首页 > 最新文献

Collabra最新文献

英文 中文
The Hazards of Daily Stressors: Comparing the Experiences of Sexual and Gender Minority Young Adults to Cisgender Heterosexual Young Adults During the COVID-19 Pandemic 日常压力源的危害:在COVID-19大流行期间,性少数和性别少数年轻人与顺性异性恋年轻人的经历比较
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.73649
Jessica P. Lougheed, Gizem Keskin, Sean Morgan
Some individuals may be at greater risk for encountering stressors in daily life than others, especially those with minority identities. Initial evidence shows that the disparities between cisgender heterosexual (CH) individuals and sexual and gender minority (SGM) individuals on stress-related experiences may be exacerbated by the COVID-19 pandemic. We examined the daily stressors experienced by undergraduate students during the COVID-19 pandemic (stressor exposure), the association between the experience of daily stress and same-day negative mood (stressor reactivity), and whether these varied between undergraduate students with SGM identities and their CH counterparts using a 14-day daily diary design. We did not find significant differences between SGM and CH groups on stressor exposure or stressor reactivity. One common feature of daily diary data is right censoring, which is when some individuals do not experience specific events during the study duration. We used multilevel survival analysis, which accounts for right censored data, to examine group differences in the risks of stressor exposure. We discuss the statistical issues involved when right-censored cases are not taken into consideration in studies of stressor exposure and propose multilevel survival analysis as one solution to move the field towards more accurately understanding whether, when, and why SGM individuals are at greater risk for stressors.
有些人在日常生活中遇到压力源的风险可能比其他人更大,尤其是那些少数族裔。初步证据表明,顺性异性恋(CH)个体与性少数群体(SGM)个体在压力相关经历方面的差异可能会因COVID-19大流行而加剧。我们使用为期14天的每日日记设计,研究了2019冠状病毒病大流行期间本科生所经历的日常压力源(压力源暴露)、日常压力体验与当日负面情绪(压力源反应)之间的关联,以及这些因素在SGM身份的本科生与CH身份的本科生之间是否存在差异。我们没有发现SGM组和CH组在应激源暴露或应激源反应性上有显著差异。日常日记数据的一个共同特征是“正确审查”,即当一些人在研究期间没有经历特定事件时。我们使用了多级生存分析,该分析解释了正确审查的数据,以检查应激源暴露风险的组间差异。我们讨论了当在压力源暴露研究中没有考虑到右审查病例时所涉及的统计问题,并提出多层次生存分析作为一种解决方案,以使该领域更准确地理解SGM个体是否,何时以及为什么面临更大的压力源风险。
{"title":"The Hazards of Daily Stressors: Comparing the Experiences of Sexual and Gender Minority Young Adults to Cisgender Heterosexual Young Adults During the COVID-19 Pandemic","authors":"Jessica P. Lougheed, Gizem Keskin, Sean Morgan","doi":"10.1525/collabra.73649","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.73649","url":null,"abstract":"Some individuals may be at greater risk for encountering stressors in daily life than others, especially those with minority identities. Initial evidence shows that the disparities between cisgender heterosexual (CH) individuals and sexual and gender minority (SGM) individuals on stress-related experiences may be exacerbated by the COVID-19 pandemic. We examined the daily stressors experienced by undergraduate students during the COVID-19 pandemic (stressor exposure), the association between the experience of daily stress and same-day negative mood (stressor reactivity), and whether these varied between undergraduate students with SGM identities and their CH counterparts using a 14-day daily diary design. We did not find significant differences between SGM and CH groups on stressor exposure or stressor reactivity. One common feature of daily diary data is right censoring, which is when some individuals do not experience specific events during the study duration. We used multilevel survival analysis, which accounts for right censored data, to examine group differences in the risks of stressor exposure. We discuss the statistical issues involved when right-censored cases are not taken into consideration in studies of stressor exposure and propose multilevel survival analysis as one solution to move the field towards more accurately understanding whether, when, and why SGM individuals are at greater risk for stressors.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135585530","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Individual Difference Correlates of Being Sexually Unrestricted Yet Declining an HIV Test 性行为不受限制但拒绝艾滋病毒检测的个体差异
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.87458
Nicholas S. Holtzman, Stephen W. Carden, Stacy W. Smallwood, Janice N. Steirn, S. Mason Garrison, Amanda N. Gesselman
Which individual differences accurately predict one’s decision to get tested for human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), and do individuals who have regular short-term sex get tested at higher rates? Two studies—one lab study (total valid N = 69, with n = 20 who were tested) and one involving a student health center (valid N = 250, n = 4 who were tested)—involved participants (total valid N = 319, with n = 24 who got tested) taking a number of personality and individual difference measures, including the dark triad (Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy). Then, in both studies, participants had the opportunity to actually get tested for HIV. After analyzing data from Study 1, for Study 2 we preregistered the prediction that narcissistic participants would tend to (a) show disinclination to get tested for HIV, and (b) show proclivity for unrestricted short-term sexual behavior, manifesting in (c) a significant difference between these two correlations. As predicted, such a difference in correlations was evident for narcissism as well as psychopathy (the latter, however, was not predicted), suggesting that such individuals are not likely to seek HIV diagnostic information, but are taking more sexual risks. A research synthesis was consistent with these ideas (although controlling for demographic factors diminished the effects). Narcissistic and psychopathic individuals may be undetected hubs in the network of sexually active individuals with HIV. These results are silent on whether the typical HIV patient is narcissistic or psychopathic; the results merely implicate narcissistic and psychopathic traits in the spread of the virus.
