首页 > 最新文献

Collabra最新文献

英文 中文
Individual Differences in Statistical Learning: Conceptual and Measurement Issues 统计学习中的个体差异:概念和测量问题
Pub Date : 2016-10-27 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.41
L. Erickson, Michael P. Kaschak, Erik D. Thiessen, C. Berry
The ability to adapt to statistical structure (often referred to as “statistical learning”) has been proposed to play a major role in the acquisition and use of natural languages. Several recent studies have explored the relationship between individual differences in statistical learning and language outcomes. These studies have produced mixed results, with some studies finding a significant relationship between statistical learning and language outcomes, and others finding weak or null results. Furthermore, the few studies that have used multiple measures of statistical learning have reported that they are not correlated (e.g., [1]). The current study assesses the reliability of various measures of auditory statistical segmentation, and their consistency over time. That is, do the generally low correlations observed between measures of statistical learning stem from task demands, the psychometric properties of the measures, or the fact that statistical learning may be a highly fragmented construct? Our results confirm previous reports that individual measures of statistical learning tend not to correlate with each other, and suggest that the somewhat weak reliability of the measures may be an important factor in the low correlations. Our data also suggest that aggregating performance across tasks may be an avenue for improving the reliability of the measures.
适应统计结构的能力(通常被称为“统计学习”)被认为在自然语言的习得和使用中起着重要作用。最近的几项研究探讨了统计学习的个体差异与语言成绩之间的关系。这些研究产生了不同的结果,一些研究发现统计学习和语言成绩之间存在显著的关系,而另一些研究发现微弱或无效的结果。此外,少数使用多种统计学习方法的研究报告称,它们并不相关(例如,[1])。目前的研究评估了听觉统计分割的各种措施的可靠性,以及它们随时间的一致性。也就是说,统计学习测量之间普遍存在的低相关性是源于任务要求、测量的心理测量特性,还是统计学习可能是一个高度碎片化的结构?我们的研究结果证实了先前的报告,即统计学习的个体测量往往彼此不相关,并表明测量的可靠性有些弱可能是低相关性的重要因素。我们的数据还表明,跨任务的聚合性能可能是提高测量可靠性的途径。
{"title":"Individual Differences in Statistical Learning: Conceptual and Measurement Issues","authors":"L. Erickson, Michael P. Kaschak, Erik D. Thiessen, C. Berry","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.41","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.41","url":null,"abstract":"The ability to adapt to statistical structure (often referred to as “statistical learning”) has been proposed to play a major role in the acquisition and use of natural languages. Several recent studies have explored the relationship between individual differences in statistical learning and language outcomes. These studies have produced mixed results, with some studies finding a significant relationship between statistical learning and language outcomes, and others finding weak or null results. Furthermore, the few studies that have used multiple measures of statistical learning have reported that they are not correlated (e.g., [1]). The current study assesses the reliability of various measures of auditory statistical segmentation, and their consistency over time. That is, do the generally low correlations observed between measures of statistical learning stem from task demands, the psychometric properties of the measures, or the fact that statistical learning may be a highly fragmented construct? Our results confirm previous reports that individual measures of statistical learning tend not to correlate with each other, and suggest that the somewhat weak reliability of the measures may be an important factor in the low correlations. Our data also suggest that aggregating performance across tasks may be an avenue for improving the reliability of the measures.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-10-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66879223","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 29
A Direct Comparison of N400 Effects of Predictability and Incongruity in Adjective-Noun Combination 形容词-名词组合中可预测性和不一致性的N400效应的直接比较
Pub Date : 2016-10-18 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.40
Ellen F. Lau, Anna M. Namyst, A. Fogel, T. Delgado
Previous work has shown that the N400 ERP component is elicited by all words, whether presented in isolation or in structured contexts, and that its amplitude is modulated by semantic association and contextual predictability. What is less clear is the extent to which the N400 response is modulated by semantic incongruity when predictability is held constant. In the current study we examine N400 modulation associated with independent manipulations of predictability and congruity in an adjective-noun paradigm that allows us to precisely control predictability through corpus counts. Our results demonstrate small N400 effects of semantic congruity (yellow bag vs. innocent bag), and much more robust N400 effects of predictability (runny nose vs. dainty nose) under the same conditions. These data argue against unitary N400 theories according to which N400 effects of both predictability and incongruity reflect a common process such as degree of integration difficulty, as large N400 effects of predictability were observed in the absence of large N400 effects of incongruity. However, the data are consistent with some versions of unitary ‘facilitated access’ N400 theories, as well as multiple-generator accounts according to which the N400 can be independently modulated by facilitated conceptual/lexical access (as with predictability) and integration difficulty (as with incongruity, perhaps to a greater extent in full sentential contexts).
