首页 > 最新文献

Frontiers in Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Flexible encoding of multiple task dimensions in human cerebral cortex 人类大脑皮层对多个任务维度的灵活编码
Pub Date : 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1438390
Benjamin J. Tamber-Rosenau, Allen T. Newton, René Marois
Cognitive models have proposed that behavioral tasks can be categorized along at least three dimensions: the sensory-motor modality of the information, its representational format (e.g., location vs. identity), and the cognitive processes that transform it (e.g., response selection). Moreover, we can quickly and flexibly encode, represent, or manipulate information along any of these dimensions. How is this flexibility in encoding such information implemented in the cerebral cortex?To address this question, we devised a series of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments in each of which participants performed two distinct tasks that differed along one of the three dimensions.Using multivariate pattern analysis of the fMRI data, we were able to decode between tasks along at least one task dimension within each of the cortical regions activated by these tasks. Moreover, the multiple demand network, a system of brain regions previously associated with flexible task encoding, was largely composed of closely juxtaposed sets of voxels that were specialized along each of the three tested task dimensions.These results suggest that flexible task encoding is primarily achieved by the juxtaposition of specialized representations processing each task dimension in the multiple demand network.
认知模型提出,行为任务至少可以从三个维度进行分类:信息的感觉运动模式、信息的表征形式(如位置与身份)以及转换信息的认知过程(如反应选择)。此外,我们还可以快速灵活地对信息进行编码、表征或处理。为了解决这个问题,我们设计了一系列功能性磁共振成像(fMRI)实验,在每项实验中,参与者都执行了两个不同的任务,这两个任务在三个维度中的一个维度上存在差异。通过对fMRI数据进行多元模式分析,我们能够在这些任务激活的每个皮层区域内,沿着至少一个任务维度对不同任务进行解码。这些结果表明,灵活的任务编码主要是通过在多重需求网络中并置处理每个任务维度的专门表征来实现的。
{"title":"Flexible encoding of multiple task dimensions in human cerebral cortex","authors":"Benjamin J. Tamber-Rosenau, Allen T. Newton, René Marois","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1438390","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1438390","url":null,"abstract":"Cognitive models have proposed that behavioral tasks can be categorized along at least three dimensions: the sensory-motor modality of the information, its representational format (e.g., location vs. identity), and the cognitive processes that transform it (e.g., response selection). Moreover, we can quickly and flexibly encode, represent, or manipulate information along any of these dimensions. How is this flexibility in encoding such information implemented in the cerebral cortex?To address this question, we devised a series of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments in each of which participants performed two distinct tasks that differed along one of the three dimensions.Using multivariate pattern analysis of the fMRI data, we were able to decode between tasks along at least one task dimension within each of the cortical regions activated by these tasks. Moreover, the multiple demand network, a system of brain regions previously associated with flexible task encoding, was largely composed of closely juxtaposed sets of voxels that were specialized along each of the three tested task dimensions.These results suggest that flexible task encoding is primarily achieved by the juxtaposition of specialized representations processing each task dimension in the multiple demand network.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":"90 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141807950","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cycle-based high-intensity sprint exercise elicits acute cognitive dysfunction in psychomotor and memory task performance 单车高强度短跑运动导致心理运动和记忆任务表现中的急性认知功能障碍
Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1419734
T. Dufner, J. Moon, Adam J. Wells
To examine the impact of an acute high-intensity sprint exercise protocol (HISEP) for eliciting post-exercise cognitive dysfunction in psychomotor, attentional, executive, and memory tasks.Twenty-four recreationally active adults (22 ± 4 yrs, 169.39 ± 10.07 cm, 75.80 ± 14.73 kg, 27.03 ± 9.55 BF%) performed a HISEP on a cycle ergometer. Average psychomotor reaction time (avgRT; Dynavision D2 Mode A & Mode B), mood (Profile of Mood States Questionnaire; POMS), and cognition (Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics; ANAM) were assessed pre- (PRE), post- (POST) and 60-min post (60POST) HISEP. One-way repeated measures ANOVAs were used to assess changes across time.Fatigue (main effect: p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.309) was significantly higher at POST compared to PRE (p = 0.007). Tension (main effect: p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.154) was significantly lower at 60POST compared to PRE (p = 0.029). Mode A avgRT (main effect: p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.153) was significantly slower at POST compared to PRE (p = 0.026). Throughput (TP) scores for ANAM code substitution-delayed (CSD) task (main effect: p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.284) and matching to sample (M2S) tasks (main effect: p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.169) were significantly lower at POST compared to PRE (p = 0.001 and p = 0.025, respectively), while mathematical processing (main effect: p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.232) was significantly higher at 60POST compared to both PRE (p = 0.019) and POST (p = 0.005). No other significant changes in cognitive task performance were observed (p's > 0.05).The HISEP is a feasible and time-effective fatiguing exercise stimulus capable of eliciting acute cognitive dysfunction in psychomotor and memory task performance. NCT05100589.