哪些个体差异准确地预测了一个人是否决定接受人类免疫缺陷病毒(HIV)检测?有规律的短期性行为的个体是否有更高的检测率?两项研究——一项是实验室研究(总有效N = 69,有N = 20被测试者),另一项是涉及学生健康中心的研究(有效N = 250,有N = 4被测试者)——涉及参与者(总有效N = 319,有N = 24被测试者),采取了一些人格和个体差异的测量,包括黑暗三合人格(马基雅维利主义、自恋和精神病)。然后,在这两项研究中,参与者都有机会接受艾滋病毒检测。在分析了研究1的数据之后,在研究2中,我们预登记了自恋参与者倾向于(a)表现出不愿意接受艾滋病毒检测,(b)表现出不受限制的短期性行为的倾向,在(c)中表现出这两种相关性之间的显著差异。正如预测的那样,这种相关性的差异在自恋者和精神病患者中都很明显(然而,后者没有被预测到),这表明这些人不太可能寻求艾滋病诊断信息,但却冒着更大的性风险。一项综合研究与这些观点一致(尽管控制人口因素减少了影响)。自恋者和精神病患者可能是性活跃的艾滋病毒感染者网络中未被发现的中心。这些结果并没有说明典型的艾滋病患者是自恋型还是精神病患者;研究结果仅仅暗示了病毒传播的自恋和精神病特征。
{"title":"Individual Difference Correlates of Being Sexually Unrestricted Yet Declining an HIV Test","authors":"Nicholas S. Holtzman, Stephen W. Carden, Stacy W. Smallwood, Janice N. Steirn, S. Mason Garrison, Amanda N. Gesselman","doi":"10.1525/collabra.87458","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.87458","url":null,"abstract":"Which individual differences accurately predict one’s decision to get tested for human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), and do individuals who have regular short-term sex get tested at higher rates? Two studies—one lab study (total valid N = 69, with n = 20 who were tested) and one involving a student health center (valid N = 250, n = 4 who were tested)—involved participants (total valid N = 319, with n = 24 who got tested) taking a number of personality and individual difference measures, including the dark triad (Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy). Then, in both studies, participants had the opportunity to actually get tested for HIV. After analyzing data from Study 1, for Study 2 we preregistered the prediction that narcissistic participants would tend to (a) show disinclination to get tested for HIV, and (b) show proclivity for unrestricted short-term sexual behavior, manifesting in (c) a significant difference between these two correlations. As predicted, such a difference in correlations was evident for narcissism as well as psychopathy (the latter, however, was not predicted), suggesting that such individuals are not likely to seek HIV diagnostic information, but are taking more sexual risks. A research synthesis was consistent with these ideas (although controlling for demographic factors diminished the effects). Narcissistic and psychopathic individuals may be undetected hubs in the network of sexually active individuals with HIV. These results are silent on whether the typical HIV patient is narcissistic or psychopathic; the results merely implicate narcissistic and psychopathic traits in the spread of the virus.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135403415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Brief Note on the Standard Error of the Pearson Correlation 关于皮尔逊相关标准误差的简要说明
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.87615
Timo Gnambs
The product-moment correlation is a central statistic in psychological research including meta-analysis. Unfortunately, it has a rather complex sampling distribution which leads to sample correlations that are biased indicators of the respective population correlations. Moreover, there seems to be some uncertainty on how to properly calculate the standard error of these correlations. Because no simple analytical solution exists, several approximations have been previously introduced. This note aims to briefly summarize 10 different ways to calculate the standard error of the Pearson correlation. Moreover, a simulation study on the accuracy of these estimators compared their relative percentage biases for different population correlations and sample sizes. The results showed that all estimators were largely unbiased for sample sizes of at least 40. For smaller samples, a simple approximation by Bonett (2008) led to the least biased results. Based on these results, it is recommended to use the expression (1−r2)/N−3 for the calculation of the standard error of the Pearson correlation.