以前的研究表明,N400 ERP成分是由所有的单词引起的,无论是在孤立的还是在结构化的语境中,它的振幅是由语义关联和语境可预测性调节的。不太清楚的是,当可预测性保持不变时,N400反应在多大程度上受到语义不一致性的调节。在当前的研究中,我们研究了在一个形容词-名词范式中与可预测性和一致性的独立操纵相关的N400调制,该范式允许我们通过语料库计数精确控制可预测性。我们的研究结果表明,在相同的条件下,语义一致性的N400效应很小(黄色袋子vs.无辜的袋子),而可预测性的N400效应更强(流鼻涕vs.精致的鼻子)。这些数据反对统一的N400理论,根据该理论,可预测性和不一致性的N400效应反映了一个共同的过程,如整合难度程度,因为在不存在不一致性的大N400效应的情况下,可预测性的大N400效应被观察到。然而,这些数据与统一的“便利获取”N400理论的某些版本是一致的,以及多重生成器的说法,根据这些说法,N400可以通过便利的概念/词汇获取(如可预测性)和整合难度(如不一致性,可能在更大程度上在完整的句子上下文中)独立调节。
{"title":"A Direct Comparison of N400 Effects of Predictability and Incongruity in Adjective-Noun Combination","authors":"Ellen F. Lau, Anna M. Namyst, A. Fogel, T. Delgado","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.40","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.40","url":null,"abstract":"Previous work has shown that the N400 ERP component is elicited by all words, whether presented in isolation or in structured contexts, and that its amplitude is modulated by semantic association and contextual predictability. What is less clear is the extent to which the N400 response is modulated by semantic incongruity when predictability is held constant. In the current study we examine N400 modulation associated with independent manipulations of predictability and congruity in an adjective-noun paradigm that allows us to precisely control predictability through corpus counts. Our results demonstrate small N400 effects of semantic congruity (yellow bag vs. innocent bag), and much more robust N400 effects of predictability (runny nose vs. dainty nose) under the same conditions. These data argue against unitary N400 theories according to which N400 effects of both predictability and incongruity reflect a common process such as degree of integration difficulty, as large N400 effects of predictability were observed in the absence of large N400 effects of incongruity. However, the data are consistent with some versions of unitary ‘facilitated access’ N400 theories, as well as multiple-generator accounts according to which the N400 can be independently modulated by facilitated conceptual/lexical access (as with predictability) and integration difficulty (as with incongruity, perhaps to a greater extent in full sentential contexts).","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66879176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 51
Correction: No Child Left Alone: Moral Judgments about Parents Affect Estimates of Risk to Children 更正:不让孩子独处:关于父母的道德判断影响对孩子的风险估计
Pub Date : 2016-10-14 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.58
Ashley J. Thomas, P. Stanford, B. Sarnecka
This article details a correction to article Thomas, A. J., Stanford, P. K., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2016). No Child Left Alone: Moral Judgments about Parents Affect Estimates of Risk to Children.  Collabra , 2(1), 10. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.33
本文详细介绍了对Thomas, a.j., Stanford, p.k., & Sarnecka, b.w.(2016)文章的更正。不让一个孩子孤独:关于父母的道德判断影响对孩子的风险估计。昆虫学报,2(1),10。DOI: http://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.33
{"title":"Correction: No Child Left Alone: Moral Judgments about Parents Affect Estimates of Risk to Children","authors":"Ashley J. Thomas, P. Stanford, B. Sarnecka","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.58","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.58","url":null,"abstract":"This article details a correction to article Thomas, A. J., Stanford, P. K., & Sarnecka, B. W. (2016). No Child Left Alone: Moral Judgments about Parents Affect Estimates of Risk to Children.  Collabra , 2(1), 10. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.33","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66879130","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
No Child Left Alone: Moral Judgments about Parents Affect Estimates of Risk to Children 不让一个孩子孤独:关于父母的道德判断影响对孩子的风险估计
Pub Date : 2016-08-23 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.33
Ashley J. Thomas, B. Sarnecka
In recent decades, Americans have adopted a parenting norm in which every child is expected to be under constant direct adult supervision. Parents who violate this norm by allowing their children to be alone, even for short periods of time, often face harsh criticism and even legal action. This is true despite the fact that children are much more likely to be hurt, for example, in car accidents. Why then do bystanders call 911 when they see children playing in parks, but not when they see children riding in cars? Here, we present results from six studies indicating that moral judgments play a role: The less morally acceptable a parent’s reason for leaving a child alone, the more danger people think the child is in. This suggests that people’s estimates of danger to unsupervised children are affected by an intuition that parents who leave their children alone have done something morally wrong.
