpersonal conflicts is the worry to achieve what they wish for themselves, and the ways and diversity of the relationships they have to pursue in order to get along with others. The assertive, self-sufficient, defensive attitude that emerges as a result when individuals act with the urge of satisfying their own needs, causes conflicts both within the individual and between individuals. (www.organizasyon.com) The outbreak reasons of interpersonal conflicts may originate from personal factors like cognition, perception, emotion, non-conscious needs, and communicative skills, or cultural factors, real differences, social and physical environments or the quality of the message given in the communication process (Dokmen, 2004). Conflicts among individuals are naturally inevitable, as they differ from each other, in every aspect of these factors. Conflicts are a normal segment of daily life. Yet, to many, conflict is considered to be negative, as it causes disagreements, stress, social chaos and violence, and moreover, the most significant indication of a good relationship is accepted to be the absence of conflicts (Johnson, 1981). According to Johnson(1981), positive aspects of conflicts are; it helps the individual in knowing themselves, enhancing their awareness about others' characteristics, noticing the relationship problems that they need to solve, and encouraging change, increases energy and motivation for problem solving, making life more interesting, and help find small problems that are perceived as big issues. After all, it is obvious that conflict, can induce constructive results both for the con
根据Cuceloglu(2002:195),普遍的观点是;“如果每个人都有良好的意图,知道如何相互交谈,就不会有争论或冲突。”如果我们从不同的角度来看待同样的理解,可以看到,结果将是“冲突的人,是有不良意图的人,是不懂礼貌的人”。这些表达,可能是在表达某些善意,但它们并不代表现实。因为,只要人们生活在一起,无论他们有多么好的意图,无论他们多么理解,冲突和内斗都是不可避免的。根据Oner(1996:191)的说法,人们认为“当一个或更多的人在一个问题上不能达成一致时,冲突就发生了”。在一些冲突案例中,冲突个体的话语和行为彼此不同,或者个体以不同的方式感知它们。冲突通常发生在人们的需求、本能和欲望相互抵消的时候。这些不同的需求、不同的本能、不同的观点和不同的认知导致了冲突。我们都偶尔会因为不同的思考方式而产生冲突。这是人性的一部分。”根据Tastan(2002:1)的说法,最好提到两个导致人们生活中人际冲突的因素。年轻人发生人际冲突的原因是他们对实现自己愿望的担忧,以及他们为了与他人相处而必须追求的关系的方式和多样性。当个体出于满足自身需求的冲动而行动时,就会出现自信、自给自足和防御的态度,这导致了个体内部和个体之间的冲突。(www.organizasyon.com)人际冲突的爆发原因可能源于认知、感知、情感、非意识需求、沟通技巧等个人因素,也可能源于文化因素、现实差异、社会和物理环境或沟通过程中所传递信息的质量(Dokmen, 2004)。个人之间的冲突自然是不可避免的,因为他们在这些因素的各个方面都是不同的。冲突是日常生活中正常的一部分。然而,对许多人来说,冲突被认为是负面的,因为它会导致分歧、压力、社会混乱和暴力,而且,良好关系的最重要标志是没有冲突(Johnson, 1981)。根据Johnson(1981)的观点,冲突的积极方面是;它帮助个人了解自己,增强他们对他人特征的认识,注意到他们需要解决的关系问题,鼓励改变,增加解决问题的能量和动力,使生活更有趣,并帮助发现被视为大问题的小问题。毕竟,很明显,冲突可以产生建设性的结果,无论是对冲突的人,还是对冲突所涉及的个人或团体。很长一段时间以来,人们都承认冲突是关系中不可避免的特征,冲突的解决决定了一段关系是否有效。(托马斯,1976)。冲突的发生不是由于一个物体或一个人,有时个人内部可能有冲突。个体内部的冲突可能发生在两个目标同样有吸引力的情况下,或者两个内部需求或动机相互对立的情况下(Atkinson等,1999)。当我们审视人际关系时,冲突有几种定义。在一个例子中,Maurer(1991)将冲突定义为“两方或多方之间不和谐/冲突欲望的不希望的结果”(引自:Sweeney, Carruthers, 1996)。处于冲突中的人们会使用特定的冲突解决方式。…
{"title":"The Determination of the Conflict Resolution Strategies of University Students that They Use when They Have Conflicts with People.","authors":"Sibel Dincyurek, A. Civelek","doi":"10.1037/H0100660","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100660","url":null,"abstract":"According to Cuceloglu (2002:195), general opinion is that; \"if individuals have good intentions and know how to speak to each other, there will be no arguments or conflicts\". If we look at the same understanding from a different point of view, it is seen that the outcome will be \"people in conflict, are people who have bad intentions and who do not know manners\". These expressions, might be pronouncing certain good intentions, yet they do not stand for reality. Because, as long as people live together, no matter how good intentions they have or how understanding they are, conflicts and infighting are inevitable. According to Oner (1996:191) people think that \"conflict occurs when one ore more people cannot agree on a subject\". In some conflict cases, the discourses and behaviours of the individuals in conflict are different from each other, or the individuals perceive them in a different way. Conflicts generally happen when needs, instincts and desires of people counteract. These different needs, different instincts, different points of view and different perceptions cause conflicts. We all live through occasional conflicts for thinking in a different way. This is a part of being human.\" According to Tastan (2002:1) it will be best to mention two factors causing interpersonal conflicts in people's lives. The reason for young individuals having interpersonal conflicts is the worry to achieve what they wish for themselves, and the ways and diversity of the relationships they have to pursue in order to get along with others. The assertive, self-sufficient, defensive attitude that emerges as a result when individuals act with the urge of satisfying their own needs, causes conflicts both within the individual and between individuals. (www.organizasyon.com) The outbreak reasons of interpersonal conflicts may originate from personal factors like cognition, perception, emotion, non-conscious needs, and communicative skills, or cultural factors, real differences, social and physical environments or the quality of the message given in the communication process (Dokmen, 2004). Conflicts among individuals are naturally inevitable, as they differ from each other, in every aspect of these factors. Conflicts are a normal segment of daily life. Yet, to many, conflict is considered to be negative, as it causes disagreements, stress, social chaos and violence, and moreover, the most significant indication of a good relationship is accepted to be the absence of conflicts (Johnson, 1981). According to Johnson(1981), positive aspects of conflicts are; it helps the individual in knowing themselves, enhancing their awareness about others' characteristics, noticing the relationship problems that they need to solve, and encouraging change, increases energy and motivation for problem solving, making life more interesting, and help find small problems that are perceived as big issues. After all, it is obvious that conflict, can induce constructive results both for the con","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"215-233"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472219","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 16
Relational Responding in Parents. 父母的关系反应。
Pub Date : 2008-06-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100659
Amy R. Murrell, K. Wilson, Cicely T. LaBorde, Chad E. Drake, L. J. Rogers
Parenting is perceived and experienced as difficult (Wahler & Dumas, 1989). Parenting involves a complex set of behaviors that emerge through a given parent's learning history, including respondent and operant processes. These same processes lead to the emergence of problematic parenting behaviors (e.g., the "coercive process"). To use a classic example, think of a mother and child in the checkout line at the grocery store. The child begs, softly at first, to get a toy. The mother says no and asks the child to be quiet. The child raises his voice and begins to cry. The mother still says no, and the child's behavior escalates. The mother gets embarrassed. Elicitation goes up, and she is extremely uncomfortable. At this point, many mothers would respond in one of two ways: aggression or acquiescence (Patterson, 1982). Let's say that the mother gives in and buys the child a toy. Both the behavior of the mother and the behavior of the child are reinforced. The child's screaming and crying is now more probable, because it worked to get what he wanted. The mother's acquiescence (and the same would be true of aggression) is negatively