积矩相关是包括元分析在内的心理学研究的中心统计量。不幸的是,它有一个相当复杂的抽样分布,导致样本相关性是各自总体相关性的有偏指标。此外,如何正确计算这些相关性的标准误差似乎存在一些不确定性。由于不存在简单的解析解,所以前面介绍了几种近似方法。本文旨在简要总结计算Pearson相关标准误差的10种不同方法。此外,对这些估计器的准确性进行了模拟研究,比较了它们在不同人口相关性和样本量下的相对百分比偏差。结果表明,对于至少40个样本量,所有估计量基本上都是无偏的。对于较小的样本,Bonett(2008)的简单近似导致偏差最小的结果。根据这些结果,建议使用表达式(1−r2)/N−3来计算Pearson相关的标准误差。
{"title":"A Brief Note on the Standard Error of the Pearson Correlation","authors":"Timo Gnambs","doi":"10.1525/collabra.87615","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.87615","url":null,"abstract":"The product-moment correlation is a central statistic in psychological research including meta-analysis. Unfortunately, it has a rather complex sampling distribution which leads to sample correlations that are biased indicators of the respective population correlations. Moreover, there seems to be some uncertainty on how to properly calculate the standard error of these correlations. Because no simple analytical solution exists, several approximations have been previously introduced. This note aims to briefly summarize 10 different ways to calculate the standard error of the Pearson correlation. Moreover, a simulation study on the accuracy of these estimators compared their relative percentage biases for different population correlations and sample sizes. The results showed that all estimators were largely unbiased for sample sizes of at least 40. For smaller samples, a simple approximation by Bonett (2008) led to the least biased results. Based on these results, it is recommended to use the expression (1−r2)/N−3 for the calculation of the standard error of the Pearson correlation.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135403938","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
COVID-19 and the Perceived Dangerousness of Everyday Objects: A Behavioural Online Study in Italy and Germany COVID-19和日常物品的感知危险:意大利和德国的一项行为在线研究
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.89171
Claudia Gianelli, Katharina Kühne, Alex Miklashevsky, Melinda Jeglinski-Mende, Nicola Canessa, Anna M. Borghi
The outbreak of COVID-19 and efforts to contain it have likely impacted our daily lives, including how we interact with objects. This online study aimed to develop and test a task to measure how different contexts affect our perception of objects, particularly those that were regarded as potentially dangerous. Two pre-registered experiments conducted in Italy and Germany showed how linguistically processed affordances (e.g., nouns) referring to objects perceived as dangerous (such as a door handle during the pandemic) were processed differently than those referring to neutral objects (like a toothbrush) in a sensibility judgement task. This effect was observed across different contexts and in both experiments. Further research is needed to determine if this effect is similar to the processing of negative words, and if it differs between objects with immanent negative properties (like a broken glass or a fire) versus those with potentially negative properties that only emerge in certain contexts. Replications of the study may clarify if the effect was temporary or produced more permanent changes in behaviour.