近几十年来,美国人已经采用了一种育儿规范,即每个孩子都应该处于成年人的直接监督之下。违反这一规范,允许孩子独处的父母,即使是很短的一段时间,往往会面临严厉的批评,甚至法律诉讼。这是正确的,尽管事实上儿童更容易受伤,例如在车祸中。那么,为什么旁观者看到孩子在公园里玩耍时会拨打911,而看到孩子开车时却不会呢?在这里,我们展示了六项研究的结果,表明道德判断起着作用:父母留下孩子独自一人的理由在道德上越不可接受,人们就越认为孩子处于危险之中。这表明,人们对无人看管的孩子所面临的危险的估计受到了一种直觉的影响,即让孩子独处的父母在道德上是错误的。
{"title":"No Child Left Alone: Moral Judgments about Parents Affect Estimates of Risk to Children","authors":"Ashley J. Thomas, B. Sarnecka","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.33","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.33","url":null,"abstract":"In recent decades, Americans have adopted a parenting norm in which every child is expected to be under constant direct adult supervision. Parents who violate this norm by allowing their children to be alone, even for short periods of time, often face harsh criticism and even legal action. This is true despite the fact that children are much more likely to be hurt, for example, in car accidents. Why then do bystanders call 911 when they see children playing in parks, but not when they see children riding in cars? Here, we present results from six studies indicating that moral judgments play a role: The less morally acceptable a parent’s reason for leaving a child alone, the more danger people think the child is in. This suggests that people’s estimates of danger to unsupervised children are affected by an intuition that parents who leave their children alone have done something morally wrong.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66876758","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
The Effects of Categorization on Perceptual Judgment are Robust across Different Assessment Tasks 分类对知觉判断的影响在不同的评估任务中都是显著的
Pub Date : 2016-07-28 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.32
J. D. Leeuw, Janet K. Andrews, Kenneth R. Livingston, Benjamin M. Chin
Learned visual categorical perception (CP) effects were assessed using three different measures (similarity rating, same-different judgment, and an XAB task) and two sets of stimuli differing in discriminability and varying on one category-relevant and one category-irrelevant dimension. Participant scores were converted to a common scale to allow assessment method to serve as an independent variable. Two different analyses using the Bayes Factor approach produced patterns of results consistent with learned CP effects: compared to a control group, participants trained on the category distinction could better discriminate between-category pairs of stimuli and were more sensitive to the category-relevant dimension. In addition, performance was better in general for the more highly discriminable stimuli, but stimulus discriminability did not influence the pattern of observed CP effects. Furthermore, these results were consistent regardless of how performance was assessed. This suggests that, for these methods at least, learned CP effects are robust across substantially different performance measures. Four different kinds of learned CP effects are reported in the literature singly or in combination: greater sensitivity between categories, reduced sensitivity within categories, increased sensitivity to category-relevant dimensions, and decreased sensitivity to category-irrelevant dimensions. The results of the current study suggest that the cause of these different patterns of CP effects is not due to either stimulus discriminability or assessment task. Other possible causes of the differences in reported CP findings are discussed.