reinforced as the child stops misbehaving in response to it. This can be a problem in the long run. Another common feature of parenting difficulties is parenting stress. Parenting stress is the "aversive psychological reaction to the demands of being a parent" (Deater-Deckard, 1998, p. 315). Parenting stress is related to insecure attachment (Teti, Nakagawa, Das, & Wirth, 1991), child abuse and neglect (Mash & Johnston, 1990), and a host of parent and child emotional problems (for review see Deater-Deckard, 1998). Numerous strategies have been employed to reduce the problems associated with parenting and parenting stress, including behavioral-parent training. Behavioral-parent training programs teach parents the necessary skills to positively interact with, and discipline, their children. These programs, which are based on principles of respondent and operant conditioning, have demonstrated large and lasting improvements in child behavior (for review see Kazdin, 2003; Patterson, 1982). However, there is evidence that gains are attenuated if the parent is experiencing psychopathology, is economically disadvantaged, or has frequent negative interactions with other adults (Dumas, 1984; Kazdin, 1997). Perhaps problems in parenting are not sufficiently explained as skills deficits resulting from direct conditioning processes alone. Certainly respondent and operant conditioning processes are featured prominently in a parent's learning history. Data indicate that these processes contribute to the development and maintenance of problematic parenting behavior as well as to the failure to respond to treatment. It is possible, however, that problems in parenting, and in the application of parenting skills, are the result of indirect relational conditioning processes as well. Indirect Learning and the Matching-to-Sample Paradigm Verba
为人父母被认为是困难的(Wahler & Dumas, 1989)。养育子女涉及一套复杂的行为,这些行为是通过特定父母的学习历史出现的,包括应答过程和操作过程。这些同样的过程导致了有问题的养育行为的出现(例如,“强制过程”)。举一个经典的例子,想象一下在杂货店排队结账的母亲和孩子。这个孩子开始时很温柔地乞求得到一个玩具。母亲说不,并要求孩子安静。那孩子提高了嗓门,开始哭了起来。母亲仍然说不,孩子的行为就会升级。母亲感到尴尬。启发式上升,她感到非常不舒服。在这一点上,许多母亲会以两种方式之一做出反应:攻击或默许(Patterson, 1982)。假设母亲让步了,给孩子买了一个玩具。母亲的行为和孩子的行为都得到了强化。孩子的尖叫和哭泣现在更有可能,因为这有效地得到了他想要的。当孩子停止不良行为作为回应时,母亲的默许(攻击性也是如此)就会被消极地强化。从长远来看,这可能是个问题。育儿困难的另一个共同特征是育儿压力。养育压力是“对为人父母的要求的厌恶心理反应”(Deater-Deckard, 1998,第315页)。养育压力与不安全依恋(Teti, Nakagawa, Das, & Wirth, 1991)、儿童虐待和忽视(Mash & Johnston, 1990)以及一系列父母和儿童的情感问题(参见deter - deckard, 1998)有关。许多策略被用来减少与养育子女和养育子女压力有关的问题,包括行为父母训练。行为父母训练项目教给父母与孩子积极互动和管教孩子的必要技能。这些基于被调查者和操作性条件反射原则的项目,已经证明了儿童行为的巨大而持久的改善(回顾见Kazdin, 2003;帕特森,1982)。然而,有证据表明,如果父母正在经历精神病理,经济上处于不利地位,或与其他成年人频繁进行负面互动,则收益会减弱(Dumas, 1984;卡兹丁,1997)。也许养育子女的问题不能充分解释为仅仅是由直接条件反射过程造成的技能缺陷。当然,应答性条件反射过程和操作性条件反射过程在父母的学习历史中占有显著地位。数据表明,这些过程有助于问题父母行为的发展和维持,以及对治疗的反应失败。然而,也有可能,在养育子女方面的问题,以及在养育技能应用方面的问题,也是间接关系条件作用过程的结果。语言能力强的人表现出一种称为衍生关系反应的间接学习形式。刺激等价是关系条件作用的基础。在Sidman(1971)的经典实验中,一个有学习障碍的参与者被训练将口头单词与代表他们的图片相匹配,然后再训练将这些口头单词与其印刷形式相匹配。然后,参与者在没有额外训练的情况下,将打印的单词与图片匹配,将图片与打印的单词匹配。西德曼说,刺激变得彼此等效,从而形成一个等价类。等价类有三个定义性特征:反身性、对称性和传递性或等价性。反身性是指相互关联的事件与自身表现出相同的关系。作为反身性的证据,当面对刺激“1”时,参与者会从1、2和3的数组中选择“1”。…
{"title":"Relational Responding in Parents.","authors":"Amy R. Murrell, K. Wilson, Cicely T. LaBorde, Chad E. Drake, L. J. Rogers","doi":"10.1037/H0100659","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100659","url":null,"abstract":"Parenting is perceived and experienced as difficult (Wahler & Dumas, 1989). Parenting involves a complex set of behaviors that emerge through a given parent's learning history, including respondent and operant processes. These same processes lead to the emergence of problematic parenting behaviors (e.g., the \"coercive process\"). To use a classic example, think of a mother and child in the checkout line at the grocery store. The child begs, softly at first, to get a toy. The mother says no and asks the child to be quiet. The child raises his voice and begins to cry. The mother still says no, and the child's behavior escalates. The mother gets embarrassed. Elicitation goes up, and she is extremely uncomfortable. At this point, many mothers would respond in one of two ways: aggression or acquiescence (Patterson, 1982). Let's say that the mother gives in and buys the child a toy. Both the behavior of the mother and the behavior of the child are reinforced. The child's screaming and crying is now more probable, because it worked to get what he wanted. The mother's acquiescence (and the same would be true of aggression) is negatively reinforced as the child stops misbehaving in response to it. This can be a problem in the long run. Another common feature of parenting difficulties is parenting stress. Parenting stress is the \"aversive psychological reaction to the demands of being a parent\" (Deater-Deckard, 1998, p. 315). Parenting stress is related to insecure attachment (Teti, Nakagawa, Das, & Wirth, 1991), child abuse and neglect (Mash & Johnston, 1990), and a host of parent and child emotional problems (for review see Deater-Deckard, 1998). Numerous strategies have been employed to reduce the problems associated with parenting and parenting stress, including behavioral-parent training. Behavioral-parent training programs teach parents the necessary skills to positively interact with, and discipline, their children. These programs, which are based on principles of respondent and operant conditioning, have demonstrated large and lasting improvements in child behavior (for review see Kazdin, 2003; Patterson, 1982). However, there is evidence that gains are attenuated if the parent is experiencing psychopathology, is economically disadvantaged, or has frequent negative interactions with other adults (Dumas, 1984; Kazdin, 1997). Perhaps problems in parenting are not sufficiently explained as skills deficits resulting from direct conditioning processes alone. Certainly respondent and operant conditioning processes are featured prominently in a parent's learning history. Data indicate that these processes contribute to the development and maintenance of problematic parenting behavior as well as to the failure to respond to treatment. It is possible, however, that problems in parenting, and in the application of parenting skills, are the result of indirect relational conditioning processes as well. Indirect Learning and the Matching-to-Sample Paradigm Verba","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"196-214"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472203","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 26
Effects of Writing-Related Contingencies on Both Quality of Writing and Multiple-Choice Exam Performance in Large College Courses. 大学大型课程中写作相关随变对写作质量和多项选择考试成绩的影响
Pub Date : 2008-06-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100658
K. Krohn, Megan R. Parker, L. N. Foster, K. Aspiranti, D. McCleary, Robert L. Williams
