首页 > 最新文献

Collabra最新文献

英文 中文
“The Effort Heuristic” Revisited: Mixed Results for Replications of Kruger et al. (2004)’s Experiments 1 and 2 “努力启发式”的重新审视:克鲁格等人(2004)的实验1和2的重复混合结果
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.87489
Ignazio Ziano, Siu Kit Yeung, Cheong Shing Lee, Jiaxin Shi, Gilad Feldman
Kruger, Wirtz, van Boven, and Altermatt (2004) described the effort heuristic as the tendency to evaluate the quality and the monetary value of an object as higher if the production of that object was perceived as involving more effort. We attempted two preregistered replications (total N = 1405; U.S. American participants from MTurk and Prolific) of their Experiments 1 and 2. Our first replication using an MTurk sample found support for the original’s findings regarding Experiment 2, yet failed to find support for the original’s findings in Experiment 1. Our second revised attempt of Experiment 1 on Prolific was mixed, with more nuanced findings, showing support for an effort heuristic effect for liking/quality and no support for an effort heuristic on monetary value. We discuss possible reasons for this discrepancy, theoretical implications and future research directions for the psychology of value and the effort heuristic. All materials, data, and code were made available on https://osf.io/qxf5c/.
Kruger, Wirtz, van Boven和Altermatt(2004)将努力启发式描述为,如果一个物体的生产被认为需要更多的努力,那么它的质量和货币价值就会更高。我们尝试了两次预注册重复(总N = 1405;来自MTurk和多产的美国参与者的实验1和2。我们使用MTurk样本进行的第一次复制发现支持了原始实验2的发现,但未能找到原始实验1的发现的支持。我们对实验1的第二次修正尝试是混合的,有更细微的发现,显示了对喜欢/质量的努力启发式效应的支持,而不支持对货币价值的努力启发式效应。本文讨论了价值心理学和努力启发式的可能原因、理论意义和未来的研究方向。所有材料、数据和代码均可在https://osf.io/qxf5c/上获得。
{"title":"“The Effort Heuristic” Revisited: Mixed Results for Replications of Kruger et al. (2004)’s Experiments 1 and 2","authors":"Ignazio Ziano, Siu Kit Yeung, Cheong Shing Lee, Jiaxin Shi, Gilad Feldman","doi":"10.1525/collabra.87489","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.87489","url":null,"abstract":"Kruger, Wirtz, van Boven, and Altermatt (2004) described the effort heuristic as the tendency to evaluate the quality and the monetary value of an object as higher if the production of that object was perceived as involving more effort. We attempted two preregistered replications (total N = 1405; U.S. American participants from MTurk and Prolific) of their Experiments 1 and 2. Our first replication using an MTurk sample found support for the original’s findings regarding Experiment 2, yet failed to find support for the original’s findings in Experiment 1. Our second revised attempt of Experiment 1 on Prolific was mixed, with more nuanced findings, showing support for an effort heuristic effect for liking/quality and no support for an effort heuristic on monetary value. We discuss possible reasons for this discrepancy, theoretical implications and future research directions for the psychology of value and the effort heuristic. All materials, data, and code were made available on https://osf.io/qxf5c/.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135503161","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Effects of Picture Naming and Categorization on Concurrent Comprehension: Evidence From the N400 图片命名和分类对并发理解的影响:来自N400的证据
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.88129
Cecília Hustá, Mante Nieuwland, Antje Meyer
In conversations, interlocutors concurrently perform two related processes: speech comprehension and speech planning. We investigated effects of speech planning on comprehension using EEG. Dutch speakers listened to sentences that ended with expected or unexpected target words. In addition, a picture was presented two seconds after target onset (Experiment 1) or 50 ms before target onset (Experiment 2). Participants’ task was to name the picture or to stay quiet depending on the picture category. In Experiment 1, we found a strong N400 effect in response to unexpected compared to expected target words. Importantly, this N400 effect was reduced in Experiment 2 compared to Experiment 1. Unexpectedly, the N400 effect was not smaller in the naming compared to categorization condition. This indicates that conceptual preparation or the decision whether to speak (taking place in both task conditions of Experiment 2) rather than processes specific to word planning interfere with comprehension.