学习视觉分类知觉(CP)效应采用三种不同的测量方法(相似性评定、相同-不同判断和XAB任务)和两组不同的可辨别性刺激,并在一个类别相关和一个类别无关的维度上变化。参与者的分数被转换成一个共同的尺度,以允许评估方法作为一个独立的变量。使用贝叶斯因子方法的两种不同分析产生了与习得CP效应一致的结果模式:与对照组相比,接受过类别区分训练的参与者可以更好地区分类别对刺激,并且对类别相关维度更敏感。此外,一般来说,高分辨刺激的表现更好,但刺激可分辨性不影响观察到的CP效应模式。此外,无论如何评估绩效,这些结果都是一致的。这表明,至少对于这些方法,习得的CP效应在本质上不同的性能测量中是稳健的。文献中单独或联合报道了四种不同的习得CP效应:类别之间的敏感性增加,类别内的敏感性降低,对类别相关维度的敏感性增加,以及对类别无关维度的敏感性降低。目前的研究结果表明,这些不同模式的CP效应的原因既不是由于刺激可辨别性,也不是由于评估任务。其他可能的原因在报告的CP发现的差异进行了讨论。
{"title":"The Effects of Categorization on Perceptual Judgment are Robust across Different Assessment Tasks","authors":"J. D. Leeuw, Janet K. Andrews, Kenneth R. Livingston, Benjamin M. Chin","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.32","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.32","url":null,"abstract":"Learned visual categorical perception (CP) effects were assessed using three different measures (similarity rating, same-different judgment, and an XAB task) and two sets of stimuli differing in discriminability and varying on one category-relevant and one category-irrelevant dimension. Participant scores were converted to a common scale to allow assessment method to serve as an independent variable. Two different analyses using the Bayes Factor approach produced patterns of results consistent with learned CP effects: compared to a control group, participants trained on the category distinction could better discriminate between-category pairs of stimuli and were more sensitive to the category-relevant dimension. In addition, performance was better in general for the more highly discriminable stimuli, but stimulus discriminability did not influence the pattern of observed CP effects. Furthermore, these results were consistent regardless of how performance was assessed. This suggests that, for these methods at least, learned CP effects are robust across substantially different performance measures. Four different kinds of learned CP effects are reported in the literature singly or in combination: greater sensitivity between categories, reduced sensitivity within categories, increased sensitivity to category-relevant dimensions, and decreased sensitivity to category-irrelevant dimensions. The results of the current study suggest that the cause of these different patterns of CP effects is not due to either stimulus discriminability or assessment task. Other possible causes of the differences in reported CP findings are discussed.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-07-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66876120","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
The Effects of Minimal Group Membership on Young Preschoolers’ Social Preferences, Estimates of Similarity, and Behavioral Attribution 最小群体成员对幼儿社会偏好、相似性估计和行为归因的影响
Pub Date : 2016-07-15 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.44
N. Richter, Harriet Over, Yarrow Dunham
We investigate young children’s sensitivity to minimal group membership. Previous research has suggested that children do not show sensitivity to minimal cues to group membership until the age of five to six, contributing to claims that this is an important transition in the development of intergroup cognition and behavior. In this study, we investigated whether even younger children are sensitive to minimal cues to group membership. Random assignment to one of either of two color groups created a temporary, visually salient minimal group membership in 3 and 4-year-old study participants. Using explicit measures, we tested whether children preferred minimal group members when making social judgments. We find that, in the absence of any knowledge regarding the two groups, children expressed greater liking for ingroup than outgroup targets. Moreover, children estimated that ingroup members would share their preferences. Our findings demonstrate that from early in development, humans assess unknown others on the basis of minimal cues to social similarity and that the perception of group boundaries potentially underlies social assortment in strangers.