Most undergraduates likely take some courses that use multiple-choice exams as a major source of course credit. Instructors may be especially inclined to use multiple-choice exams in large courses because of ease and efficiency of grading (Hautau et al., 2006b). Nonetheless, many students report difficulty in taking multiple-choice exams, claiming that they could do better on essay exams. Consequently, discovering how to heighten student performance on multiple-choice exams is an important issue at the college level (Wallace & Williams, 2003). One possibility for improving multiple-choice exam scores may be the use of daily writing activities related to concepts included on the multiple-choice exams. Past research demonstrates that brief essay quizzes may improve performance on a variety of exam formats (e.g., short answer, essay, fill-in-the-blank, and multiple-choice). Padilla-Walker (2006) found that brief, extra-credit daily quizzes on assigned reading material predicted performance on major exams (short-answer and essay) better than did gender, self-reported college GPA, and self-reported ACT scores. Daniel and Broida (2004) also reported that completing in-class quizzes over course concepts boosted performance on course exams (multiple-choice and short answer). Narloch, Garbin, and Turnage (2006) showed that prelecture quizzes, compared to no quizzes, produced better performance on both multiple-choice and essay exam items. Additionally, Leeming (2002) found that participating in daily 1015 min writing activities on course concepts significantly improved performance on a comprehensive final exam that included short essay, fill-in-the-blank, and multiple-choice questions. Similarly, Turner et al. (2006) demonstrated that students required to complete a daily in-class writing activity performed better on the course's multiple-choice exams than students without daily writing. Although some research supports using daily essay quizzes to boost major exam performance, maximizing the impact of these quizzes is not without logistical challenges. For example, grading the quizzes could be labor intensive for the instructor. The writing activities may only consume a small percentage of class time, but the time required to grade and record the scores may detract from instructor time needed to organize and prepare for class. Leeming's (2002) daily quizzes required about 10 to 20% of class time and an hour of instructor grading time each day. Hautau et al. (2006a) and Turner et al. (2006) reduced instructor time for grading quizzes by grading quizzes only on randomly selected days rather than on all days. The quizzes took 6 to 7% of total class time and required about 1 min per student for instructor grading. Given that students in these studies did not know what day's quizzes would be randomly selected for grading, the researchers expected the quizzes to have much the same impact on student performance as would daily grading and crediting of quizzes. Ho
大多数本科生可能会选择一些以多项选择考试作为课程学分的主要来源的课程。教师可能特别倾向于在大型课程中使用选择题考试,因为评分容易且高效(Hautau等人,2006b)。尽管如此,许多学生反映在多项选择考试中遇到困难,声称他们可以在论文考试中做得更好。因此,发现如何提高学生在多项选择考试中的表现是大学水平的一个重要问题(华莱士和威廉姆斯,2003年)。提高多项选择考试成绩的一种可能是使用与多项选择考试中包含的概念相关的日常写作活动。过去的研究表明,短文测验可以提高学生在各种考试形式(如简答、短文、填空和多项选择)中的表现。帕迪拉-沃克(2006)发现,与性别、自我报告的大学GPA和自我报告的ACT分数相比,关于指定阅读材料的简短、额外学分的每日小测验更能预测学生在主要考试(简答题和论文)中的表现。Daniel和Broida(2004)还报告说,完成课程概念的课堂小测验可以提高课程考试(选择题和简答题)的成绩。Narloch, Garbin和Turnage(2006)表明,与没有测验相比,课前测验在多项选择题和论文考试项目上都有更好的表现。此外,Leeming(2002)发现,每天参加1015分钟的课程概念写作活动,可以显著提高学生在包括短文、填空和选择题在内的综合期末考试中的表现。同样,Turner等人(2006)证明,在课程的多项选择考试中,被要求每天完成课堂写作活动的学生比没有每天写作的学生表现得更好。尽管一些研究支持使用每日论文测验来提高主要考试成绩,但最大化这些测验的影响并非没有后勤挑战。例如,对教师来说,批改测验可能是一项劳动密集型工作。写作活动可能只占用课堂时间的一小部分,但评分和记录分数所需的时间可能会减少教师组织和准备课堂所需的时间。Leeming(2002)的每日测验需要大约10%到20%的课堂时间和每天一个小时的教师评分时间。Hautau等人(2006a)和Turner等人(2006)通过只在随机选择的日子而不是在所有的日子里给测验评分,减少了教师给测验评分的时间。小测验占总上课时间的6%到7%,每位学生需要1分钟的时间来给老师打分。考虑到这些研究中的学生不知道哪一天的小测验将被随机选择进行评分,研究人员希望小测验对学生表现的影响与日常评分和记入小测验的影响大致相同。然而,结果证明这一期望好坏参半,这就产生了额外的问题,即如何在不需要过多教师时间的情况下,最大限度地提高学生在写作测试和多项选择考试中的表现。Hautau等人(2006a)最近试图阐明日常写作可以有效提高写作和考试成绩的条件。2006 b)。Hautau等人(2006a)要求学生以书面形式分析学生可以在教师笔记中找到的概念对。课程网站每天确定两到五对概念,学生可能会被要求在下一次测验中回答这些概念。在接下来的课堂上,老师随机选择其中一组,并指导学生识别概念的共性、差异以及一个概念对另一个概念的影响。当每个单元完成最后的写作活动时,学生随机选择一天的写作活动来计算课程学分。…
{"title":"Effects of Writing-Related Contingencies on Both Quality of Writing and Multiple-Choice Exam Performance in Large College Courses.","authors":"K. Krohn, Megan R. Parker, L. N. Foster, K. Aspiranti, D. McCleary, Robert L. Williams","doi":"10.1037/H0100658","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100658","url":null,"abstract":"Most undergraduates likely take some courses that use multiple-choice exams as a major source of course credit. Instructors may be especially inclined to use multiple-choice exams in large courses because of ease and efficiency of grading (Hautau et al., 2006b). Nonetheless, many students report difficulty in taking multiple-choice exams, claiming that they could do better on essay exams. Consequently, discovering how to heighten student performance on multiple-choice exams is an important issue at the college level (Wallace & Williams, 2003). One possibility for improving multiple-choice exam scores may be the use of daily writing activities related to concepts included on the multiple-choice exams. Past research demonstrates that brief essay quizzes may improve performance on a variety of exam formats (e.g., short answer, essay, fill-in-the-blank, and multiple-choice). Padilla-Walker (2006) found that brief, extra-credit daily quizzes on assigned reading material predicted performance on major exams (short-answer and essay) better than did gender, self-reported college GPA, and self-reported ACT scores. Daniel and Broida (2004) also reported that completing in-class quizzes over course concepts boosted performance on course exams (multiple-choice and short answer). Narloch, Garbin, and Turnage (2006) showed that prelecture quizzes, compared to no quizzes, produced better performance on both multiple-choice and essay exam items. Additionally, Leeming (2002) found that participating in daily 1015 min writing activities on course concepts significantly improved performance on a comprehensive final exam that included short essay, fill-in-the-blank, and multiple-choice questions. Similarly, Turner et al. (2006) demonstrated that students required to complete a daily in-class writing activity performed better on the course's multiple-choice exams than students without daily writing. Although some research supports using daily essay quizzes to boost major exam performance, maximizing the impact of these quizzes is not without logistical challenges. For example, grading the quizzes could be labor intensive for the instructor. The writing activities may only consume a small percentage of class time, but the time required to grade and record the scores may detract from instructor time needed to organize and prepare for class. Leeming's (2002) daily quizzes required about 10 to 20% of class time and an hour of instructor grading time each day. Hautau et al. (2006a) and Turner et al. (2006) reduced instructor time for grading quizzes by grading quizzes only on randomly selected days rather than on all days. The quizzes took 6 to 7% of total class time and required about 1 min per student for instructor grading. Given that students in these studies did not know what day's quizzes would be randomly selected for grading, the researchers expected the quizzes to have much the same impact on student performance as would daily grading and crediting of quizzes. Ho","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"184-195"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472151","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Case Record Analysis 病例记录分析
Pub Date : 2008-06-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100657
S. Whitaker
It is argued that the determinates of low frequency (less than once an hour) challenging behavior are likely to be more complex than those of high frequency behavior involving setting events that may not be present when the behavior occurs. The analysis of case records is then examined as a method of identifying possible setting events to low frequency behaviours. It is suggested that time series analysis, correlational analysis and time lag sequential analysis may all be useful methods in the examination of case records.