COVID-19的爆发和控制它的努力可能会影响我们的日常生活,包括我们与物体的互动方式。这项在线研究旨在开发和测试一项任务,以衡量不同的环境如何影响我们对物体的感知,特别是那些被认为具有潜在危险的物体。在意大利和德国进行的两项预先注册的实验表明,在感性判断任务中,对被认为是危险物体(如大流行期间的门把手)的可见性(如名词)的语言处理与指中性物体(如牙刷)的语言处理是如何不同的。在不同的环境和两个实验中都观察到了这种效应。需要进一步的研究来确定这种效应是否类似于消极词汇的处理,以及具有内在消极属性的物体(如碎玻璃或火)与仅在特定环境中出现的潜在消极属性的物体之间是否存在差异。这项研究的重复研究可能会澄清这种影响是暂时的,还是在行为上产生了更持久的变化。
{"title":"COVID-19 and the Perceived Dangerousness of Everyday Objects: A Behavioural Online Study in Italy and Germany","authors":"Claudia Gianelli, Katharina Kühne, Alex Miklashevsky, Melinda Jeglinski-Mende, Nicola Canessa, Anna M. Borghi","doi":"10.1525/collabra.89171","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.89171","url":null,"abstract":"The outbreak of COVID-19 and efforts to contain it have likely impacted our daily lives, including how we interact with objects. This online study aimed to develop and test a task to measure how different contexts affect our perception of objects, particularly those that were regarded as potentially dangerous. Two pre-registered experiments conducted in Italy and Germany showed how linguistically processed affordances (e.g., nouns) referring to objects perceived as dangerous (such as a door handle during the pandemic) were processed differently than those referring to neutral objects (like a toothbrush) in a sensibility judgement task. This effect was observed across different contexts and in both experiments. Further research is needed to determine if this effect is similar to the processing of negative words, and if it differs between objects with immanent negative properties (like a broken glass or a fire) versus those with potentially negative properties that only emerge in certain contexts. Replications of the study may clarify if the effect was temporary or produced more permanent changes in behaviour.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135561899","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Similar, but Different: Gender Differences in Working Time Arrangements and the Work–Life Interface 相似,但不同:工作时间安排和工作-生活界面的性别差异
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.87546
Jens Mazei, Nils Backhaus, Anne Marit Wöhrmann, Corinna Brauner-Sommer, Joachim Hüffmeier
Gender inequities can be partly traced back to gender differences in working time arrangements. In fact, it is established knowledge that women as compared to men are more (less) likely to work part-time (overtime). Based on social role theory, however, we also expect gender differences among part-time and overtime workers, such that women and men differ in why they work part-time or overtime. In a preregistered and highly powered study conducted in Germany (N = 3,844–17,361, depending on the analysis), we observed that, on average, women were more likely than men to work part-time (i.e., fewer than 35 hours per week) because of personal or family obligations. Moreover, in comparison to men, women were less likely to work overtime (i.e., at least two hours per week) to attain additional income, but more likely to work overtime to step in for colleagues. Altogether, people had “gendered” reasons to work certain hours. Furthermore, as people’s paid working time arrangements are intertwined with their lives outside of the workplace, we examined women’s and men’s work–life interface and observed that women (as compared to men) deemed it less acceptable to be available for work-related issues during leisure time. We discuss implications for future theorizing and for practitioners who aim to design work schedules that consider the different lived experiences of women and men.
性别不平等的部分原因可以追溯到工作时间安排上的性别差异。事实上,众所周知,与男性相比,女性更多(更少)从事兼职(加班)工作。然而,根据社会角色理论,我们也预计兼职和加班工人之间存在性别差异,例如女性和男性在兼职或加班的原因上存在差异。在德国进行的一项预先登记的强有力的研究(N = 3,844-17,361,取决于分析结果)中,我们观察到,平均而言,由于个人或家庭责任,女性比男性更有可能兼职(即每周少于35小时)。此外,与男性相比,女性不太可能加班(即每周至少两小时)来获得额外收入,但更有可能加班来代替同事。总的来说,人们有“性别”的理由来工作特定的时间。此外,由于人们的带薪工作时间安排与他们在工作场所之外的生活交织在一起,我们研究了女性和男性的工作生活界面,发现女性(与男性相比)认为在闲暇时间处理与工作有关的问题是不可接受的。我们讨论了对未来理论和从业者的影响,他们的目标是设计工作时间表,考虑到女性和男性的不同生活经历。