研究急性高强度冲刺运动方案(HISEP)对引起运动后心理运动、注意力、执行力和记忆任务认知功能障碍的影响。24 名从事娱乐活动的成年人(22 ± 4 岁,169.39 ± 10.07 厘米,75.80 ± 14.73 千克,27.03 ± 9.55 BF%)在自行车测力计上进行了 HISEP 运动。对HISEP前(PRE)、后(POST)和60分钟后(60POST)的平均精神运动反应时间(avgRT;Dynavision D2模式A和模式B)、情绪(情绪状态问卷;POMS)和认知(自动神经心理评估指标;ANAM)进行了评估。疲劳(主效应:p < 0.001,ηp2 = 0.309)与 PRE 相比,POST 显著增加(p = 0.007)。张力(主效应:p = 0.021,ηp2 = 0.154)在 60POST 显著低于 PRE(p = 0.029)。模式 A 的 avgRT(主效应:p = 0.022,ηp2 = 0.153)与前期相比,后期明显较慢(p = 0.026)。ANAM 代码替换延迟(CSD)任务(主效应:p < 0.001,ηp2 = 0.284)和样本匹配(M2S)任务(主效应:p = 0.014,ηp2 = 0.169)的吞吐量(TP)得分在 POST 阶段明显低于 PRE 阶段(p = 0.001 和 p = 0.025),而数学处理(主效应:p = 0.002,ηp2 = 0.232)在 60POST 阶段明显高于 PRE 阶段(p = 0.019)和 POST 阶段(p = 0.005)。HISEP是一种可行且具有时间效率的疲劳运动刺激,能够在精神运动和记忆任务中引起急性认知功能障碍。NCT05100589。
{"title":"Cycle-based high-intensity sprint exercise elicits acute cognitive dysfunction in psychomotor and memory task performance","authors":"T. Dufner, J. Moon, Adam J. Wells","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1419734","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1419734","url":null,"abstract":"To examine the impact of an acute high-intensity sprint exercise protocol (HISEP) for eliciting post-exercise cognitive dysfunction in psychomotor, attentional, executive, and memory tasks.Twenty-four recreationally active adults (22 ± 4 yrs, 169.39 ± 10.07 cm, 75.80 ± 14.73 kg, 27.03 ± 9.55 BF%) performed a HISEP on a cycle ergometer. Average psychomotor reaction time (avgRT; Dynavision D2 Mode A & Mode B), mood (Profile of Mood States Questionnaire; POMS), and cognition (Automated Neuropsychological Assessment Metrics; ANAM) were assessed pre- (PRE), post- (POST) and 60-min post (60POST) HISEP. One-way repeated measures ANOVAs were used to assess changes across time.Fatigue (main effect: p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.309) was significantly higher at POST compared to PRE (p = 0.007). Tension (main effect: p = 0.021, ηp2 = 0.154) was significantly lower at 60POST compared to PRE (p = 0.029). Mode A avgRT (main effect: p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.153) was significantly slower at POST compared to PRE (p = 0.026). Throughput (TP) scores for ANAM code substitution-delayed (CSD) task (main effect: p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.284) and matching to sample (M2S) tasks (main effect: p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.169) were significantly lower at POST compared to PRE (p = 0.001 and p = 0.025, respectively), while mathematical processing (main effect: p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.232) was significantly higher at 60POST compared to both PRE (p = 0.019) and POST (p = 0.005). No other significant changes in cognitive task performance were observed (p's > 0.05).The HISEP is a feasible and time-effective fatiguing exercise stimulus capable of eliciting acute cognitive dysfunction in psychomotor and memory task performance. NCT05100589.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":"81 10","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141810258","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Genetic background of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease 帕金森病认知能力下降的遗传背景
Pub Date : 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1379896
Antonela Blažeković, Kristina Gotovac Jerčić, Sabina Devedija, F. Borovečki
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a complex disorder that is influenced by multiple genetic risk factors. There is a significant heterogeneity in PD presentation, both pathologically and clinically. Some of the most common and important symptoms affecting the patient are cognitive impairment and dementia. However, the genetic and biological basis underlying the differences in cognitive profiles, including the development of dementia in PD, is not yet well understood. Understanding the role of genes in cognitive outcomes is crucial for effective patient counseling and treatment. Research on familial PD has discovered more than 20 genes that can cause the disease. The identified genes responsible for familial cases of PD are LRRK2, PARK7, PINK1, PRKN, or SNCA gene, although there may be other genes that also contribute. Additionally, some of these genes may also play a role in cases that were previously thought to be sporadic. Currently, numerous well-described genes increase the risk of cognitive decline in PD, each with varying levels of penetrance. The aim of this review is to identify the relevant genetic factors that contribute to differences in cognition. We discuss the genes that may affect cognition and the challenges in establishing a clear genetic diagnostic and prognostic assessment. This article aims to demonstrate the complexity of the genetic background of cognition in PD and to present the different types of genotype changes that can impact cognition through various neurobiological mechanisms.