在对话中,对话者同时进行两个相关的过程:言语理解和言语计划。我们利用脑电图研究言语规划对理解的影响。说荷兰语的人会听以预期或意外目标词结尾的句子。此外,在目标开始后2秒(实验1)或目标开始前50毫秒(实验2)呈现一张图片。参与者的任务是根据图片类别命名图片或保持安静。在实验1中,我们发现意想不到的目标词比预期的目标词具有更强的N400效应。重要的是,与实验1相比,实验2中的N400效应有所降低。出乎意料的是,与分类条件相比,N400效应在命名方面并不小。这表明概念准备或决定是否说话(发生在实验2的两个任务条件下)而不是特定于单词计划的过程干扰理解。
{"title":"Effects of Picture Naming and Categorization on Concurrent Comprehension: Evidence From the N400","authors":"Cecília Hustá, Mante Nieuwland, Antje Meyer","doi":"10.1525/collabra.88129","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.88129","url":null,"abstract":"In conversations, interlocutors concurrently perform two related processes: speech comprehension and speech planning. We investigated effects of speech planning on comprehension using EEG. Dutch speakers listened to sentences that ended with expected or unexpected target words. In addition, a picture was presented two seconds after target onset (Experiment 1) or 50 ms before target onset (Experiment 2). Participants’ task was to name the picture or to stay quiet depending on the picture category. In Experiment 1, we found a strong N400 effect in response to unexpected compared to expected target words. Importantly, this N400 effect was reduced in Experiment 2 compared to Experiment 1. Unexpectedly, the N400 effect was not smaller in the naming compared to categorization condition. This indicates that conceptual preparation or the decision whether to speak (taking place in both task conditions of Experiment 2) rather than processes specific to word planning interfere with comprehension.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136304175","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Having a Positive Attitude or Doing Good Deeds? An Experimental Investigation of Poker Players’ Responses to the Gambling Fallacies Measure 有积极的态度还是做好事?扑克玩家对赌博谬误测量反应的实验研究
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.89007
Philip Newall, Jamie Torrance
Gambling fallacies are irrational beliefs about how gambling works, which are common among disordered gamblers, and measured by questionnaires such as the Gambling Fallacies Measure (GFM). Less is known about the potentially rational cognitions of some skilled gamblers, such as professional poker players. The present research experimentally manipulated item 5 from the GFM, “A positive attitude or doing good deeds increases your likelihood of winning money when gambling”, by comparing two new versions focusing only on a “positive attitude” or “doing good deeds” to the original version (control). Item 5 is scored so that “disagree” is the non-fallacious correct answer, but it was hypothesized that the words “a positive attitude” might increase rates of poker players selecting “agree” in a non-fallacious manner. Online experiments were conducted on samples of professional poker players (N = 379), and a broad sample of poker players with no inclusion criteria (N = 1,510). Participants’ responses to item 5 were associated with the rest of their GFM scores (GFM-9). Participants in both samples were more likely to disagree with the good deeds version, and less likely to disagree with the positive attitude version, compared to control. In comparison to the other conditions, good deeds responses were most strongly associated with GFM-9 scores among professionals, while positive attitude responses were least strongly associated with GFM-9 scores among the broad sample. The good deeds version of item 5 has advantageous measurement properties among professional poker players. New approaches are needed to better understand the potentially rational cognitions of skilled gamblers.
赌博谬误是对赌博如何运作的非理性信念,这在混乱的赌徒中很常见,并通过赌博谬误测量(GFM)等问卷进行测量。对于一些熟练的赌徒(如职业扑克玩家)潜在的理性认知,人们所知甚少。本研究通过比较两个只关注“积极态度”或“做好事”的新版本和原始版本(对照),实验性地操纵了GFM中的第5项“积极态度或做好事会增加你在赌博时赢钱的可能性”。对第5项进行评分,使“不同意”成为非谬误的正确答案,但假设“积极态度”可能会增加扑克玩家以非谬误的方式选择“同意”的比率。在线实验对职业扑克玩家样本(N = 379)和无纳入标准的扑克玩家样本(N = 1510)进行。参与者对第5项的回答与其GFM得分(GFM-9)的其余部分相关。与对照组相比,两个样本的参与者更有可能不同意善行版本,而不太可能不同意积极态度版本。与其他条件相比,专业人士的善行反应与ggm -9得分的相关性最强,而广泛样本的积极态度反应与ggm -9得分的相关性最低。项目5的善行版本在职业扑克玩家中具有优势的测量属性。需要新的方法来更好地理解熟练赌徒潜在的理性认知。
{"title":"Having a Positive Attitude or Doing Good Deeds? An Experimental Investigation of Poker Players’ Responses to the Gambling Fallacies Measure","authors":"Philip Newall, Jamie Torrance","doi":"10.1525/collabra.89007","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.89007","url":null,"abstract":"Gambling fallacies are irrational beliefs about how gambling works, which are common among disordered gamblers, and measured by questionnaires such as the Gambling Fallacies Measure (GFM). Less is known about the potentially rational cognitions of some skilled gamblers, such as professional poker players. The present research experimentally manipulated item 5 from the GFM, “A positive attitude or doing good deeds increases your likelihood of winning money when gambling”, by comparing two new versions focusing only on a “positive attitude” or “doing good deeds” to the original version (control). Item 5 is scored so that “disagree” is the non-fallacious correct answer, but it was hypothesized that the words “a positive attitude” might increase rates of poker players selecting “agree” in a non-fallacious manner. Online experiments were conducted on samples