{"title":"Similar, but Different: Gender Differences in Working Time Arrangements and the Work–Life Interface","authors":"Jens Mazei, Nils Backhaus, Anne Marit Wöhrmann, Corinna Brauner-Sommer, Joachim Hüffmeier","doi":"10.1525/collabra.87546","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.87546","url":null,"abstract":"Gender inequities can be partly traced back to gender differences in working time arrangements. In fact, it is established knowledge that women as compared to men are more (less) likely to work part-time (overtime). Based on social role theory, however, we also expect gender differences among part-time and overtime workers, such that women and men differ in why they work part-time or overtime. In a preregistered and highly powered study conducted in Germany (N = 3,844–17,361, depending on the analysis), we observed that, on average, women were more likely than men to work part-time (i.e., fewer than 35 hours per week) because of personal or family obligations. Moreover, in comparison to men, women were less likely to work overtime (i.e., at least two hours per week) to attain additional income, but more likely to work overtime to step in for colleagues. Altogether, people had “gendered” reasons to work certain hours. Furthermore, as people’s paid working time arrangements are intertwined with their lives outside of the workplace, we examined women’s and men’s work–life interface and observed that women (as compared to men) deemed it less acceptable to be available for work-related issues during leisure time. We discuss implications for future theorizing and for practitioners who aim to design work schedules that consider the different lived experiences of women and men.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135700978","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Dynamics of Self-Control Conflicts in Daily Life in Predicting Self-Control Success and Perceived Self-Regulatory Effectiveness 日常生活中自我控制冲突的动态预测自我控制成功和感知自我调节效能
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.88158
Emily M. Britton, Kristin Laurin, Igor Grossmann, Anna Dorfman, Harrison Oakes, Abigail A. Scholer
People often face conflicts where they must choose between their long-term goals and tempting alternatives. Using an open-ended daily diary design, we investigated the characteristics of self-control conflicts in daily life, both replicating and extending past work. Specifically, we examined the factors that affected self-control conflict success, as well as how the nature and resolution of the conflict affected general perceptions of self-regulatory effectiveness. Self-control conflicts varied considerably within-persons including the domain of the conflict, the use of strategies, and whether they were successfully resolved. There was also variability in people’s subjective perceptions of how pulled they felt towards the temptation and the opposing goal, as well as how difficult and important the overall decision was. Furthermore, these factors predicted whether a conflict was resolved successfully (i.e., in favor of the goal), with pull towards the temptation emerging as the strongest predictor. People were also more successful in resolving self-control conflicts when they reported using any type of self-regulatory strategy; no specific strategy emerged as most effective. On days when participants successfully resolved conflicts, they also felt more confident in their general ability to self-regulate. Overall, our findings largely conceptually replicate past work using an open-ended diary format, and suggest that factors influencing self-control conflict resolution are also linked to general feelings of self-regulatory effectiveness.
人们经常面临冲突,必须在长期目标和诱人的选择之间做出选择。采用开放式的日常日记设计,我们研究了日常生活中自我控制冲突的特征,包括复制和扩展过去的工作。具体来说,我们研究了影响自我控制冲突成功的因素,以及冲突的性质和解决如何影响自我调节有效性的一般看法。自我控制冲突在个人内部有很大的差异,包括冲突的领域、策略的使用以及是否成功解决。人们对诱惑和对方目标的主观感受,以及整个决定的难度和重要性,也存在差异。此外,这些因素预测了冲突是否被成功解决(即,支持目标),对诱惑的吸引力成为最强的预测因素。当人们报告使用任何类型的自我调节策略时,他们在解决自我控制冲突方面也更成功;没有出现最有效的具体策略。在参与者成功解决冲突的日子里,他们也对自己的一般自我调节能力更有信心。总的来说,我们的发现在概念上很大程度上复制了过去使用开放式日记格式的工作,并表明影响自我控制冲突解决的因素也与自我调节有效性的一般感觉有关。
{"title":"The Dynamics of Self-Control Conflicts in Daily Life in Predicting Self-Control Success and Perceived Self-Regulatory Effectiveness","authors":"Emily M. Britton, Kristin Laurin, Igor Grossmann, Anna Dorfman, Harrison Oakes, Abigail A. Scholer","doi":"10.1525/collabra.88158","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.88158","url":null,"abstract":"People often face conflicts where they must choose between their long-term goals and tempting alternatives. Using an open-ended daily diary design, we investigated the characteristics of self-control conflicts in daily life, both replicating and extending past work. Specifically, we examined the factors that affected self-control conflict success, as well as how the nature and resolution of the conflict affected general perceptions of self-regulatory effectiveness. Self-control conflicts varied considerably within-persons including the domain of the conflict, the use of strategies, and whether they were successfully resolved. There was also variability in people’s subjective perceptions of how pulled they felt towards the temptation and the opposing goal, as well as how difficult and important the overall decision was. Furthermore, these factors predicted whether a conflict was resolved successfully (i.e., in favor of the goal), with pull towards the temptation emerging as the strongest predictor. People were also more successful in resolving self-control conflicts when they reported using any type of self-regulatory strategy; no specific strategy emerged as most effective. On days when participants successfully resolved conflicts, they also felt more confident in their general ability to self-regulate. Overall, our findings largely conceptually replicate past work using an open-ended diary format, and suggest that factors influencing self-control conflict resolution are also linked to general feelings of self-regulatory effectiveness.