帕金森病(PD)是一种受多种遗传风险因素影响的复杂疾病。帕金森病在病理和临床表现上存在明显的异质性。影响患者的一些最常见和最重要的症状是认知障碍和痴呆。然而,人们对认知特征差异(包括帕金森病痴呆症的发生)的遗传和生物学基础尚不十分清楚。了解基因在认知结果中的作用对于有效的患者咨询和治疗至关重要。对家族性帕金森病的研究发现了 20 多个可导致该病的基因。已发现的导致家族性帕金森病的基因有 LRRK2、PARK7、PINK1、PRKN 或 SNCA 基因,但可能还有其他基因也会导致该病。此外,其中一些基因也可能在以前被认为是散发性的病例中发挥作用。目前,许多已明确描述的基因会增加帕金森病认知能力下降的风险,每种基因的渗透程度各不相同。本综述旨在确定导致认知差异的相关遗传因素。我们讨论了可能影响认知的基因,以及建立明确的基因诊断和预后评估所面临的挑战。本文旨在展示认知障碍症遗传背景的复杂性,并介绍可通过各种神经生物学机制影响认知的不同类型的基因型变化。
{"title":"Genetic background of cognitive decline in Parkinson's disease","authors":"Antonela Blažeković, Kristina Gotovac Jerčić, Sabina Devedija, F. Borovečki","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1379896","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1379896","url":null,"abstract":"Parkinson's disease (PD) is a complex disorder that is influenced by multiple genetic risk factors. There is a significant heterogeneity in PD presentation, both pathologically and clinically. Some of the most common and important symptoms affecting the patient are cognitive impairment and dementia. However, the genetic and biological basis underlying the differences in cognitive profiles, including the development of dementia in PD, is not yet well understood. Understanding the role of genes in cognitive outcomes is crucial for effective patient counseling and treatment. Research on familial PD has discovered more than 20 genes that can cause the disease. The identified genes responsible for familial cases of PD are LRRK2, PARK7, PINK1, PRKN, or SNCA gene, although there may be other genes that also contribute. Additionally, some of these genes may also play a role in cases that were previously thought to be sporadic. Currently, numerous well-described genes increase the risk of cognitive decline in PD, each with varying levels of penetrance. The aim of this review is to identify the relevant genetic factors that contribute to differences in cognition. We discuss the genes that may affect cognition and the challenges in establishing a clear genetic diagnostic and prognostic assessment. This article aims to demonstrate the complexity of the genetic background of cognition in PD and to present the different types of genotype changes that can impact cognition through various neurobiological mechanisms.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":"20 26","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141817399","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
This time with feeling: recommendations for full-bodied reporting of research on dance 这一次有感而发:关于全面报道舞蹈研究的建议
Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1385087
Rebecca Barnstaple, Jessie Laurita-Spanglet, Jason Fanning, C. Soriano, C. Hugenschmidt
Collaborations that employ methods from arts and sciences to address research questions through multimodal study design are becoming more frequent, as it is increasingly apparent that complex challenges require transdisciplinary solutions. These different modalities rely on interdisciplinary exchange while involving unique expertise in associated delivery practices. In human subject driven scientific research specifically, guidelines for arts-based interventions deserve detailed reporting to allow for fidelity, replicability, and uptake of innovation and results. Details such as frequency, duration, delivery method, expected outcomes, historical precedence, and instructor training are crucial, along with nuanced descriptions pertaining to embodied aspects of specific dance or movement style(s) and adaptations made for the population or study design. This Perspective Paper outlines the current state and challenges of reporting on dance interventions and makes recommendations based on our experience as teaching artists who work in research settings alongside researchers who collaborate with dance professionals.