有人认为,低频(少于一小时一次)挑战性行为的决定因素可能比高频行为的决定因素更复杂,这些高频行为涉及到行为发生时可能不存在的设定事件。然后检查病例记录的分析,作为识别可能的低频行为设置事件的方法。建议时间序列分析、相关分析和时间滞后序列分析都可以作为检验病例记录的有效方法。
{"title":"Case Record Analysis","authors":"S. Whitaker","doi":"10.1037/H0100657","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100657","url":null,"abstract":"It is argued that the determinates of low frequency (less than once an hour) challenging behavior are likely to be more complex than those of high frequency behavior involving setting events that may not be present when the behavior occurs. The analysis of case records is then examined as a method of identifying possible setting events to low frequency behaviours. It is suggested that time series analysis, correlational analysis and time lag sequential analysis may all be useful methods in the examination of case records.","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"172-183"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472143","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Defining Frequency: A Natural Scientific Term 定义频率:一个自然科学术语
Pub Date : 2008-03-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100651
R. Kubina, Fan-Yu Lin
Frequency represents a standard measurement that pervades all motion and movement. "Combining observation time with count yields one of the most widely use of measures in applied behavior analysis, rate (or frequency) or responding, defined as the number of responses per unit of time." (Cooper, et al., 2007, p.76) By providing the number of occurrences over time, a frequency measurement can precisely describe the dimensional quantity of a repeating event. A standard measurement must be applied precisely in all cases. Once the fundamental properties of the event are chosen, a standard measurement can be implemented and used across all cases without yielding faulty interpretation. For example, when instances of a repeating event are of interest, frequency offers precise measurements across any behavior topography such as typing, walking, running, and so on. The National Institute of Standards and Technology, or NIST, defines the time interval as "one of the four basic standards of measurement (the others are length, mass, and temperature). Of these four basic standards, the time interval (and frequency) can be measured with the most resolution and the least uncertainty (NIST, 2007). A measurement remains standardized in any occurrence, which is especially true within natural science. NIST defines frequency as the time of a repetitive event. If T equals the period or time interval elapse for a repetitive event, then frequency, or f, is its reciprocal or f = 1/T (NIST, 2006a). An example of a special unit of measurement for frequency is the Hertz, named after Heinrich Hertz. A hertz represents 1 crest of a wavelength passing a given point per second (Hazen & Trefil, 1990). In other words, we want to measure the frequency of sound by capturing the number of repetitive crests of the wave moving per second. T equals one second. Frequency of one Hz is one cycle of wave moving per second. As an example of a Hertz measure, the musical note "A" corresponds to a frequency of 440 crests of the wave moving per second, known as 440Hz, and middle "C" on a piano equals 246 Hz. By using standardized measurement of sound frequency, we can identify and compare various musical notes. Frequency settings also play a vital role beyond science in technology mediums such as television and radio broadcasting. Although seconds are commonly used in frequency measurement, the passage of time can vary depending on the repeated events of interest. The frequency quantity may include units of waves per second, cycles per minute, responses per hour, or occurrences per given period of time. Importance of Measuring Frequency in Behavior Science In behavioral science, quantitative data of behaviors are collected and converted to units of measurement for purpose of comparison. Skinner had long maintained the importance of frequency when measuring behaviors. In Skinner's words: "It follows that the main datum to be measured in the study of the dynamic laws of an operant is the length o
频率是一种标准的度量,它遍及所有的运动和运动。“将观察时间与计数相结合,产生了应用行为分析中最广泛使用的测量方法之一,即反应率(或频率)或反应,定义为每单位时间内的反应次数。”(Cooper, et al., 2007, p.76)通过提供随时间的发生次数,频率测量可以精确地描述重复事件的维度数量。在所有情况下都必须精确地应用标准测量。一旦选择了事件的基本属性,就可以在所有情况下实现和使用标准度量,而不会产生错误的解释。例如,当对重复事件的实例感兴趣时,频率提供了跨任何行为结构(如打字、行走、跑步等)的精确度量。美国国家标准与技术研究所(NIST)将时间间隔定义为“四个基本测量标准之一(其他三个是长度、质量和温度)”。在这四个基本标准中,时间间隔(和频率)可以以最高的分辨率和最小的不确定性进行测量(NIST, 2007)。在任何情况下,测量都是标准化的,在自然科学中尤其如此。NIST将频率定义为重复事件发生的时间。如果T等于一个重复事件的周期或时间间隔,那么频率,或f,是它的倒数或f = 1/T (NIST, 2006a)。频率的一个特殊测量单位是赫兹,以海因里希·赫兹的名字命名。赫兹表示每秒通过一个给定点的波长的一个波峰(Hazen & Trefil, 1990)。换句话说,我们想通过捕捉每秒移动的波的重复波峰的数量来测量声音的频率。T等于1秒。一赫兹的频率是每秒移动一个周期的波。作为赫兹测量的一个例子,音符“a”对应于每秒移动的440个波峰的频率,称为440Hz,钢琴上的中音“C”等于246 Hz。通过使用标准化的声音频率测量,我们可以识别和比较不同的音符。频率设置在电视和无线电广播等技术媒介中也发挥着科学以外的重要作用。虽然在频率测量中通常使用秒,但时间的流逝可能会因感兴趣的重复事件而变化。频率量可以包括每秒波的单位、每分钟的周期、每小时的响应或每给定时间段的发生次数。在行为科学中,收集行为的定量数据并将其转换为测量单位以进行比较。斯金纳长期以来一直坚持频率在衡量行为时的重要性。用斯金纳的话来说:“由此可见,在研究一个被试的动态规律时,要衡量的主要基准是一个反应和它之前的反应之间的时间长度……”(1938,第58页)。目视观察在一段时间内发生的反应的累积数量,或者一段时间内发生的事件的数量,在发现行为的基本原则方面发挥了关键作用。Ferster和Skinner(1957)解释了频率的图形显示和强化时间表的操纵如何证明了有机体反应可能性的可靠变化。在《科学与人类行为》(Science and Human Behavior, 1953)一书中,斯金纳(Skinner, 1953)描述了频率如何推动了反应概率的概念:“当我们为了科学用途而改进反应概率的概念时,我们发现,在这里,我们的数据也是频率,并且必须指定观察频率的条件”(第63页)。在很多场合,斯金纳也用“率”这个词来指代频率。“反应率”使斯金纳能够将特定时间内发生的事件数量作为行为模式进行阐述。…
{"title":"Defining Frequency: A Natural Scientific Term","authors":"R. Kubina, Fan-Yu Lin","doi":"10.1037/H0100651","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100651","url":null,"abstract":"Frequency represents a standard measurement that pervades all motion and movement. \"Combining observation time with count yields one of the most widely use of measures in applied behavior analysis, rate (or frequency) or responding, defined as the number of responses per unit of time.\" (Cooper, et al., 2007, p.76) By providing the number of occurrences over time, a frequency measurement can precisely describe the dimensional quantity of a repeating event. A standard measurement must be applied precisely in all cases. Once the fundamental properties of the event are chosen, a standard measurement can be implemented and used across all cases without yielding faulty interpretation. For example, when instances of a repeating event are of interest, frequency offers precise measurements across any behavior topography such as typing, walking, running, and so on. The National Institute of Standards and Technology, or NIST, defines the time interval as \"one of the four basic standards of measurement (the others are length, mass, and temperature). Of these four basic standards, the time interval (and frequency) can be measured with the most resolution and the least uncertainty (NIST, 2007). A measurement remains standardized in any occurrence, which is especially true within natural science. NIST defines frequency as the time of a repetitive event. If T equals the period or time interval elapse for a repetitive event, then frequency, or f, is its reciprocal or f = 1/T (NIST, 2006a). An example of a special unit of measurement for frequency is the Hertz, named after Heinrich Hertz. A hertz represents 1 crest of a wavelength passing a given point per second (Hazen & Trefil, 1990). In other words, we want to measure the frequency of sound by capturing the number of repetitive crests of the wave moving per second. T equals one second. Frequency of one Hz is one cycle of wave moving per second. As an example of a Hertz measure, the musical note \"A\" corresponds to a frequency of 440 crests of the wave moving per second, known as 440Hz, and middle \"C\" on a piano equals 246 Hz. By using standardized measurement of sound frequency, we can identify and compare various musical notes. Frequency settings also play a vital role beyond science in technology mediums such as television and radio broadcasting. Although seconds are commonly used in frequency measurement, the passage of time can vary depending on the repeated events of interest. The frequency quantity may include units of waves per second, cycles per minute, responses per hour, or occurrences per given period of time. Importance of Measuring Frequency in Behavior Science In behavioral science, quantitative data of behaviors are collected and converted to units of measurement for purpose of comparison. Skinner had long maintained the importance of frequency when measuring behaviors. In Skinner's words: \"It follows that the main datum to be measured in the study of the dynamic laws of an operant is the length o","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"125-129"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58471947","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