of professional poker players (N = 379), and a broad sample of poker players with no inclusion criteria (N = 1,510). Participants’ responses to item 5 were associated with the rest of their GFM scores (GFM-9). Participants in both samples were more likely to disagree with the good deeds version, and less likely to disagree with the positive attitude version, compared to control. In comparison to the other conditions, good deeds responses were most strongly associated with GFM-9 scores among professionals, while positive attitude responses were least strongly associated with GFM-9 scores among the broad sample. The good deeds version of item 5 has advantageous measurement properties among professional poker players. New approaches are needed to better understand the potentially rational cognitions of skilled gamblers.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135010328","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Interplay of Time-of-day and Chronotype Results in No General and Robust Cognitive Boost 一天中的时间和时间类型的相互作用不会产生普遍和强劲的认知提升
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.88337
Alodie Rey-Mermet, Nicolas Rothen
Using physiologically validated questionnaires in which the peak of circadian arousal is determined through morningness-eveningness preferences, individuals can be categorized into morning or evening chronotypes. Typically, individuals with such chronotypes are assumed to show better cognitive performance at their subjective peak of circadian arousal than at off peak. Although this so-called synchrony effect is accepted as common knowledge, empirical evidence is rather mixed. This may be explained by two methodical challenges. First, most studies are underpowered. Second, they include one task, but tasks differ across studies. Here, we tested the synchrony effect by focusing on two cognitive constructs that are assumed to underlie a wide variety of behaviors, that is: short-term maintenance and attentional control. Short-term maintenance refers to our ability to maintain information temporarily. Attentional control refers to our ability to avoid being distracted by irrelevant information. We addressed the methodical challenges by asking 446 young adults to perform eight tasks at on- and off-peak times. Four tasks were used to assess temporary maintenance of information (i.e., short-term memory). Four tasks were used to assess temporary maintenance and manipulation of information (i.e., working memory). Using structural equation modeling, we modeled attentional control as the goal-directed nature of the working-memory tasks without their maintenance aspects. At the individual-task level, there was some evidence for a synchrony effect. However, the evidence was weak and limited to two tasks. Moreover, at the latent-variable level, the results showed no evidence for a robust and general synchrony effect. These results were observed for the full sample (N = 446) and the subsample including participants with moderate to definite morning or evening chronotypes (N = 191). We conclude that the synchrony effect is most likely a methodical artefact and discuss the impact of our research on psychological science and scientific research more widely.
通过生理上有效的问卷调查,通过对早睡晚睡的偏好来确定昼夜节律觉醒的高峰,个体可以被分为早晨型和晚上型。通常情况下,具有这种时间类型的个体被认为在他们主观的昼夜节律觉醒高峰时比在非高峰时表现出更好的认知表现。虽然这种所谓的同步效应被认为是常识,但经验证据却相当复杂。这可以用两个方法上的挑战来解释。首先,大多数研究的力度不足。其次,它们包括一个任务,但不同研究的任务不同。在这里,我们通过关注两种认知结构来测试同步效应,这两种认知结构被认为是多种行为的基础,即短期维持和注意力控制。短期维护是指我们暂时维护信息的能力。注意力控制是指我们避免被无关信息分散注意力的能力。我们通过要求446名年轻人在高峰时段和非高峰时段执行八项任务来解决有条不紊的挑战。四个任务被用来评估信息的临时维持(即短期记忆)。四个任务被用来评估信息的临时维持和操作(即工作记忆)。利用结构方程模型,我们将注意力控制建模为工作记忆任务的目标导向性质,而不考虑其维持方面。在个人任务层面上,有一些证据表明存在同步效应。然而,证据不足,而且仅限于两项任务。此外,在潜在变量水平上,结果显示没有证据表明存在稳健和普遍的同步效应。这些结果是在整个样本(N = 446)和子样本(N = 191)中观察到的,子样本包括中度到明确的早晨或晚上时间类型的参与者。我们的结论是,同步效应很可能是一种有条理的人工产物,并讨论了我们的研究对心理科学和更广泛的科学研究的影响。
{"title":"The Interplay of Time-of-day and Chronotype Results in No General and Robust Cognitive Boost","authors":"Alodie Rey-Mermet, Nicolas Rothen","doi":"10.1525/collabra.88337","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.88337","url":null,"abstract":"Using physiologically validated questionnaires in which the peak of circadian arousal is determined through morningness-eveningness preferences, individuals can be categorized into morning or evening chronotypes. Typically, individuals with such chronotypes are assumed to show better cognitive performance at their subjective peak of circadian arousal than at off peak. Although this so-called synchrony effect is accepted as common knowledge, empirical evidence is rather mixed. This may be explained by two methodical challenges. First, most studies are underpowered. Second, they include one task, but tasks differ across studies. Here, we tested the synchrony effect by focusing on two cognitive constructs that are assumed to underlie a wide variety of behaviors, that is: short-term maintenance and attentional control. Short-term maintenance refers to our ability to maintain information temporarily. Attentional control refers to our ability to avoid being distracted by irrelevant information. We addressed the methodical challenges by asking 446 young adults to perform eight tasks at on- and off-peak times. Four tasks were used to assess