通过多模式研究设计,采用艺术和科学方法解决研究问题的合作越来越频繁,因为人们越来越清楚地认识到,复杂的挑战需要跨学科的解决方案。这些不同的模式依赖于跨学科交流,同时涉及相关交付实践中的独特专业知识。具体来说,在以人为研究对象的科学研究中,以艺术为基础的干预指南需要详细的报告,以保证创新和成果的忠实性、可复制性和可接受性。频率、持续时间、实施方法、预期结果、历史先例和指导者培训等细节至关重要,同时还要对特定舞蹈或动作风格的体现方面以及针对人群或研究设计所做的调整进行细致入微的描述。本视角论文概述了舞蹈干预报告的现状和挑战,并根据我们作为在研究环境中工作的教学艺术家以及与舞蹈专业人员合作的研究人员的经验提出了建议。
{"title":"This time with feeling: recommendations for full-bodied reporting of research on dance","authors":"Rebecca Barnstaple, Jessie Laurita-Spanglet, Jason Fanning, C. Soriano, C. Hugenschmidt","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1385087","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1385087","url":null,"abstract":"Collaborations that employ methods from arts and sciences to address research questions through multimodal study design are becoming more frequent, as it is increasingly apparent that complex challenges require transdisciplinary solutions. These different modalities rely on interdisciplinary exchange while involving unique expertise in associated delivery practices. In human subject driven scientific research specifically, guidelines for arts-based interventions deserve detailed reporting to allow for fidelity, replicability, and uptake of innovation and results. Details such as frequency, duration, delivery method, expected outcomes, historical precedence, and instructor training are crucial, along with nuanced descriptions pertaining to embodied aspects of specific dance or movement style(s) and adaptations made for the population or study design. This Perspective Paper outlines the current state and challenges of reporting on dance interventions and makes recommendations based on our experience as teaching artists who work in research settings alongside researchers who collaborate with dance professionals.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":" 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141826965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's recognition of slapstick humor is linked to their Theory of Mind 儿童对滑稽幽默的识别与他们的心智理论有关
Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1369638
E. Ger, Moritz M. Daum, Mirella Manfredi
Humor is an important component of children's learning and development. Yet, the cognitive mechanisms that underlie humor recognition in children have not been well-researched. In this pre-registered study, we asked whether (1) 4- to 5-year-old children recognize and categorize a misfortunate situation as funny only if the victims show a funny bewildered face (slapstick humor), and not a painful or angry expression, (2) this ability increases with age, (3) it is associated with children's Theory of Mind (ToM) abilities, (4) it is related to the ability to recognize facial emotional expressions. In an online experiment platform, children (N = 61, Mage = 53 months) were asked to point to the funny picture between a funny and an affective picture. Then, children were asked to point to the happy, sad, fearful, or angry face among four faces displaying these emotions. Children's ToM was assessed using the Children's Social Understanding Scale (CSUS), which was filled out online by parents. Results showed that from the earliest age onward, the predicted probability of humor recognition exceeded the chance level. Only ToM but not age was a significant predictor. Children with higher ToM scores showed better humor recognition. We found no evidence for a relation between children's humor recognition and their recognition of any emotion (happy, sad, fearful, or angry). Our findings suggest that 4–5-year-old children recognize facial emotional expressions and slapstick humor, although these abilities seem unrelated. Instead, children's understanding of mental states appears to play a role in their recognition of slapstick humor.