我们调查幼儿对最小群体成员的敏感性。先前的研究表明,儿童在5到6岁之前不会对群体成员的最小提示表现出敏感性,这有助于声称这是群体间认知和行为发展的重要转变。在这项研究中,我们调查了甚至更小的孩子是否对群体成员的最小线索敏感。随机分配到两个颜色组中的任意一个,在3岁和4岁的研究参与者中创造了一个临时的,视觉上显著的最小群体成员。使用明确的测量方法,我们测试了儿童在做出社会判断时是否更喜欢最小的群体成员。我们发现,在不了解这两组的情况下,儿童对内组目标的喜爱程度高于外组目标。此外,孩子们估计群体内的成员会分享他们的偏好。我们的研究结果表明,从发育早期开始,人类就会根据最小的社会相似性线索来评估未知的他人,而对群体边界的感知可能是陌生人社会分类的基础。
{"title":"The Effects of Minimal Group Membership on Young Preschoolers’ Social Preferences, Estimates of Similarity, and Behavioral Attribution","authors":"N. Richter, Harriet Over, Yarrow Dunham","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.44","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.44","url":null,"abstract":"We investigate young children’s sensitivity to minimal group membership. Previous research has suggested that children do not show sensitivity to minimal cues to group membership until the age of five to six, contributing to claims that this is an important transition in the development of intergroup cognition and behavior. In this study, we investigated whether even younger children are sensitive to minimal cues to group membership. Random assignment to one of either of two color groups created a temporary, visually salient minimal group membership in 3 and 4-year-old study participants. Using explicit measures, we tested whether children preferred minimal group members when making social judgments. We find that, in the absence of any knowledge regarding the two groups, children expressed greater liking for ingroup than outgroup targets. Moreover, children estimated that ingroup members would share their preferences. Our findings demonstrate that from early in development, humans assess unknown others on the basis of minimal cues to social similarity and that the perception of group boundaries potentially underlies social assortment in strangers.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-07-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66879241","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 40
How iconicity helps people learn new words: neural correlates and individual differences in sound-symbolic bootstrapping. 象似性如何帮助人们学习新单词:声音符号引导的神经关联和个体差异。
Pub Date : 2016-07-06 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.42
Gwilym Lockwood, P. Hagoort, Mark Dingemanse
Sound symbolism is increasingly understood as involving iconicity, or perceptual analogies and cross-modal correspondences between form and meaning, but the search for its functional and neural correlates is ongoing. Here we study how people learn sound-symbolic words, using behavioural, electrophysiological and individual difference measures. Dutch participants learned Japanese ideophones —lexical sound- symbolic words— with a translation of either the real meaning (in which form and meaning show cross-modal correspondences) or the opposite meaning (in which form and meaning show cross-modal clashes). Participants were significantly better at identifying the words they learned in the real condition, correctly remembering the real word pairing 86.7% of the time, but the opposite word pairing only 71.3% of the time. Analysing event-related potentials (ERPs) during the test round showed that ideophones in the real condition elicited a greater P3 component and late positive complex than ideophones in the opposite condition. In a subsequent forced choice task, participants were asked to guess the real translation from two alternatives. They did this with 73.0% accuracy, well above chance level even for words they had encountered in the opposite condition, showing that people are generally sensitive to the sound-symbolic cues in ideophones. Individual difference measures showed that the ERP effect in the test round of the learning task was greater for participants who were more sensitive to sound symbolism in the forced choice task. The main driver of the difference was a lower amplitude of the P3 component in response to ideophones in the opposite condition, suggesting that people who are more sensitive to sound symbolism may have more difficulty to suppress conflicting cross-modal information. The findings provide new evidence that cross-modal correspondences between sound and meaning facilitate word learning, while cross-modal clashes make word learning harder, especially for people who are more sensitive to sound symbolism.
声音象征主义越来越多地被理解为涉及象似性,或感性类比和形式与意义之间的跨模态对应,但对其功能和神经相关性的研究仍在进行中。在这里,我们研究人们如何学习声音符号词,使用行为,电生理和个体差异测量。荷兰的参与者学习日语意指音——词汇的声音——象征性的词——通过翻译真实的意思(形式和意义表现出跨模态对应)或相反的意思(形式和意义表现出跨模态冲突)。参与者在识别他们在真实条件下学习的单词方面明显更好,正确记住真实单词配对的准确率为86.7%,而相反单词配对的准确率仅为71.3%。事件相关电位(ERPs)分析表明,真实条件下的听音者比相反条件下的听音者诱发了更大的P3分量和晚期正复合体。在随后的强制选择任务中,参与者被要求从两个选项中猜测真实的翻译。他们的准确率为73.0%,甚至远高于他们在相反条件下遇到的单词的概率水平,这表明人们通常对表意语音中的声音符号线索很敏感。个体差异测量显示,在学习任务的测试回合中,对强迫选择任务中声音象征主义更敏感的参与者的ERP效应更大。造成这种差异的主要原因是,在相反的情况下,听音者的P3分量的振幅较低,这表明对声音象征主义更敏感的人可能更难以抑制相互冲突的跨模态信息。这些发现提供了新的证据,证明声音和意义之间的跨模态对应有助于单词学习,而跨模态冲突会使单词学习变得更加困难,尤其是对声音象征主义更敏感的人。