The Translation of Basic Behavioral Research to School Psychology: A Citation Analysis. 《基本行为研究》对学校心理学的翻译:引文分析。
Pub Date : 2008-03-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100653
Derek D. Reed
In recent years, school psychology has entered into a new era of accountability where scientifically-based practices are not only encouraged, but are mandated by law (IDEA, 2004). For school-based practitioners, this means documenting the rationale and empirical support for behavior change procedures in the classroom. Failing to do so is not only a poor practice of school psychology, it may also have legal ramifications. Thus, it is of no surprise that the field of school psychology has turned to applied behavior analysis due to its rich empirical support, behavioral assessment methodology, and function-based approaches to solving behavioral concerns (Kratochwill & Martens, 1994). Vollmer and Northup (1997) suggest that one of the key aspects of applied behavior analysis that makes it complimentary to school psychology is its foundation in the basic principles of behavior. In The Technology of Teaching, Skinner remarked that "a really effective educational system cannot be set up until we understand the processes of learning and teaching" (Skinner, 1968, p. 95). Skinner's views on education continue to resonate today as practitioners and applied researchers alike attempt to pinpoint the active treatment ingredient responsible for behavior change through an understanding of the basic behavioral processes at play in a successful behavior change program. As any practicing school psychologist can attest, the classroom ecology is extremely complex, with multiple schedules of reinforcement operating at any given time (Martens & Kelly, 1993). For instance, within any classroom, many students will be exposed to behavior support plans, Individualized Education Programs (IEPs), direct instruction programs, behavior altering medication for behavioral concerns, differing reinforcement histories, etc. With such confounding variables, it becomes very difficult to truly understand what is accounting for the observed behavior change after an intervention is implemented. Thus, it is not always readily apparent which procedures are responsible for behavior change during intervention implementation. One of the fundamental attributes of behavioral research is its reliance on parsimony in understanding behavior change (Johnston & Pennypacker, 1993). While it is easy to monitor gains in the classroom without an understanding of the controlling behavioral processes, this oversight is not only dangerous to the long-term sustainability of the intervention, it actually does a disservice to the field. When an intervention works, the most we can walk away with is an understanding of what works with that individual in that setting for that target behavior. In sum, the generalizability of the intervention across students, time, settings, and responses is compromised. However, with an understanding of the basic behavioral principles at play, one can individually tailor the intervention to account for these individual and situational differences. The avenue to accomplishing a t
近年来,学校心理学进入了一个新的问责时代,在这个时代,基于科学的实践不仅受到鼓励,而且受到法律的强制要求(IDEA, 2004)。对于以学校为基础的实践者来说,这意味着在课堂上记录行为改变过程的基本原理和经验支持。不这样做不仅是学校心理学的不良实践,还可能产生法律后果。因此,学校心理学领域转向应用行为分析也就不足为奇了,因为它有丰富的经验支持、行为评估方法和基于功能的方法来解决行为问题(Kratochwill & Martens, 1994)。Vollmer和Northup(1997)认为,应用行为分析与学校心理学互补的一个关键方面是它以行为基本原则为基础。在《教学技术》一书中,斯金纳指出:“在我们理解学习和教学的过程之前,不可能建立真正有效的教育系统”(斯金纳,1968,第95页)。斯金纳关于教育的观点在今天仍然引起共鸣,因为从业人员和应用研究人员都试图通过理解成功的行为改变项目中起作用的基本行为过程,来确定导致行为改变的积极治疗因素。正如任何一位在职的学校心理学家可以证明的那样,课堂生态是极其复杂的,在任何给定的时间都有多种强化计划在运行(Martens & Kelly, 1993)。例如,在任何课堂中,许多学生都将接触到行为支持计划、个性化教育计划(IEPs)、直接指导计划、针对行为问题的行为改变药物治疗、不同的强化历史等。有了这样的混杂变量,就很难真正理解是什么导致了实施干预后观察到的行为变化。因此,在实施干预期间,哪些程序对行为改变负责并不总是很明显的。行为研究的基本属性之一是它在理解行为变化时依赖于简约(Johnston & Pennypacker, 1993)。虽然在不了解控制行为过程的情况下很容易监控课堂上的收益,但这种监督不仅对干预的长期可持续性有害,而且实际上对该领域有害。当一种干预有效时,我们最多能得到的是理解在那个环境下,什么对那个人的目标行为有效。总之,干预在学生、时间、环境和反应之间的普遍性受到了损害。然而,随着对基本行为原则的理解,人们可以根据这些个体和情境差异量身定制干预措施。真正理解学术行为或与学术相关行为的行为原则的途径是通过一种被称为“桥梁”或“转化”研究的研究形式。这种研究方法的特点是将实验室研究简单地复制到应用情况和临床人群中(Lerman, 2003)。从本质上讲,转化研究为研究人员提供了更多的实验控制(即类似于实验室环境),同时仍然指向应用问题。然而,尽管似乎迫切需要基础研究和应用研究之间的关系,但两者之间的脱节继续扩大(Mace, 1994;Mace & Wacker, 1994)。虽然研究人员已经成功地将基础实验研究转化为应用环境(Iwata, 1991),但许多实践者和学校心理学家可能会觉得基础研究对他们的环境来说太深奥了。…
{"title":"The Translation of Basic Behavioral Research to School Psychology: A Citation Analysis.","authors":"Derek D. Reed","doi":"10.1037/H0100653","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100653","url":null,"abstract":"In recent years, school psychology has entered into a new era of accountability where scientifically-based practices are not only encouraged, but are mandated by law (IDEA, 2004). For school-based practitioners, this means documenting the rationale and empirical support for behavior change procedures in the classroom. Failing to do so is not only a poor practice of school psychology, it may also have legal ramifications. Thus, it is of no surprise that the field of school psychology has turned to applied behavior analysis due to its rich empirical support, behavioral assessment methodology, and function-based approaches to solving behavioral concerns (Kratochwill & Martens, 1994). Vollmer and Northup (1997) suggest that one of the key aspects of applied behavior analysis that makes it complimentary to school psychology is its foundation in the basic principles of behavior. In The Technology of Teaching, Skinner remarked that \"a really effective educational system cannot be set up until we understand the processes of learning and teaching\" (Skinner, 1968, p. 95). Skinner's views on education continue to resonate today as practitioners and applied researchers alike attempt to pinpoint the active treatment ingredient responsible for behavior change through an understanding of the basic behavioral processes at play in a successful behavior change program. As any practicing school psychologist can attest, the classroom ecology is extremely complex, with multiple schedules of reinforcement operating at any given time (Martens & Kelly, 1993). For instance, within any classroom, many students will be exposed to behavior support plans, Individualized Education Programs (IEPs), direct instruction programs, behavior altering medication for behavioral concerns, differing reinforcement histories, etc. With such confounding variables, it becomes very difficult to truly understand what is accounting for the observed behavior change after an intervention is implemented. Thus, it is not always readily apparent which procedures are responsible for behavior change during intervention implementation. One of the fundamental attributes of behavioral research is its reliance on parsimony in understanding behavior change (Johnston & Pennypacker, 1993). While it is easy to monitor gains in the classroom without an understanding of the controlling behavioral processes, this oversight is not only dangerous to the long-term sustainability of the intervention, it actually does a disservice to the field. When an intervention works, the most we can walk away with is an understanding of what works with that individual in that setting for that target behavior. In sum, the generalizability of the intervention across students, time, settings, and responses is compromised. However, with an understanding of the basic behavioral principles at play, one can individually tailor the intervention to account for these individual and situational differences. The avenue to accomplishing a t","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"143-149"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472124","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Facing the Challenge: The Behavior Analysis of Gambling 面对挑战:赌博行为分析