幽默是儿童学习和发展的重要组成部分。然而,对儿童识别幽默的认知机制还没有进行深入研究。在这项预先登记的研究中,我们询问:(1) 只有当受害者表现出滑稽茫然的表情(滑稽幽默),而不是痛苦或愤怒的表情时,4-5 岁的儿童才能识别并将不幸的情况归类为有趣的;(2) 这种能力是否会随着年龄的增长而增强;(3) 这种能力是否与儿童的心智理论(ToM)能力有关;(4) 这种能力是否与识别面部情绪表达的能力有关。在一个在线实验平台上,儿童(N = 61,Mage = 53 个月)被要求在滑稽图片和情感图片之间指出滑稽图片。然后,要求儿童在四张显示这些情绪的脸部表情中指出快乐、悲伤、恐惧或愤怒的脸部表情。儿童的 ToM 采用儿童社会理解量表 (CSUS) 进行评估,该量表由家长在线填写。结果显示,从最早的年龄开始,幽默识别的预测概率就超过了偶然水平。只有 ToM 而不是年龄是一个重要的预测因素。ToM 分数越高的儿童对幽默的识别能力越强。我们没有发现任何证据表明儿童的幽默识别能力与他们对任何情绪(快乐、悲伤、恐惧或愤怒)的识别能力有关。我们的研究结果表明,4-5 岁的儿童可以识别面部情绪表达和滑稽幽默,尽管这些能力似乎并不相关。相反,儿童对心理状态的理解似乎对他们识别滑稽幽默起了作用。
{"title":"Children's recognition of slapstick humor is linked to their Theory of Mind","authors":"E. Ger, Moritz M. Daum, Mirella Manfredi","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1369638","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1369638","url":null,"abstract":"Humor is an important component of children's learning and development. Yet, the cognitive mechanisms that underlie humor recognition in children have not been well-researched. In this pre-registered study, we asked whether (1) 4- to 5-year-old children recognize and categorize a misfortunate situation as funny only if the victims show a funny bewildered face (slapstick humor), and not a painful or angry expression, (2) this ability increases with age, (3) it is associated with children's Theory of Mind (ToM) abilities, (4) it is related to the ability to recognize facial emotional expressions. In an online experiment platform, children (N = 61, Mage = 53 months) were asked to point to the funny picture between a funny and an affective picture. Then, children were asked to point to the happy, sad, fearful, or angry face among four faces displaying these emotions. Children's ToM was assessed using the Children's Social Understanding Scale (CSUS), which was filled out online by parents. Results showed that from the earliest age onward, the predicted probability of humor recognition exceeded the chance level. Only ToM but not age was a significant predictor. Children with higher ToM scores showed better humor recognition. We found no evidence for a relation between children's humor recognition and their recognition of any emotion (happy, sad, fearful, or angry). Our findings suggest that 4–5-year-old children recognize facial emotional expressions and slapstick humor, although these abilities seem unrelated. Instead, children's understanding of mental states appears to play a role in their recognition of slapstick humor.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":"45 17","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141109537","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Passive exercise provides a simultaneous and postexercise executive function benefit 被动运动可同时为运动后的执行功能带来益处
Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1334258
Connor Dalton, Chloe Edgar, Benjamin Tari, Matthew Heath
Passive exercise involves limb movement via an external force and is an intervention providing an immediate postexercise executive function (EF) benefit. It is, however, unknown whether EF is improved simultaneous with passive exercise—a salient question given the advent of passive (and active) exercise workstations designed to enhance productivity and wellbeing for individuals engaged in sedentary occupations.Here, participants (N = 23) completed separate 20-min conditions involving active (i.e., via volitional muscle activation) and passive (i.e., via mechanically driven cycle ergometer) cycle ergometry and a non-exercise control condition. EF was assessed prior to (i.e., preintervention), simultaneous with, and immediately after (post-intervention) each condition via the antipointing task. Antipointing involves a goal-directed limb movement mirror-symmetrical to a target and is an ideal tool for the current investigation given that the task is mediated via EF inhibitory control networks that show response-dependent changes following a single bout of exercise.Results showed that passive exercise produced a simultaneous and post-intervention reduction in antipointing reaction time (RT), whereas active exercise selectively produced a post-intervention—but not simultaneous—RT reduction. Thus, passive and active exercise elicited a postexercise EF benefit; however, only passive exercise produced a simultaneous benefit. That passive—but not active—exercise produced a simultaneous benefit may reflect that the intervention provides the necessary physiological or psychological changes to elicit improved EF efficiency without the associated dual-task cost(s) of volitional muscle activity.