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136373450","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Association Between Smokers’ Approach Bias and Heaviness of Use: A Focus on Light Smokers 吸烟者方法偏差与烟瘾的关系:以轻度吸烟者为研究对象
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.88926
Marine Rougier, Dominique Muller, Annique Smeding, Reinout W. Wiers, Lison Neyroud
The relationship between heaviness of use and the approach bias (i.e., stronger approach than avoidance tendencies) toward tobacco remains ambiguous at both theoretical and empirical levels. Indeed, some models of addition would formulate opposite predictions (i.e., positive vs. negative relationship) and, as it turns out, current evidence is mixed. In three studies, we investigated this relationship among smokers (relying on a continuous measure of heaviness) and compared approach/avoidance tendencies of light smokers and non-smokers (relying on group comparison). To measure approach/avoidance tendencies, we used the Visual Approach/Avoidance by the Self Task (VAAST) that visually simulates whole body movements. This task was used as irrelevant-feature version (i.e., instructions about another dimension). Heaviness of use was assessed continuously with daily cigarette use. Data were analyzed in two Integrative Data Analyses (IDAs; a kind of meta-analysis considering jointly the raw data of the three studies), thus taking into account both significant and non-significant effects (total N = 173). In our first integrative analysis (Studies 1-3), we observed an increase in the approach bias toward tobacco as a function of heaviness of use, as well as an avoidance bias among light smokers. In our second integrative analysis (Studies 2 and 3), we found that light smokers have a stronger avoidance bias than non-smokers. While the positive relationship between heaviness of use and approach tendencies toward tobacco is consistent with most addiction models, our finding on light smokers’ avoidance bias stands in sharp contrast. These findings, however, can be incorporated into general motivational models or single-process propositional models that consider the role of goal-oriented or propositional processes, respectively.
在理论和经验层面上,重度使用与烟草倾向偏差(即倾向倾向大于回避倾向)之间的关系仍然不明确。事实上,一些加法模型会形成相反的预测(即,积极与消极的关系),而事实证明,目前的证据是混合的。在三项研究中,我们调查了吸烟者之间的这种关系(依赖于连续的体重测量),并比较了轻度吸烟者和非吸烟者的接近/避免倾向(依赖于群体比较)。为了测量接近/回避倾向,我们使用了视觉上模拟全身运动的自我任务接近/回避(VAAST)。这个任务被用作不相关的特征版本(即,关于另一个维度的指令)。吸烟的严重程度通过每日吸烟情况进行持续评估。数据采用两种综合数据分析(IDAs;(一种综合考虑三项研究原始数据的荟萃分析),从而兼顾显著效应和非显著效应(总N = 173)。在我们的第一个综合分析(研究1-3)中,我们观察到,在轻度吸烟者中,对烟草的方法偏差随着使用程度的增加而增加,同时也出现了避免偏差。在我们的第二个综合分析(研究2和3)中,我们发现轻度吸烟者比不吸烟者有更强的回避偏见。虽然重度使用和接近烟草倾向之间的正相关关系与大多数成瘾模型一致,但我们对轻度吸烟者的回避偏见的研究结果与之形成鲜明对比。然而,这些发现可以分别纳入考虑目标导向或命题过程作用的一般动机模型或单过程命题模型。
{"title":"The Association Between Smokers’ Approach Bias and Heaviness of Use: A Focus on Light Smokers","authors":"Marine Rougier, Dominique Muller, Annique Smeding, Reinout W. Wiers, Lison Neyroud","doi":"10.1525/collabra.88926","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.88926","url":null,"abstract":"The relationship between heaviness of use and the approach bias (i.e., stronger approach than avoidance tendencies) toward tobacco remains ambiguous at both theoretical and empirical levels. Indeed, some models of addition would formulate opposite predictions (i.e., positive vs. negative relationship) and, as it turns out, current evidence is mixed. In three studies, we investigated this relationship among smokers (relying on a continuous measure of heaviness) and compared approach/avoidance tendencies of light smokers and non-smokers (relying on group comparison). To measure approach/avoidance tendencies, we used the Visual Approach/Avoidance by the Self Task (VAAST) that visually simulates whole body movements. This task was used as irrelevant-feature version (i.e., instructions about another dimension). Heaviness of use was assessed continuously with daily cigarette use. Data were analyzed in two Integrative Data Analyses (IDAs; a kind of meta-analysis considering jointly the raw data of the three studies), thus