{"title":"How iconicity helps people learn new words: neural correlates and individual differences in sound-symbolic bootstrapping.","authors":"Gwilym Lockwood, P. Hagoort, Mark Dingemanse","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.42","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.42","url":null,"abstract":"Sound symbolism is increasingly understood as involving iconicity, or perceptual analogies and cross-modal correspondences between form and meaning, but the search for its functional and neural correlates is ongoing. Here we study how people learn sound-symbolic words, using behavioural, electrophysiological and individual difference measures. Dutch participants learned Japanese ideophones —lexical sound- symbolic words— with a translation of either the real meaning (in which form and meaning show cross-modal correspondences) or the opposite meaning (in which form and meaning show cross-modal clashes). Participants were significantly better at identifying the words they learned in the real condition, correctly remembering the real word pairing 86.7% of the time, but the opposite word pairing only 71.3% of the time. Analysing event-related potentials (ERPs) during the test round showed that ideophones in the real condition elicited a greater P3 component and late positive complex than ideophones in the opposite condition. In a subsequent forced choice task, participants were asked to guess the real translation from two alternatives. They did this with 73.0% accuracy, well above chance level even for words they had encountered in the opposite condition, showing that people are generally sensitive to the sound-symbolic cues in ideophones. Individual difference measures showed that the ERP effect in the test round of the learning task was greater for participants who were more sensitive to sound symbolism in the forced choice task. The main driver of the difference was a lower amplitude of the P3 component in response to ideophones in the opposite condition, suggesting that people who are more sensitive to sound symbolism may have more difficulty to suppress conflicting cross-modal information. The findings provide new evidence that cross-modal correspondences between sound and meaning facilitate word learning, while cross-modal clashes make word learning harder, especially for people who are more sensitive to sound symbolism.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66879231","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 43
The interplay between subjectivity, statistical practice, and psychological science 主观性、统计实践和心理科学之间的相互作用
Pub Date : 2016-05-11 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.28
Jeffrey N. Rouder, R. Morey, E. Wagenmakers
Bayesian inference has been advocated as an alternative to conventional analysis in psychological science. Bayesians stress that subjectivity is needed for principled inference, and subjectivity by-and-large has not been seen as desirable. This paper provides the broader rationale and context for subjectivity, and in it we show that subjectivity is the key to principled measures of evidence for theory from data. By making our subjective elements focal, we provide an avenue for common sense and expertise to enter the analysis. We cover the role of models in linking theory to data, the notion that models are abstractions which are neither true nor false, the need for relative model comparison, the role of predictions in stating relative evidence for models, and the role of subjectivity in specifying models that yield predictions. In the end, we conclude that transparent subjectivity leads to a more honest and fruitful analyses in psychological science.
在心理科学中,贝叶斯推理一直被提倡作为传统分析的替代方法。贝叶斯学派强调原则性推理需要主观性,而主观性总体上并不可取。本文为主观性提供了更广泛的理论基础和背景,在其中,我们表明主观性是从数据中为理论提供证据的原则措施的关键。通过使我们的主观元素成为焦点,我们为进入分析的常识和专业知识提供了一个途径。我们涵盖了模型在将理论与数据联系起来方面的作用,模型是既非真也非假的抽象概念,相对模型比较的必要性,预测在陈述模型的相对证据中的作用,以及主观性在指定产生预测的模型中的作用。最后,我们得出结论,透明的主观性使心理科学的分析更加诚实和富有成效。