temporary maintenance of information (i.e., short-term memory). Four tasks were used to assess temporary maintenance and manipulation of information (i.e., working memory). Using structural equation modeling, we modeled attentional control as the goal-directed nature of the working-memory tasks without their maintenance aspects. At the individual-task level, there was some evidence for a synchrony effect. However, the evidence was weak and limited to two tasks. Moreover, at the latent-variable level, the results showed no evidence for a robust and general synchrony effect. These results were observed for the full sample (N = 446) and the subsample including participants with moderate to definite morning or evening chronotypes (N = 191). We conclude that the synchrony effect is most likely a methodical artefact and discuss the impact of our research on psychological science and scientific research more widely.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135156480","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Linearly-Additive Decomposed 2 × 2 Games: A Primer for Research 线性加性分解的2 × 2游戏:研究入门
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.84916
Benjamin James Kuper-Smith, Christoph W. Korn
2 × 2 games (such as the Prisoner’s Dilemma) are economic games for studying cooperation and social decision-making. Linearly-additive decomposed games are variants of 2 × 2 games that can change the framing of the game and thereby provide researchers with additional flexibility for measuring preferences and social cognition that would not be possible with standard (matrix-form) 2 × 2 games. In this paper, we provide a systematic overview of linearly-additive decomposed 2 × 2 games. We show which 2 × 2 games can be decomposed in a linearly-additive way and how to calculate possible decompositions for a given game. We close by suggesting for which experiments decomposed games might be more conducive than matrix games.
2 × 2博弈(如囚徒困境)是研究合作和社会决策的经济博弈。线性加性分解游戏是2x2游戏的变体,可以改变游戏的框架,从而为研究人员提供额外的灵活性来测量偏好和社会认知,这是标准(矩阵形式)2x2游戏无法做到的。本文系统地介绍了线性加性分解的2 × 2对策。我们展示了哪些2x2博弈可以用线性加性的方式分解,以及如何计算给定博弈的可能分解。最后,我们提出分解博弈可能比矩阵博弈更有利于实验。
{"title":"Linearly-Additive Decomposed 2 × 2 Games: A Primer for Research","authors":"Benjamin James Kuper-Smith, Christoph W. Korn","doi":"10.1525/collabra.84916","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.84916","url":null,"abstract":"2 × 2 games (such as the Prisoner’s Dilemma) are economic games for studying cooperation and social decision-making. Linearly-additive decomposed games are variants of 2 × 2 games that can change the framing of the game and thereby provide researchers with additional flexibility for measuring preferences and social cognition that would not be possible with standard (matrix-form) 2 × 2 games. In this paper, we provide a systematic overview of linearly-additive decomposed 2 × 2 games. We show which 2 × 2 games can be decomposed in a linearly-additive way and how to calculate possible decompositions for a given game. We close by suggesting for which experiments decomposed games might be more conducive than matrix games.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135440171","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Common Is Undergraduate Publication in Psychology? An Examination of Faculty Vitae From Top Colleges and Universities 大学生发表心理学论文有多普遍?顶尖高校教师简历考试
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.84521
Traci A. Giuliano, William I. Hebl, Jennifer L. Howell
Despite extensive research on the benefits of faculty-undergraduate collaborative research, little is known about the outcomes of such collaborations, such as coauthored publication in peer-reviewed journals. To address this gap, we analyzed faculty curricula vitae from 60 top U.S. institutions (20 primarily undergraduate, 20 masters-granting, and 20 research-intensive) to obtain estimates of the prevalence of faculty-undergraduate coauthored publication in psychology. We sent email requests to 8 randomly-selected faculty members from each psychology department in this top 60 (or to the whole department if they had fewer than 8 members). In total, 157 of 459 faculty contacted (34.2%) responded with their full vita. Our results revealed that a substantial majority of faculty (83.4%) had coauthored at least one paper with an undergraduate, with an average of 7.5 coauthored publications (approximately 2 of which were first authored by an undergraduate). Moreover, these publications involved an average of 12.6 undergraduate coauthors (typically in the 2nd or 3rd author position) and accounted for almost 20% of faculty members’ total publications. We also found that, even controlling for overall productivity, faculty of higher rank and those at primarily undergraduate institutions generally coauthored more undergraduate publications compared to faculty of lower rank and/or at masters-granting and especially research-intensive universities. Finally, an analysis of publication trends over time showed that undergraduate publication is becoming increasingly common in psychology, and that faculty are publishing with undergraduates earlier in their careers. We hope our findings inspire more faculty to publish with their undergraduate students.