Pub Date : 2008-03-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100652
J. Weatherly, K. Flannery
For many, gambling is an entertaining, leisure-time activity. For a small percentage of individuals, however, gambling represents a severe behavioral disorder that has very negative, life-changing consequences. Although estimates of the frequency of pathological gambling in the general population may seem small (e.g., 1-3%; see Petry, 2005, for a review), those estimates translate into millions of individuals. Furthermore, there are certain populations who suffer from pathological gambling at rates well above those seen in the general population. For instance, data suggest that American Indians display the symptoms of pathological gambling at well over 10 times the frequency of the majority population (Wardman, el-Guebaly, & Hodgins, 2001). In short, pathological and problem gambling is a serious societal problem that deserves research attention. Despite the seriousness of the disorder, an argument could be made that the research community, and specifically behavior analysis, has done little to identify the underlying causes (or contingencies) that lead to pathological gambling. A cursory review of the literature supports that argument. A search of the PsycINFO database using the term "gambling" in an all-text search (conducted on September 8, 2007) yielded a total of 3,354 references. An identical search that cross referenced "gambling" with "experiment" identified 166 sources. A separate search that cross referenced "gambling" and "behavior analysis" yielded only 14 citations. One that cross referenced "gambling" and "reinforcement" yielded a total of 87. These searches suggest that less than 5% of the research literature on pathological gambling even mentions the word experiment, let alone uses experimental methodology. The importance of this small percentage cannot be overstated given that experiments represent the strongest and most straightforward method of determining cause-and-effect relationships. The searches also suggest that less than 3% of the published work on gambling is coming from a behavior-analytic perspective. This percentage is disturbing because behavior analysis has been successfully applied to serious behavioral disorders (e.g., Autism), yet little has been done to apply behavior analysis to pathological gambling. That is not to say that behavior analysts have completely ignored the study of gambling. They have not. For instance, Rachlin (1997) forwarded behaviorally oriented theories of addiction. Although discussed in the context of substance abuse, these theories can certainly be applied to pathological gambling. More recently, Fantino, Navarro, and O'Daly (2005) and Madden, Ewan, and Lagorio (2007) have argued that behavioral research perspectives and techniques can potentially forward our understanding of gambling behavior relative to other approaches. Petry and Roll (2001) suggested some treatments for pathological gambling from a behavioral perspective. Unfortunately, these significant contributions have not led to
对许多人来说,赌博是一种娱乐的休闲活动。然而,对于一小部分人来说,赌博代表了一种严重的行为障碍,会产生非常负面的、改变生活的后果。尽管对一般人群中病态赌博频率的估计可能看起来很小(例如,1-3%;参见Petry, 2005,以获得评论),这些估计转化为数百万人。此外,某些人群患病态赌博的比率远高于一般人群。例如,数据表明,美洲印第安人表现出病态赌博症状的频率是大多数人的10倍以上(Wardman, el-Guebaly, & Hodgins, 2001)。简而言之,病态和问题赌博是一个严重的社会问题,值得研究关注。尽管这种疾病很严重,但可以提出一个论点,即研究界,特别是行为分析,在确定导致病态赌博的潜在原因(或偶然事件)方面做得很少。对文献的粗略回顾支持了这一观点。在2007年9月8日进行的一次全文搜索中,在PsycINFO数据库中使用“赌博”一词进行了搜索,总共得到了3354篇参考文献。一项相同的搜索将“赌博”与“实验”交叉引用,确定了166个来源。另一项交叉引用“赌博”和“行为分析”的单独搜索只得到14条引用。其中一个交叉引用“赌博”和“强化”的结果是87。这些搜索表明,只有不到5%的病态赌博研究文献提到“实验”这个词,更不用说使用实验方法了。考虑到实验是确定因果关系的最有力和最直接的方法,这个小比例的重要性怎么强调都不为过。搜索结果还表明,在已发表的有关赌博的研究中,只有不到3%是从行为分析的角度出发的。这一比例令人不安,因为行为分析已经成功地应用于严重的行为障碍(如自闭症),但将行为分析应用于病态赌博却很少。这并不是说行为分析师完全忽略了对赌博的研究。但事实并非如此。例如,Rachlin(1997)提出了行为导向的成瘾理论。虽然在药物滥用的背景下讨论,这些理论当然可以应用于病态赌博。最近,Fantino, Navarro, and O'Daly(2005)和Madden, Ewan, and Lagorio(2007)认为行为研究的视角和技术可以潜在地推动我们对赌博行为的理解。Petry和Roll(2001)从行为的角度提出了一些病态赌博的治疗方法。不幸的是,这些重要的贡献并没有导致大量的行为分析研究。最近的两项努力有望启动这类研究。首先是建立一份专门研究赌博行为的行为分析杂志。《赌博行为分析》杂志于2007年出版了第一期。第二次尝试是在第一期杂志上发表的一篇文章。具体来说,Weatherly和Dixon(2007)试图提出一个赌博行为的综合行为分析模型。他们认为,通过将研究注意力集中在特定变量上,并为在更广泛的文献中进行以行为为导向的研究提供一般理论基础,这样做可能有助于赌博研究。他们还建议,这种模式还可能促进从机构和基金会获得研究经费,因为这些实体往往严重依赖理性主义的科学方法。...
{"title":"Facing the Challenge: The Behavior Analysis of Gambling","authors":"J. Weatherly, K. Flannery","doi":"10.1037/H0100652","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100652","url":null,"abstract":"For many, gambling is an entertaining, leisure-time activity. For a small percentage of individuals, however, gambling represents a severe behavioral disorder that has very negative, life-changing consequences. Although estimates of the frequency of pathological gambling in the general population may seem small (e.g., 1-3%; see Petry, 2005, for a review), those estimates translate into millions of individuals. Furthermore, there are certain populations who suffer from pathological gambling at rates well above those seen in the general population. For instance, data suggest that American Indians display the symptoms of pathological gambling at well over 10 times the frequency of the majority population (Wardman, el-Guebaly, & Hodgins, 2001). In short, pathological and problem gambling is a serious societal problem that deserves research attention. Despite the seriousness of the disorder, an argument could be made that the research community, and specifically behavior analysis, has done little to identify the underlying causes (or contingencies) that lead to pathological gambling. A cursory review of the literature supports that argument. A search of the PsycINFO database using the term \"gambling\" in an all-text search (conducted on September 8, 2007) yielded a total of 3,354 references. An identical search that cross referenced \"gambling\" with \"experiment\" identified 166 sources. A separate search that cross referenced \"gambling\" and \"behavior analysis\" yielded only 14 citations. One that cross referenced \"gambling\" and \"reinforcement\" yielded a total of 87. These searches suggest that less than 5% of the research literature on pathological gambling even mentions the word experiment, let alone uses experimental methodology. The importance of this small percentage cannot be overstated given that experiments represent the strongest and most straightforward method of determining cause-and-effect relationships. The searches also suggest that less than 3% of the published work on gambling is coming from a behavior-analytic perspective. This percentage is disturbing because behavior analysis has been successfully applied to serious behavioral disorders (e.g., Autism), yet little has been done to apply behavior analysis to pathological gambling. That is not to say that behavior analysts have completely ignored the study of gambling. They have not. For instance, Rachlin (1997) forwarded behaviorally oriented theories of addiction. Although discussed in the context of substance abuse, these theories can certainly be applied to pathological gambling. More recently, Fantino, Navarro, and O'Daly (2005) and Madden, Ewan, and Lagorio (2007) have argued that behavioral research perspectives and techniques can potentially forward our understanding of gambling behavior relative to other approaches. Petry and Roll (2001) suggested some treatments for pathological gambling from a behavioral perspective. Unfortunately, these significant contributions have not led to ","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"130-142"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472038","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Using Vicarious Reinforcement to Increase Client Completion of Between-Session Assignments 使用替代强化来增加客户完成会话之间的任务