taking into account both significant and non-significant effects (total N = 173). In our first integrative analysis (Studies 1-3), we observed an increase in the approach bias toward tobacco as a function of heaviness of use, as well as an avoidance bias among light smokers. In our second integrative analysis (Studies 2 and 3), we found that light smokers have a stronger avoidance bias than non-smokers. While the positive relationship between heaviness of use and approach tendencies toward tobacco is consistent with most addiction models, our finding on light smokers’ avoidance bias stands in sharp contrast. These findings, however, can be incorporated into general motivational models or single-process propositional models that consider the role of goal-oriented or propositional processes, respectively.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135102466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Roadmap for Future Interactions Between Research on Personality and Learning 未来人格研究与学习互动的路线图
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.88334
Jan De Houwer, Marco Perugini, Yannick Boddez, Florin Sava
In this introduction to the special collection of papers on the relation between learning and personality research, we provide a functional-cognitive framework that can guide interactions between learning and personality researchers. It highlights that learning researchers can treat personality variables as potential (first or second order) moderators of learning effects. They can also examine whether the effects of personality on learning itself depend on other moderators. Personality researchers can benefit from looking for stable individual differences in known learning effects. Together with learning researchers, they can analyze personality as learned and thus malleable behavior. We end by summarizing the papers of the special collection and by situating them within our framework.
在这篇关于学习与人格研究之间关系的特别文集的介绍中,我们提供了一个功能-认知框架,可以指导学习与人格研究人员之间的互动。它强调学习研究者可以将人格变量视为学习效果的潜在(一阶或二阶)调节因子。他们还可以研究性格对学习本身的影响是否取决于其他调节者。人格研究者可以从寻找已知学习效果中稳定的个体差异中获益。与学习研究人员一起,他们可以将人格分析为习得的,因此是可塑的行为。最后,我们总结了特别收藏的论文,并将它们置于我们的框架内。
{"title":"A Roadmap for Future Interactions Between Research on Personality and Learning","authors":"Jan De Houwer, Marco Perugini, Yannick Boddez, Florin Sava","doi":"10.1525/collabra.88334","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.88334","url":null,"abstract":"In this introduction to the special collection of papers on the relation between learning and personality research, we provide a functional-cognitive framework that can guide interactions between learning and personality researchers. It highlights that learning researchers can treat personality variables as potential (first or second order) moderators of learning effects. They can also examine whether the effects of personality on learning itself depend on other moderators. Personality researchers can benefit from looking for stable individual differences in known learning effects. Together with learning researchers, they can analyze personality as learned and thus malleable behavior. We end by summarizing the papers of the special collection and by situating them within our framework.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136053446","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Deep Dive Into Distributive Concession Making and the Likelihood of Impasses in Negotiations 深入探讨分配让步和谈判僵局的可能性
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.88929
Marc Mertes, Dana Kunz, Joachim Hüffmeier
Negotiation impasses can have severe negative consequences, but only little research attention has been devoted to investigating their causes. Studies on distributive concession making (i.e., high demands and low concessions) as a cause of impasses were inconclusive due to low sample sizes and methodological choices. Moreover, distributive concession making entails two hitherto fully entangled properties: reduction of conceded value and violation of the reciprocity norm. In our experiment, participants negotiated with a confederate who administered different concession patterns that allowed us to disentangle these properties. We found unambiguous evidence that distributive concession making increases the likelihood of impasses. This effect was driven by the reduction of conceded value rather than the violation of the reciprocity norm. Confrontation with distributive concession making led participants to develop negative internal attributions and anger, which mediated the effect of distributive concession making on the impasse rate. Our study contributes to a better understanding of the causes and underlying mechanisms of negotiation impasses.