尽管对教师与本科生合作研究的好处进行了广泛的研究,但对这种合作的结果知之甚少,例如在同行评议的期刊上共同发表论文。为了解决这一差距,我们分析了美国60所顶尖大学(20所主要是本科生,20所授予硕士学位,20所研究密集型大学)的教师简历,以估计教师与本科生合著的心理学出版物的流行程度。我们从排名前60位的每个心理学系随机选择了8名教师(如果少于8人,则向整个系发送电子邮件)。在我们联系的459名教师中,总共有157名(34.2%)回复了他们的完整简历。我们的研究结果显示,绝大多数教师(83.4%)与本科生合作撰写了至少一篇论文,平均发表了7.5篇论文(其中约2篇是由本科生首次发表的)。此外,这些出版物平均涉及12.6名本科生共同作者(通常位于第二或第三作者位置),几乎占教职员工总出版物的20%。我们还发现,即使控制整体生产力,与排名较低和/或硕士授予大学,特别是研究密集型大学的教师相比,排名较高的教师和主要本科院校的教师通常共同撰写了更多的本科出版物。最后,对一段时间以来出版趋势的分析表明,本科生发表论文在心理学领域正变得越来越普遍,而且教师在他们职业生涯的早期就与本科生一起发表论文。我们希望我们的发现能激励更多的教师和他们的本科生一起发表文章。
{"title":"How Common Is Undergraduate Publication in Psychology? An Examination of Faculty Vitae From Top Colleges and Universities","authors":"Traci A. Giuliano, William I. Hebl, Jennifer L. Howell","doi":"10.1525/collabra.84521","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.84521","url":null,"abstract":"Despite extensive research on the benefits of faculty-undergraduate collaborative research, little is known about the outcomes of such collaborations, such as coauthored publication in peer-reviewed journals. To address this gap, we analyzed faculty curricula vitae from 60 top U.S. institutions (20 primarily undergraduate, 20 masters-granting, and 20 research-intensive) to obtain estimates of the prevalence of faculty-undergraduate coauthored publication in psychology. We sent email requests to 8 randomly-selected faculty members from each psychology department in this top 60 (or to the whole department if they had fewer than 8 members). In total, 157 of 459 faculty contacted (34.2%) responded with their full vita. Our results revealed that a substantial majority of faculty (83.4%) had coauthored at least one paper with an undergraduate, with an average of 7.5 coauthored publications (approximately 2 of which were first authored by an undergraduate). Moreover, these publications involved an average of 12.6 undergraduate coauthors (typically in the 2nd or 3rd author position) and accounted for almost 20% of faculty members’ total publications. We also found that, even controlling for overall productivity, faculty of higher rank and those at primarily undergraduate institutions generally coauthored more undergraduate publications compared to faculty of lower rank and/or at masters-granting and especially research-intensive universities. Finally, an analysis of publication trends over time showed that undergraduate publication is becoming increasingly common in psychology, and that faculty are publishing with undergraduates earlier in their careers. We hope our findings inspire more faculty to publish with their undergraduate students.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135502393","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unfamiliar Contexts Compared to Familiar Contexts Impair Learning in Humans 与熟悉的环境相比,不熟悉的环境会损害人类的学习能力
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.71316
Marjan Alizadeh Asfestani, Juliane Nagel, Sina Beer, Ghazaleh Nikpourian, Jan Born, Gordon B. Feld
Re-exposure to the context that information was learned in facilitates its memory retrieval. However, the influence of context changes on the ability to learn new information is less well understood, which the present work investigated in two experiments with healthy participants (n = 40 per experiment; 20 female). In experiment 1, participants learned a list of word-pairs (A-B) in the morning, after which their memory for the word-pairs was immediately tested. In the evening, they learned and were tested on a second non-overlapping list (C-D), either in the same context or in a different context than the first list (between-subjects). We found that new learning is enhanced in the same context, and that new learning in the other context was decreased compared to baseline. In experiment 2, participants were exposed to both contexts in the morning, but only learned word-pairs in one of them. In the second learning session in the evening, this familiarization with the other context abolished differences between the same and other context group. These data point to context novelty interfering with new learning rather than context familiarity enhancing it. Importantly, the reduction of new learning in the other context in the first experiment, where the context was unfamiliar in both learning sessions, suggests mechanisms beyond attention processes that are bound by the novelty of the other context. Rather, the old context impairs the processing of the new context, possibly by biasing pattern completion and pattern separation trade-offs within the hippocampus.