被动运动涉及通过外力进行肢体运动,是一种能在运动后立即改善执行功能(EF)的干预措施。这里,参与者(N = 23)分别完成了20分钟的主动(即通过意志肌肉激活)和被动(即通过机械驱动的自行车测力计)自行车测力以及非运动对照条件。通过反指向任务,在每个条件之前(即干预前)、同时和紧随其后(干预后)对EF进行评估。反指向任务包括与目标镜像对称的目标指向性肢体运动,是目前研究的理想工具,因为该任务是通过EF抑制控制网络介导的,而这些网络在单次运动后会出现反应依赖性变化。结果显示,被动运动会导致反指向反应时间(RT)同时和干预后缩短,而主动运动会选择性地导致干预后RT缩短,但不是同时缩短。因此,被动运动和主动运动都能在运动后产生 EF 益处;但只有被动运动能同时产生益处。被动运动--而非主动运动--产生了同步益处,这可能反映出干预措施提供了必要的生理或心理变化,从而提高了 EF 效率,而不需要意志肌肉活动的相关双任务成本。
{"title":"Passive exercise provides a simultaneous and postexercise executive function benefit","authors":"Connor Dalton, Chloe Edgar, Benjamin Tari, Matthew Heath","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1334258","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1334258","url":null,"abstract":"Passive exercise involves limb movement via an external force and is an intervention providing an immediate postexercise executive function (EF) benefit. It is, however, unknown whether EF is improved simultaneous with passive exercise—a salient question given the advent of passive (and active) exercise workstations designed to enhance productivity and wellbeing for individuals engaged in sedentary occupations.Here, participants (N = 23) completed separate 20-min conditions involving active (i.e., via volitional muscle activation) and passive (i.e., via mechanically driven cycle ergometer) cycle ergometry and a non-exercise control condition. EF was assessed prior to (i.e., preintervention), simultaneous with, and immediately after (post-intervention) each condition via the antipointing task. Antipointing involves a goal-directed limb movement mirror-symmetrical to a target and is an ideal tool for the current investigation given that the task is mediated via EF inhibitory control networks that show response-dependent changes following a single bout of exercise.Results showed that passive exercise produced a simultaneous and post-intervention reduction in antipointing reaction time (RT), whereas active exercise selectively produced a post-intervention—but not simultaneous—RT reduction. Thus, passive and active exercise elicited a postexercise EF benefit; however, only passive exercise produced a simultaneous benefit. That passive—but not active—exercise produced a simultaneous benefit may reflect that the intervention provides the necessary physiological or psychological changes to elicit improved EF efficiency without the associated dual-task cost(s) of volitional muscle activity.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":"39 15","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141119002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contextual cueing—Eye movements in rotated and recombined displays 语境提示--旋转和重组显示屏中的眼球运动
Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1403749
Lei Zheng, Nico Marek, Natalia Melnik, Stefan Pollmann
Contextual cueing leads to improved efficiency in visual search resulting from the extraction of spatial regularities in repeated visual stimuli. Previous research has demonstrated the independent contributions of global configuration and spatial position to contextual cueing. The present study aimed to investigate whether learned spatial configuration or individual locations would elicit fixation patterns resembling those observed in the original displays. We found that search guidance based on either local or global spatial context, by combining distractor locations from two learned displays or rotating displays, kept not only search time facilitation intact, in agreement with previous studies, but also enabled search with less fixations and more direct scan paths to the target. Fixation distribution maps of recombined or rotated displays were more similar to the original displays than random new displays. However, for rotated displays this was only true when the rotation angle was taken into account. Overall, this shows an astonishingly flexible use of the oculomotor system for search in incompletely repeated displays.