{"title":"The interplay between subjectivity, statistical practice, and psychological science","authors":"Jeffrey N. Rouder, R. Morey, E. Wagenmakers","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.28","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.28","url":null,"abstract":"Bayesian inference has been advocated as an alternative to conventional analysis in psychological science. Bayesians stress that subjectivity is needed for principled inference, and subjectivity by-and-large has not been seen as desirable. This paper provides the broader rationale and context for subjectivity, and in it we show that subjectivity is the key to principled measures of evidence for theory from data. By making our subjective elements focal, we provide an avenue for common sense and expertise to enter the analysis. We cover the role of models in linking theory to data, the notion that models are abstractions which are neither true nor false, the need for relative model comparison, the role of predictions in stating relative evidence for models, and the role of subjectivity in specifying models that yield predictions. In the end, we conclude that transparent subjectivity leads to a more honest and fruitful analyses in psychological science.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66874602","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 37
Space Use by Swainson’s Hawk ( Buteo swainsoni ) in the Natomas Basin, California 斯温森鹰(Buteo swainsoni)在加州纳托马斯盆地的太空使用
Pub Date : 2016-04-07 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.35
E. Fleishman, J. Anderson, B. Dickson, D. Krolick, J. A. Estep, R. Anderson, C. Elphick, D. Dobkin, D. Bell
We used satellite-based remote sensing to estimate home ranges for Swainson’s Hawk, a species listed as threatened in California (USA), on its breeding grounds in the Natomas Basin (northern Central Valley, California) and to evaluate whether the species’ space-use intensity (statistically derived density of telemetry locations) was associated with land cover, sex, reproductive success, or life stage of offspring. We differentiated seven classes of land cover—alfalfa, annually rotated irrigated crops, developed, grassland, orchard / vineyard, rice, and water. From 2011–2013, we fitted transmitters with global positioning systems to 23 adult Swainson’s Hawks. We recorded a minimum of six locations per day per bird from spring through early autumn of each year. We used a fixed, bivariate-normal kernel estimator to calculate a utilization distribution at 30-m resolution for each life stage of each individual within each year. We used a linear mixed model to estimate the associations between intensity of space use and land cover, sex, and reproductive status. The majority of adult Swainson’s Hawks traveled distances up to 8–10 km from the nest throughout the breeding season. Median seasonal home-range sizes in a given year ranged from 87–172 km 2 . The association between intensity of space use and grassland was 50–139% stronger, and the association between intensity of space use and alfalfa 23–59% stronger, than the associations between intensity of space use and any other land-cover type. Intensity of space use did not vary as a function of sex, reproductive status, or life stage. Given our results and additional knowledge of the species’ ecology, we suggest that reproductive success and, in turn, population-level recruitment may be associated equally if not more closely with availability of nesting sites than with the current distribution of land cover.
我们使用基于卫星的遥感技术估算了美国加州濒危物种斯温森鹰在纳马斯盆地(加州中央山谷北部)的繁殖地的活动范围,并评估了该物种的空间利用强度(统计得出的遥测地点密度)是否与土地覆盖、性别、繁殖成功率或后代的生命阶段有关。我们将土地覆被划分为7类:苜蓿、轮作灌溉作物、发达、草地、果园/葡萄园、水稻和水。从2011年到2013年,我们为23只成年斯温森鹰安装了全球定位系统的发射器。每年从春天到初秋,我们每天至少记录每只鸟的六个地点。我们使用固定的双变量正态核估计器,以30米分辨率计算每个个体在每年的每个生命阶段的利用率分布。我们使用线性混合模型来估计空间利用强度与土地覆盖、性别和生殖状况之间的关系。在整个繁殖季节,大多数成年斯温森鹰都要离开巢穴8-10公里。一年中,季节性房屋面积的中位数在87-172平方公里之间。空间利用强度与草地的关联度为50 ~ 139%,与苜蓿的关联度为23 ~ 59%,高于其他土地覆被类型。空间使用的强度并没有随着性别、生育状况或生命阶段的变化而变化。鉴于我们的研究结果和对物种生态的进一步了解,我们认为繁殖成功以及种群水平的招募可能与筑巢地点的可用性相关联,如果不是与当前土地覆盖的分布更密切相关的话。
{"title":"Space Use by Swainson’s Hawk ( Buteo swainsoni ) in the Natomas Basin, California","authors":"E. Fleishman, J. Anderson, B. Dickson, D. Krolick, J. A. Estep, R. Anderson, C. Elphick, D. Dobkin, D. Bell","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.35","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.35","url":null,"abstract":"We used satellite-based remote sensing to estimate home ranges for Swainson’s Hawk, a species listed as threatened in California (USA), on its breeding grounds in the Natomas Basin (northern Central Valley, California) and to evaluate whether the species’ space-use intensity (statistically derived density of telemetry locations) was associated with land cover, sex, reproductive success, or life stage of offspring. We differentiated seven classes of land cover—alfalfa, annually rotated irrigated crops, developed, grassland, orchard / vineyard, rice, and water. From 2011–2013, we fitted transmitters with global positioning systems to 23 adult Swainson’s Hawks. We recorded a minimum of six locations per day per bird from spring