重新暴露在信息学习的环境中有助于记忆检索。然而,环境变化对学习新信息能力的影响尚不清楚,目前的工作在两个实验中对健康参与者进行了调查(每个实验n = 40;20个女性)。在实验1中,参与者在早上学习了一组单词(a - b),之后立即测试了他们对这些单词的记忆。在晚上,他们学习并在第二个不重叠列表(C-D)上进行测试,要么在与第一个列表相同的环境中,要么在不同的环境中(受试者之间)。我们发现,在相同的环境中,新学习得到了加强,而在另一个环境中,新学习与基线相比有所减少。在实验2中,参与者在早上接触了两种情境,但只学习了其中一种情境中的成对单词。在晚上的第二次学习中,这种对其他情境的熟悉消除了相同情境组和其他情境组之间的差异。这些数据表明,背景新颖性会干扰新学习,而不是背景熟悉性会增强新学习。重要的是,在第一个实验中,在两个学习阶段的情境都不熟悉的情况下,在其他情境中新学习的减少,表明了受其他情境新颖性约束的注意力过程之外的机制。相反,旧的情境会损害新情境的处理,这可能是海马体内模式完成和模式分离权衡的偏误。
{"title":"Unfamiliar Contexts Compared to Familiar Contexts Impair Learning in Humans","authors":"Marjan Alizadeh Asfestani, Juliane Nagel, Sina Beer, Ghazaleh Nikpourian, Jan Born, Gordon B. Feld","doi":"10.1525/collabra.71316","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.71316","url":null,"abstract":"Re-exposure to the context that information was learned in facilitates its memory retrieval. However, the influence of context changes on the ability to learn new information is less well understood, which the present work investigated in two experiments with healthy participants (n = 40 per experiment; 20 female). In experiment 1, participants learned a list of word-pairs (A-B) in the morning, after which their memory for the word-pairs was immediately tested. In the evening, they learned and were tested on a second non-overlapping list (C-D), either in the same context or in a different context than the first list (between-subjects). We found that new learning is enhanced in the same context, and that new learning in the other context was decreased compared to baseline. In experiment 2, participants were exposed to both contexts in the morning, but only learned word-pairs in one of them. In the second learning session in the evening, this familiarization with the other context abolished differences between the same and other context group. These data point to context novelty interfering with new learning rather than context familiarity enhancing it. Importantly, the reduction of new learning in the other context in the first experiment, where the context was unfamiliar in both learning sessions, suggests mechanisms beyond attention processes that are bound by the novelty of the other context. Rather, the old context impairs the processing of the new context, possibly by biasing pattern completion and pattern separation trade-offs within the hippocampus.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135584282","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Mental Number Representations Are Spatially Mapped Both by Their Magnitudes and Ordinal Positions 心数表征是由它们的大小和序数位置在空间上映射的
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.67908
Nadine N. Koch, Julia F. Huber, Johannes Lohmann, Krzysztof Cipora, Martin V. Butz, Hans-Christoph Nuerk
The Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) effect – i.e., faster responses to small numbers with the left compared to the right side and to large numbers with the right compared to the left side – suggests that numbers are associated with space. However, it remains unclear whether the SNARC effect evolves from a number’s magnitude or the ordinal position of a number in working memory. One problem is that, in different paradigms, the task demands influence the role of ordinality and magnitude. While single-task setups in which participants judge the parity of a displayed number indicate the importance of magnitude for the SNARC effect, evidence for ordinal influences usually comes from experiments where ordinal sequences have to be memorized or setups in which participants possess pre-existing knowledge of the ordinality of stimuli. Therefore, in this preregistered study, we employed a SNARC task without secondary ordinal sequence memorization. We dissociate ordinal and magnitude accounts by carefully manipulating experimental stimulus sets. The results indicate that even though the magnitude model better accounts for the observed data, the ordinal position seems to matter as well. Hence, numbers are associated with space in both a magnitude- and an order-respective manner, yielding a mixture of both compatibility effects. Moreover, a multiple coding framework may most accurately explain the roots of the SNARC effect.