Pub Date : 2008-03-22 DOI: 10.1037/H0100654
J. Malouff, N. Schutte, S. Rooke
There is a significant relationship between adherence to psychotherapy between-sessions assignments and treatment outcome (Addis & Jacobson, 2003; Bryant, Simons, & Thase, 1999; Edelman & Chambless, 1995; Kazantzis & Lampropoulos, 2002; Startup & Edmond, 1994). However, clients often do not complete assignments. Thus, it can be good practice for psychotherapists to use methods to maximize the chances that clients will carry out assigned tasks. Although many therapists have offered suggestions about how to increase the chances that clients will complete therapy assignments (e.g., Malouff & Schutte, 2004), only a few studies show that specific methods help. In a noteworthy study, Cox, Tisdale, and Culbert (1988) found that giving clients psychological-medical assignments in writing as well as orally led to more adherence than assignments given only orally. Beyond this one finding, to our knowledge the only evidence supporting the value of any specific strategy to increase rates of mental health assignment completion have come from our studies on vicarious reinforcement. Vicarious reinforcement involves an observer emulating the behavior of another individual (a model) because that individual benefited from performing the behavior (Malouff & Rooke, 2008). Vicarious reinforcement can occur either through direct observation of modeled behavior, or symbolic modeling, such as through a story, told orally or in writing. For example, symbolic vicarious reinforcement may occur when a model tells a personal anecdote about how she or he behaved in a particular way and experienced positive outcomes. The behavior analytic perspective on vicarious reinforcement holds that a focus on mediating cognitive factors is not needed and that the essential element of vicarious learning is the relationship between an individual's learning history and his or her current environment (Deguchi, 1984; Masia & Chase, 1997). Masia and Chase (1997) argued that a behavior analytic approach to vicarious learning is especially useful for clinicians drawing on vicarious learning as part of therapeutic interventions. Research on Vicarious Reinforcement in Increasing Assignment Completion Our first study on vicarious reinforcement found that providing individuals with video models of psychotherapy clients who talked about how they benefitted from treatment led to increases in positive attitude toward obtaining mental health treatment (Buckley & Malouff, 2004). We have since completed the only published studies on the value of vicarious reinforcement in increasing the rate of client completion of assigned tasks. In the first of these studies (Rooke and Malouff, 2006), individuals in the experimental condition received information about how to use expressive writing to reduce distress and received a written anecdote about a person who used the method and benefitted. They also viewed a video of three individuals who described the beneficial consequences expressive writing had for them. In
心理治疗间歇任务的依从性与治疗结果之间存在显著的关系(Addis & Jacobson, 2003;布莱恩特,西蒙斯和塞斯,1999;Edelman & Chambless, 1995;Kazantzis & lamprooulos, 2002;Startup & Edmond, 1994)。然而,客户经常不完成任务。因此,对于心理治疗师来说,使用方法来最大化客户执行指定任务的机会是一种很好的实践。尽管许多治疗师提供了关于如何增加来访者完成治疗任务的机会的建议(例如,Malouff & Schutte, 2004),但只有少数研究表明具体的方法有帮助。在一项值得注意的研究中,Cox, Tisdale和Culbert(1988)发现,给来访者书面和口头的心理医学任务比只口头的任务更能提高依从性。除了这一发现之外,据我们所知,支持任何提高心理健康任务完成率的特定策略的价值的唯一证据来自我们对替代强化的研究。替代强化包括观察者模仿另一个个体(一个模型)的行为,因为这个个体从执行行为中受益(Malouff & Rooke, 2008)。替代强化既可以通过直接观察被模仿的行为,也可以通过象征性的模仿,比如通过口头或书面讲述的故事。例如,当一个模特讲述她或他如何以特定方式行事并经历积极结果的个人轶事时,象征性的替代强化可能会发生。替代强化的行为分析观点认为,不需要关注中介认知因素,替代学习的基本要素是个体的学习历史与其当前环境之间的关系(Deguchi, 1984;Masia & Chase, 1997)。Masia和Chase(1997)认为,替代学习的行为分析方法对于临床医生将替代学习作为治疗干预的一部分特别有用。我们的第一项关于替代强化的研究发现,向个体提供心理治疗客户的视频模型,这些客户讲述了他们如何从治疗中受益,从而增加了他们对获得心理健康治疗的积极态度(Buckley & Malouff, 2004)。此后,我们完成了唯一发表的关于替代强化在提高客户完成指定任务率方面的价值的研究。在第一个研究中(Rooke和Malouff, 2006),实验条件下的个体收到了关于如何使用表达性写作来减少痛苦的信息,并收到了一个使用这种方法并从中受益的人的书面轶事。他们还观看了一段视频,视频中有三个人描述了表达性写作对他们的有益影响。在控制条件下的个体收到了关于如何使用写作来减少痛苦的信息,并观看了一个关于创造性的控制视频。结果表明,表达性写作的目标行为的间接强化导致参与者使用表达性写作的水平显著提高,这是通过坚持的自我评分和写的单词数来衡量的。在一项研究中,一组想要学习一种减少痛苦程度的方法的个人,我们发现,三个有用的解决问题方法的书面轶事导致更多地使用这些方法,通过测量问题解决表格上的单词数量,通过依从性的自我评价,以及通过观察者对完成问题解决表格的依从性的评价(Coates, Malouff, and Rooke,出版)。影响模型的属性模型的一些属性倾向于增加它们对观察者行为的影响。…
{"title":"Using Vicarious Reinforcement to Increase Client Completion of Between-Session Assignments","authors":"J. Malouff, N. Schutte, S. Rooke","doi":"10.1037/H0100654","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100654","url":null,"abstract":"There is a significant relationship between adherence to psychotherapy between-sessions assignments and treatment outcome (Addis & Jacobson, 2003; Bryant, Simons, & Thase, 1999; Edelman & Chambless, 1995; Kazantzis & Lampropoulos, 2002; Startup & Edmond, 1994). However, clients often do not complete assignments. Thus, it can be good practice for psychotherapists to use methods to maximize the chances that clients will carry out assigned tasks. Although many therapists have offered suggestions about how to increase the chances that clients will complete therapy assignments (e.g., Malouff & Schutte, 2004), only a few studies show that specific methods help. In a noteworthy study, Cox, Tisdale, and Culbert (1988) found that giving clients psychological-medical assignments in writing as well as orally led to more adherence than assignments given only orally. Beyond this one finding, to our knowledge the only evidence supporting the value of any specific strategy to increase rates of mental health assignment completion have come from our studies on vicarious reinforcement. Vicarious reinforcement involves an observer emulating the behavior of another individual (a model) because that individual benefited from performing the behavior (Malouff & Rooke, 2008). Vicarious reinforcement can occur either through direct observation of modeled behavior, or symbolic modeling, such as through a story, told orally or in writing. For example, symbolic vicarious reinforcement may occur when a model tells a personal anecdote about how she or he behaved in a particular way and experienced positive outcomes. The behavior analytic perspective on vicarious reinforcement holds that a focus on mediating cognitive factors is not needed and that the essential element of vicarious learning is the relationship between an individual's learning history and his or her current environment (Deguchi, 1984; Masia & Chase, 1997). Masia and Chase (1997) argued that a behavior analytic approach to vicarious learning is especially useful for clinicians drawing on vicarious learning as part of therapeutic interventions. Research on Vicarious Reinforcement in Increasing Assignment Completion Our first study on vicarious reinforcement found that providing individuals with video models of psychotherapy clients who talked about how they benefitted from treatment led to increases in positive attitude toward obtaining mental health treatment (Buckley & Malouff, 2004). We have since completed the only published studies on the value of vicarious reinforcement in increasing the rate of client completion of assigned tasks. In the first of these studies (Rooke and Malouff, 2006), individuals in the experimental condition received information about how to use expressive writing to reduce distress and received a written anecdote about a person who used the method and benefitted. They also viewed a video of three individuals who described the beneficial consequences expressive writing had for them. In","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"150-152"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-03-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58472127","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Extinction-Induced Response Resurgence: A Selective Review 灭绝诱导的反应复苏:选择性回顾