情境提示可通过提取重复视觉刺激中的空间规律性来提高视觉搜索的效率。以前的研究已经证明,全局配置和空间位置对情境提示有独立的贡献。本研究旨在探讨学习到的空间构型或单个位置是否会引发与原始显示中观察到的类似的固定模式。我们发现,基于局部或全局空间情境的搜索引导,通过结合两个已学过的显示或旋转显示中的分心位置,不仅能保持搜索时间上的促进作用,这与之前的研究一致,而且还能使搜索时的定点次数更少,扫描路径更直接地到达目标。与随机新显示相比,重组或旋转显示的定点分布图与原始显示更为相似。然而,对于旋转显示,只有在考虑到旋转角度时才会出现这种情况。总之,这表明在不完全重复的显示中,眼动系统可以非常灵活地用于搜索。
{"title":"Contextual cueing—Eye movements in rotated and recombined displays","authors":"Lei Zheng, Nico Marek, Natalia Melnik, Stefan Pollmann","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1403749","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1403749","url":null,"abstract":"Contextual cueing leads to improved efficiency in visual search resulting from the extraction of spatial regularities in repeated visual stimuli. Previous research has demonstrated the independent contributions of global configuration and spatial position to contextual cueing. The present study aimed to investigate whether learned spatial configuration or individual locations would elicit fixation patterns resembling those observed in the original displays. We found that search guidance based on either local or global spatial context, by combining distractor locations from two learned displays or rotating displays, kept not only search time facilitation intact, in agreement with previous studies, but also enabled search with less fixations and more direct scan paths to the target. Fixation distribution maps of recombined or rotated displays were more similar to the original displays than random new displays. However, for rotated displays this was only true when the rotation angle was taken into account. Overall, this shows an astonishingly flexible use of the oculomotor system for search in incompletely repeated displays.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":" 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140992249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Yoga improves self-reported cognitive function among cancer survivors: results from the STAYFit trial 瑜伽能改善癌症幸存者的自我认知功能:STAYFit 试验的结果
Pub Date : 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1334727
N. Gothe, Emily D. Erlenbach, Elizabeth A. Salerno
Various physical activity-based interventions have been tested to determine their efficacy in improving cancer related cognitive decline (CRCD), however the role of mind-body practices such as yoga remains to be explored. In this manuscript we present preliminary effects of yoga vs. aerobic and stretching-toning modalities of exercise on CRCD among adult cancer survivors.Participants (N = 78) were randomized to one of the three exercise groups for a duration of 12-weeks and engaged in ≥150 min per week of supervised group exercises. At baseline and following the 12-week interventions, participants completed the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy–Cognitive Function.Results demonstrated a significant group*time interaction for FACT-Cog perceived cognitive abilities subscale, with participants in the yoga group demonstrating a significant increase as compared to the aerobic and stretching-toning groups. The FACT-Cog total score showed a significant time effect with all groups demonstrating a significant increase at follow-up. Other subscales did not show any significant improvements.These findings provide promising evidence for the effects of yoga on self-reported cognitive function in cancer survivors. Notably, 12-weeks of yoga showed an increase in the perceived cognitive abilities and demonstrated a clinically meaningful increase in total cognitive function as measured by the FACT-Cog, suggesting that this exercise modality has the potential to impact this important health outcome during cancer survivorship.ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier: NCT03650322.