反应代码的空间-数字关联效应(SNARC)表明,数字与空间有关——也就是说,用左手比用右手更快地反应小的数字,用右手比用左手更快地反应大的数字。然而,SNARC效应是由数字的大小还是数字在工作记忆中的顺序位置演变而来,目前还不清楚。一个问题是,在不同的范式中,任务需求的影响作用是普通性和重要性。在单任务设置中,参与者判断所显示的数字的奇偶性表明了SNARC效应的重要性,而序数影响的证据通常来自于必须记住序数序列的实验或参与者对刺激的序数性具有预先存在的知识的实验。因此,在这个预注册的研究中,我们采用了一个没有二次有序序列记忆的SNARC任务。我们通过小心地操纵实验刺激集来分离序数和数量级。结果表明,即使星等模型更好地解释了观测数据,序数位置似乎也很重要。因此,数字以大小和顺序的方式与空间相关联,从而产生两种兼容效果的混合。此外,多重编码框架可能最准确地解释SNARC效应的根源。
{"title":"Mental Number Representations Are Spatially Mapped Both by Their Magnitudes and Ordinal Positions","authors":"Nadine N. Koch, Julia F. Huber, Johannes Lohmann, Krzysztof Cipora, Martin V. Butz, Hans-Christoph Nuerk","doi":"10.1525/collabra.67908","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.67908","url":null,"abstract":"The Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) effect – i.e., faster responses to small numbers with the left compared to the right side and to large numbers with the right compared to the left side – suggests that numbers are associated with space. However, it remains unclear whether the SNARC effect evolves from a number’s magnitude or the ordinal position of a number in working memory. One problem is that, in different paradigms, the task demands influence the role of ordinality and magnitude. While single-task setups in which participants judge the parity of a displayed number indicate the importance of magnitude for the SNARC effect, evidence for ordinal influences usually comes from experiments where ordinal sequences have to be memorized or setups in which participants possess pre-existing knowledge of the ordinality of stimuli. Therefore, in this preregistered study, we employed a SNARC task without secondary ordinal sequence memorization. We dissociate ordinal and magnitude accounts by carefully manipulating experimental stimulus sets. The results indicate that even though the magnitude model better accounts for the observed data, the ordinal position seems to matter as well. Hence, numbers are associated with space in both a magnitude- and an order-respective manner, yielding a mixture of both compatibility effects. Moreover, a multiple coding framework may most accurately explain the roots of the SNARC effect.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136298133","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Explaining Why Headlines Are True or False Reduces Intentions to Share False Information 解释为什么标题是真的或假的减少了分享虚假信息的意图
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.87617
Raunak M. Pillai, Lisa K. Fazio
Recent years have seen a growing interest among academics and the public in ways to curb the spread of misinformation on social media. A recent experiment demonstrated that explanation prompts—simply asking people to explain why they think information is true or false—can reduce intentions to share false, but not true, political headlines on social media (Fazio, 2020). However, there is currently only one experiment demonstrating the benefits of this intervention, and this experiment manipulated the treatment between-subjects, raising concerns about differential attrition across the treatment and control groups over the course of the experiment. Thus, the present experiment (N = 499 US MTurkers) replicates Fazio (2020) in a within-subjects design, with all participants taking part in both the treatment and control conditions in two successive blocks. We replicate the effect of the intervention—explaining why headlines were true or false selectively reduced intentions to share false headlines. Our results also reveal that the longevity of the impact of these prompts is limited—encountering the explanation prompts did not reduce subsequent intentions to share false information when the explanation prompts were removed. Overall, our results suggest that encouraging people to pause and think about the truth of information can improve the quality of user-shared information on social media.