Pub Date : 2008-01-01 DOI: 10.1037/H0100644
Adam H. Doughty, Gabriella Oken
Resurgence refers to the recovery of previously extinguished responding when a recently reinforced response is extinguished (e.g., Epstein, 1983, 1985). Figure 1 illustrates resurgence using hypothetical data. In the first condition, only Response A is reinforced. Response A is extinguished in the second condition concurrent with the reinforcement of Response B. Response B is extinguished in the third condition, and the subsequent recovery of Response A is resurgence. The degree of recovery of Response A can be compared to a different (control) response that never was reinforced (Response C). Measuring Response C in this third condition serves as a measure of extinction-induced response variability (e.g., Antonitis, 1951). Thus, resurgence provides a more refined characterization of extinction-induced behavior. That is, responses predicted to occur in extinction are those responses that previously were reinforced under comparable stimulus conditions. Although resurgence and related extinction-induced phenomena were investigated in the 1970s (e.g., Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976; Pacitti, & Smith, 1977; Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977), as well as earlier (see Epstein, 1985), Epstein (1983, 1985) usually is viewed as being the most forceful commentator regarding the utility of understanding the determinants and consequences of resurgence (see also Epstein & Skinner, 1980). Epstein argued that resurgence provides behavior analysts with a more descriptive means of understanding findings sometimes attributed to Freud's concept of regression. For Freud, regression involved an individual regressing to previously demonstrated behavior considered acceptable earlier in the individual's development. In addition to linking resurgence and regression, Epstein also summarized much of the experimental findings related to resurgence. The aim of the present paper, therefore, is to describe more recent experimental findings related to resurgence. By showing the relevance of the topic to a variety of topics in and outside behavior analysis (behavioral history, drug relapse, severe problem behavior, communication disorders, and cognition), this paper urges both basic and applied behavior analysts to continue the analysis of resurgence. Behavioral history Appealing to the behavioral history of an organism often provides behavior analysts with a means of explaining complex behavior (e.g., Barrett, 1986; Doughty, Cirino, Mayfield, da Silva, Okouchi, & Lattal, 2005; Lattal, & Neef, 1996; Tatham, & Wanchisen, 1998; Wanchisen, & Tatham, 1991). Broadly speaking, resurgence is a behavioral-history effect in that current behavior only can be understood by appealing to contingencies of reinforcement exposed to the organism previously. A primary issue, therefore, in the study of resurgence is: what are the necessary and sufficient conditions to produce resurgence, in terms of both past experiences and current environmental conditions? Lieving and Lattal (2003)
死灰复燃是指当最近增强的反应消失时,先前消失的反应恢复(例如,Epstein, 1983,1985)。图1使用假设数据说明了复苏。在第一种情况下,只有反应A得到了强化。反应A在第二种情况下消失,同时反应B得到强化。反应B在第三种情况下消失,随后反应A的恢复是死灰复燃。反应A的恢复程度可以与从未加强的不同(对照)反应(反应C)进行比较。在第三种情况下测量反应C可以作为灭绝引起的反应变异性的测量(例如,Antonitis, 1951)。因此,死灰复燃提供了一种更精确的灭绝诱导行为特征。也就是说,预测在灭绝中发生的反应是那些先前在可比刺激条件下得到加强的反应。虽然在20世纪70年代研究了复苏和相关的灭绝现象(例如,Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976;Pacitti, & Smith, 1977;Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977),以及更早的(见Epstein, 1985), Epstein(1983, 1985)通常被认为是关于理解复苏的决定因素和后果的效用的最有力的评论员(另见Epstein & Skinner, 1980)。爱泼斯坦认为,这种复苏为行为分析师提供了一种更具描述性的方法来理解有时归因于弗洛伊德回归概念的发现。对弗洛伊德来说,回归是指个体回归到在个体发展早期被认为是可接受的先前表现的行为。除了将复苏和回归联系起来之外,Epstein还总结了许多与复苏相关的实验发现。因此,本文的目的是描述与复苏有关的最新实验发现。通过展示该主题与行为分析内外的各种主题(行为史,药物复发,严重问题行为,沟通障碍和认知)的相关性,本文敦促基础和应用行为分析师继续分析复苏。诉诸于生物体的行为史通常为行为分析家提供了解释复杂行为的手段(例如,Barrett, 1986;Doughty, Cirino, Mayfield, da Silva, Okouchi, & Lattal, 2005;Lattal, & Neef, 1996;Tatham, & Wanchisen, 1998;Wanchisen, & Tatham, 1991)。广义地说,复苏是一种行为历史效应,因为当前的行为只能通过诉诸于生物体先前暴露的强化的偶然性来理解。因此,复兴研究中的一个主要问题是:从过去的经验和当前的环境条件来看,产生复兴的必要和充分条件是什么?living和latal(2003)通过几个实验解决了上面提出的问题。在一项实验中,在第一种情况下,鸽子的啄键行为通过传递谷物得到加强。在第二种情况下,键啄被消灭,同时踏板压的加强(也通过粮食输送)。在第三种情况下,停止踩踏板,并测量啄键的恢复情况。通过重复这三个条件,并在第三个条件下持续观察键击死灰复燃,证实了死灰复燃是一个普遍和可复制的发现。在随后的两个实验中,研究了在产生按键回火过程中灭火踩压的必要性。在一个实验中,在第三种情况下不安排灭绝,而是使送餐与反应无关。在这种情况下,啄键现象不仅没有出现,而且在随后的情况下,当灭绝取代了与反应无关的食物递送计划时,啄键现象又出现了。…
{"title":"Extinction-Induced Response Resurgence: A Selective Review","authors":"Adam H. Doughty, Gabriella Oken","doi":"10.1037/H0100644","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/H0100644","url":null,"abstract":"Resurgence refers to the recovery of previously extinguished responding when a recently reinforced response is extinguished (e.g., Epstein, 1983, 1985). Figure 1 illustrates resurgence using hypothetical data. In the first condition, only Response A is reinforced. Response A is extinguished in the second condition concurrent with the reinforcement of Response B. Response B is extinguished in the third condition, and the subsequent recovery of Response A is resurgence. The degree of recovery of Response A can be compared to a different (control) response that never was reinforced (Response C). Measuring Response C in this third condition serves as a measure of extinction-induced response variability (e.g., Antonitis, 1951). Thus, resurgence provides a more refined characterization of extinction-induced behavior. That is, responses predicted to occur in extinction are those responses that previously were reinforced under comparable stimulus conditions. Although resurgence and related extinction-induced phenomena were investigated in the 1970s (e.g., Mulick, Leitenberg, & Rawson, 1976; Pacitti, & Smith, 1977; Rawson, Leitenberg, Mulick, & Lefebvre, 1977), as well as earlier (see Epstein, 1985), Epstein (1983, 1985) usually is viewed as being the most forceful commentator regarding the utility of understanding the determinants and consequences of resurgence (see also Epstein & Skinner, 1980). Epstein argued that resurgence provides behavior analysts with a more descriptive means of understanding findings sometimes attributed to Freud's concept of regression. For Freud, regression involved an individual regressing to previously demonstrated behavior considered acceptable earlier in the individual's development. In addition to linking resurgence and regression, Epstein also summarized much of the experimental findings related to resurgence. The aim of the present paper, therefore, is to describe more recent experimental findings related to resurgence. By showing the relevance of the topic to a variety of topics in and outside behavior analysis (behavioral history, drug relapse, severe problem behavior, communication disorders, and cognition), this paper urges both basic and applied behavior analysts to continue the analysis of resurgence. Behavioral history Appealing to the behavioral history of an organism often provides behavior analysts with a means of explaining complex behavior (e.g., Barrett, 1986; Doughty, Cirino, Mayfield, da Silva, Okouchi, & Lattal, 2005; Lattal, & Neef, 1996; Tatham, & Wanchisen, 1998; Wanchisen, & Tatham, 1991). Broadly speaking, resurgence is a behavioral-history effect in that current behavior only can be understood by appealing to contingencies of reinforcement exposed to the organism previously. A primary issue, therefore, in the study of resurgence is: what are the necessary and sufficient conditions to produce resurgence, in terms of both past experiences and current environmental conditions? Lieving and Lattal (2003)","PeriodicalId":88717,"journal":{"name":"The behavior analyst today","volume":"9 1","pages":"27-33"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2008-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"58471914","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 46
期刊
The behavior analyst today
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1