through early autumn of each year. We used a fixed, bivariate-normal kernel estimator to calculate a utilization distribution at 30-m resolution for each life stage of each individual within each year. We used a linear mixed model to estimate the associations between intensity of space use and land cover, sex, and reproductive status. The majority of adult Swainson’s Hawks traveled distances up to 8–10 km from the nest throughout the breeding season. Median seasonal home-range sizes in a given year ranged from 87–172 km 2 . The association between intensity of space use and grassland was 50–139% stronger, and the association between intensity of space use and alfalfa 23–59% stronger, than the associations between intensity of space use and any other land-cover type. Intensity of space use did not vary as a function of sex, reproductive status, or life stage. Given our results and additional knowledge of the species’ ecology, we suggest that reproductive success and, in turn, population-level recruitment may be associated equally if not more closely with availability of nesting sites than with the current distribution of land cover.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-04-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66877614","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
'Standing in Awe': The effects of awe on body perception and the relation with absorption “敬畏”:敬畏对身体感知的影响以及与吸收的关系
Pub Date : 2016-03-23 DOI: 10.1525/COLLABRA.36
M. Elk, Annika K. Karinen, Eva Specker, E. Stamkou, M. Baas
The experience of awe is typically elicited in response to perceptually vast stimuli and is often characterized by feeling small and insignificant. In the present series of studies we aimed (1) to determine the effects of awe on body perception and (2) to elucidate the role of the personality trait of ‘absorption’ (i.e. the tendency to get fully immersed in one’s experiences) in relation to the feeling of awe. Across 4 different studies, involving both lab-based and field experiments, we found that feelings of awe are associated with smaller body size estimates. We also found that absorption is a strong predictor of feelings of awe: people scoring high on absorption tended to report overall stronger feelings of awe – irrespective of the experimental manipulation. In addition, experimentally manipulating absorption, by instructing participants to get fully absorbed in an external stimulus resulted in stronger feelings of awe. Thereby these findings illustrate that two key features underlying the experience of awe are changes in the perception of one’s body and a tendency to get absorbed in internal or external stimuli.
敬畏的体验通常是对巨大的感知刺激的反应,通常以感觉渺小和微不足道为特征。在本系列研究中,我们的目的是:(1)确定敬畏对身体感知的影响;(2)阐明“吸收”人格特质(即完全沉浸在个人经历中的倾向)在敬畏感中的作用。通过四项不同的研究,包括实验室和现场实验,我们发现敬畏感与较小的体型有关。我们还发现,吸收力是敬畏感的一个强有力的预测指标:无论实验操作如何,吸收力得分高的人总体上倾向于报告更强烈的敬畏感。此外,通过实验操纵吸收,通过指示参与者完全吸收外部刺激,导致更强的敬畏感。因此,这些发现表明,敬畏体验的两个关键特征是对身体感知的变化,以及被内部或外部刺激所吸引的倾向。
{"title":"'Standing in Awe': The effects of awe on body perception and the relation with absorption","authors":"M. Elk, Annika K. Karinen, Eva Specker, E. Stamkou, M. Baas","doi":"10.1525/COLLABRA.36","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/COLLABRA.36","url":null,"abstract":"The experience of awe is typically elicited in response to perceptually vast stimuli and is often characterized by feeling small and insignificant. In the present series of studies we aimed (1) to determine the effects of awe on body perception and (2) to elucidate the role of the personality trait of ‘absorption’ (i.e. the tendency to get fully immersed in one’s experiences) in relation to the feeling of awe. Across 4 different studies, involving both lab-based and field experiments, we found that feelings of awe are associated with smaller body size estimates. We also found that absorption is a strong predictor of feelings of awe: people scoring high on absorption tended to report overall stronger feelings of awe – irrespective of the experimental manipulation. In addition, experimentally manipulating absorption, by instructing participants to get fully absorbed in an external stimulus resulted in stronger feelings of awe. Thereby these findings illustrate that two key features underlying the experience of awe are changes in the perception of one’s body and a tendency to get absorbed in internal or external stimuli.","PeriodicalId":93422,"journal":{"name":"Collabra","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2016-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"66878248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 60
期刊
Collabra
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1