近年来,学术界和公众对遏制社交媒体上错误信息传播的方式越来越感兴趣。最近的一项实验表明,解释提示——简单地要求人们解释为什么他们认为信息是真的或假的——可以减少在社交媒体上分享虚假而不是真实的政治头条的意图(Fazio, 2020)。然而,目前只有一个实验证明了这种干预的好处,并且这个实验操纵了受试者之间的治疗,在实验过程中引起了对治疗组和对照组之间差异损耗的关注。因此,本实验(N = 499名美国MTurkers)在受试者内设计中重复了Fazio(2020),所有参与者在两个连续的区块中同时参加了治疗和控制条件。我们复制了干预的效果,解释了为什么标题是真实的或虚假的,选择性地减少了分享虚假标题的意图。我们的研究结果还表明,这些提示的影响的持续时间是有限的——当解释提示被删除时,遇到解释提示并没有减少随后分享虚假信息的意图。总的来说,我们的研究结果表明,鼓励人们停下来思考信息的真相可以提高社交媒体上用户共享信息的质量。
{"title":"Explaining Why Headlines Are True or False Reduces Intentions to Share False Information","authors":"Raunak M. Pillai, Lisa K. Fazio","doi":"10.1525/collabra.87617","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.87617","url":null,"abstract":"Recent years have seen a growing interest among academics and the public in ways to curb the spread of misinformation on social media. A recent experiment demonstrated that explanation prompts—simply asking people to explain why they think information is true or false—can reduce intentions to share false, but not true, political headlines on social media (Fazio, 2020). However, there is currently only one experiment demonstrating the benefits of this intervention, and this experiment manipulated the treatment between-subjects, raising concerns about differential attrition across the treatment and control groups over the course of the experiment. Thus, the present experiment (N = 499 US MTurkers) replicates Fazio (2020) in a within-subjects design, with all participants taking part in both the treatment and control conditions in two successive blocks. We replicate the effect of the intervention—explaining why headlines were true or false selectively reduced intentions to share false headlines. Our results also reveal that the longevity of the impact of these prompts is limited—encountering the explanation prompts did not reduce subsequent intentions to share false information when the explanation prompts were removed. Overall, our results suggest that encouraging people to pause and think about the truth of information can improve the quality of user-shared information on social media.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135403667","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Unfulfilled Promise of Longitudinal Designs for Causal Inference 因果推理的纵向设计的未实现的承诺
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.89142
Wen Wei Loh, Dongning Ren
Longitudinal designs are frequently used in psychological research. An intuitive analytic approach is to adjust for previous measurements to bolster the validity of causal conclusions when estimating the effect of a focal predictor (i.e., treatment) on an outcome. This approach is routinely applied but rarely substantiated in practice. What are the implications of adjusting for previous measurements? Does it necessarily improve causal inferences? In this paper, we demonstrate that answers to these questions are far from straightforward. We explain how adjusting for previous measurements can reduce or induce bias in common longitudinal scenarios. We further demonstrate, in scenarios with less stringent causal assumptions, adjusting or not adjusting for previous measurements can induce bias one way or the other. Put differently, adjusting or not adjusting for a previous measurement can simultaneously strengthen and undermine causal inferences from longitudinal research, even in the simplest scenarios. We urge researchers to overcome the unwarranted complacency brought on by using longitudinal designs to test causality. Practical recommendations for strengthening causal conclusions in psychology research are provided.
纵向设计常用于心理学研究。一种直观的分析方法是在估计焦点预测因子(即治疗)对结果的影响时,对先前的测量进行调整,以支持因果结论的有效性。这种方法是常规应用,但很少在实践中得到证实。调整以前的测量值意味着什么?它一定能改善因果推论吗?在本文中,我们证明了这些问题的答案远非直截了当。我们解释如何调整以前的测量可以减少或诱导偏差在常见的纵向方案。我们进一步证明,在因果假设不太严格的情况下,调整或不调整先前的测量结果可能会以某种方式引起偏差。换句话说,即使在最简单的情况下,调整或不调整先前的测量结果也会同时加强或破坏纵向研究的因果推论。我们敦促研究人员克服使用纵向设计来测试因果关系所带来的毫无根据的自满情绪。提出了加强心理学研究中因果结论的实用建议。
{"title":"The Unfulfilled Promise of Longitudinal Designs for Causal Inference","authors":"Wen Wei Loh, Dongning Ren","doi":"10.1525/collabra.89142","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.89142","url":null,"abstract":"Longitudinal designs are frequently used in psychological research. An intuitive analytic approach is to adjust for previous measurements to bolster the validity of causal conclusions when estimating the effect of a focal predictor (i.e., treatment) on an outcome. This approach is routinely applied but rarely substantiated in practice. What are the implications of adjusting for previous measurements? Does it necessarily improve causal inferences? In this paper, we demonstrate that answers to these questions are far from straightforward. We explain how adjusting for previous measurements can reduce or induce bias in common longitudinal scenarios. We further demonstrate, in scenarios with less stringent causal assumptions, adjusting or not adjusting for previous measurements can induce bias one way or the other. Put differently, adjusting or not adjusting for a previous measurement can simultaneously strengthen and undermine causal inferences from longitudinal research, even in the simplest scenarios. We urge researchers to overcome the unwarranted complacency brought on by using longitudinal designs to test causality. Practical recommendations for strengthening causal conclusions in psychology research are provided.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135502277","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Collabra
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1