谈判僵局可能会产生严重的负面后果,但对其原因的调查研究却很少。关于分配让步(即高要求和低让步)作为僵局原因的研究由于样本量小和方法选择不确定。此外,分配让步涉及两个迄今为止完全纠缠在一起的属性:减少让步价值和违反互惠准则。在我们的实验中,参与者与一个管理不同让步模式的同盟者进行谈判,使我们能够解开这些属性。我们发现明确的证据表明,分配让步增加了僵局的可能性。这种效应是由让步价值的减少所驱动的,而不是违反互惠准则。面对分配让步导致被试产生消极的内部归因和愤怒情绪,从而中介了分配让步对僵局率的影响。我们的研究有助于更好地理解谈判僵局的原因和潜在机制。
{"title":"A Deep Dive Into Distributive Concession Making and the Likelihood of Impasses in Negotiations","authors":"Marc Mertes, Dana Kunz, Joachim Hüffmeier","doi":"10.1525/collabra.88929","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.88929","url":null,"abstract":"Negotiation impasses can have severe negative consequences, but only little research attention has been devoted to investigating their causes. Studies on distributive concession making (i.e., high demands and low concessions) as a cause of impasses were inconclusive due to low sample sizes and methodological choices. Moreover, distributive concession making entails two hitherto fully entangled properties: reduction of conceded value and violation of the reciprocity norm. In our experiment, participants negotiated with a confederate who administered different concession patterns that allowed us to disentangle these properties. We found unambiguous evidence that distributive concession making increases the likelihood of impasses. This effect was driven by the reduction of conceded value rather than the violation of the reciprocity norm. Confrontation with distributive concession making led participants to develop negative internal attributions and anger, which mediated the effect of distributive concession making on the impasse rate. Our study contributes to a better understanding of the causes and underlying mechanisms of negotiation impasses.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135503150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Assessing the Replicability of Cognitive Psychology During Remote Experiential Learning via Mobile Phone Technology 基于移动电话技术的远程体验式学习认知心理可复制性评估
Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1525/collabra.88320
Benjamin Dyson
A recent global health crisis demanded the wholesale configuration of both teaching and research from in-person to on-line formats. This allowed for an environmental sweep regarding the replicability of some classic and contemporary findings in Cognitive Psychology in the context of an undergraduate course, in which eight portable experimental packages were written for mobile phone. Running across three semesters (average n per study = 585), data consistently produced evidence either for (Faces, Search, Object, RPS, Rotate) or against (Doodle, Trivia) the original findings, with the exception of one study (House) that produced ambiguous findings. The scheme not only allows students exposure to and discussion of the replication crisis within empirical science, but also provides a framework for the future implementation of experiential learning during remote and asynchronous teaching. With continued evaluation made possible via Open Science Framework, a central question is whether on-line data collection violates an essential auxiliary assumption for the replication of in-person data.
最近的全球卫生危机要求教学和研究从面对面到在线形式的大规模配置。在本科课程的背景下,这允许对认知心理学中一些经典和当代发现的可复制性进行环境扫描,其中为手机编写了八个便携式实验包。在三个学期中(平均每个研究n = 585),数据一致地提供了支持(Faces, Search, Object, RPS, Rotate)或反对(Doodle, Trivia)原始发现的证据,除了一项研究(House)产生了模棱两可的发现。该方案不仅允许学生接触和讨论经验科学中的复制危机,而且还为远程和异步教学中体验式学习的未来实施提供了框架。随着开放科学框架的持续评估成为可能,一个核心问题是在线数据收集是否违反了个人数据复制的基本辅助假设。
{"title":"Assessing the Replicability of Cognitive Psychology During Remote Experiential Learning via Mobile Phone Technology","authors":"Benjamin Dyson","doi":"10.1525/collabra.88320","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.88320","url":null,"abstract":"A recent global health crisis demanded the wholesale configuration of both teaching and research from in-person to on-line formats. This allowed for an environmental sweep regarding the replicability of some classic and contemporary findings in Cognitive Psychology in the context of an undergraduate course, in which eight portable experimental packages were written for mobile phone. Running across three semesters (average n per study = 585), data consistently produced evidence either for (Faces, Search, Object, RPS, Rotate) or against (Doodle, Trivia) the original findings, with the exception of one study (House) that produced ambiguous findings. The scheme not only allows students exposure to and discussion of the replication crisis within empirical science, but also provides a framework for the future implementation of experiential learning during remote and asynchronous teaching. With continued evaluation made possible via Open Science Framework, a central question is whether on-line data collection violates an essential auxiliary assumption for the replication of in-person data.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136207222","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Collabra
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1