人们已经测试了各种基于体育锻炼的干预措施,以确定它们在改善与癌症相关的认知功能下降(CRCD)方面的功效,但瑜伽等身心练习的作用仍有待探索。在本手稿中,我们介绍了瑜伽与有氧运动和伸展调理运动对成年癌症幸存者认知功能下降的初步影响。参与者(N = 78)被随机分配到三个运动组中的一个,为期 12 周,每周参加≥150 分钟的有指导的集体运动。结果显示,在FACT-Cog认知能力分量表中,组别与时间之间存在显著的交互作用,瑜伽组的参与者与有氧组和伸展调理组相比,认知能力分量表有显著提高。FACT-Cog 总分显示出显著的时间效应,所有组别在随访时都有显著提高。这些发现为瑜伽对癌症幸存者自我报告的认知功能的影响提供了有希望的证据。值得注意的是,为期 12 周的瑜伽训练显示了认知能力的提高,并通过 FACT-Cog 测定显示了有临床意义的总认知功能的提高,这表明这种锻炼方式有可能对癌症幸存者的这一重要健康结果产生影响:NCT03650322。
{"title":"Yoga improves self-reported cognitive function among cancer survivors: results from the STAYFit trial","authors":"N. Gothe, Emily D. Erlenbach, Elizabeth A. Salerno","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1334727","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1334727","url":null,"abstract":"Various physical activity-based interventions have been tested to determine their efficacy in improving cancer related cognitive decline (CRCD), however the role of mind-body practices such as yoga remains to be explored. In this manuscript we present preliminary effects of yoga vs. aerobic and stretching-toning modalities of exercise on CRCD among adult cancer survivors.Participants (N = 78) were randomized to one of the three exercise groups for a duration of 12-weeks and engaged in ≥150 min per week of supervised group exercises. At baseline and following the 12-week interventions, participants completed the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy–Cognitive Function.Results demonstrated a significant group*time interaction for FACT-Cog perceived cognitive abilities subscale, with participants in the yoga group demonstrating a significant increase as compared to the aerobic and stretching-toning groups. The FACT-Cog total score showed a significant time effect with all groups demonstrating a significant increase at follow-up. Other subscales did not show any significant improvements.These findings provide promising evidence for the effects of yoga on self-reported cognitive function in cancer survivors. Notably, 12-weeks of yoga showed an increase in the perceived cognitive abilities and demonstrated a clinically meaningful increase in total cognitive function as measured by the FACT-Cog, suggesting that this exercise modality has the potential to impact this important health outcome during cancer survivorship.ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier: NCT03650322.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":" 3","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140992479","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relationship of physical activity and cognitive functioning among breast cancer survivors: a cross-sectional analysis 体育锻炼与乳腺癌幸存者认知功能的关系:横断面分析
Pub Date : 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1332960
Sheri J Hartman, Rong W Zablocki, Rowena M Tam, Barton W. Palmer, Barbara A. Parker, Dorothy D. Sears, T. Ahles, Loki Natarajan
Cancer related cognitive decline is a common long-term side effect of cancer and its treatments among breast cancer survivors. Physical activity is a modifiable risk factor related to cognitive decline. However, existing research lacks consensus regarding the relationship between cognition and exercise as well as the impact of cancer treatments on this relationship. Baseline data from an ongoing randomized clinical trial was utilized to examine the relationship between self-reported and objectively measured cognition with physical activity. Exploratory analyses examined cancer treatments as potential moderators.Breast cancer survivors (N = 253) completed a battery of neurocognitive tests, the PROMIS Cognitive abilities questionnaire, medical charts abstracted for treatment information, and wore an ActiGraph accelerometer at the waist for 7 days. Data were analyzed using multiple linear regression models.Participants were on average 58.5 (SD = 8.88) years old, diagnosed 3 years prior to enrollment (SD = 1.27) with 57% treated with chemotherapy and 80% receiving hormone therapy at baseline. Better self-reported cognitive ability was significantly associated with greater min of moderate to vigorous physical activity (MVPA; β = 0.070, se = 0.028, p = 0.012). There were no significant associations with any objectively measured cognitive domains. Time since diagnosis (years) was a significant moderator of MVPA and Processing Speed (β = −0.103, se = 0.043, p = 0.017). Treatment with chemotherapy and/or hormones did not significantly moderate the relationship between MVPA and any of the cognitive measures or domains.Findings suggest that physical activity is related to self-reported cognition but not objectively measured cognition. Greater physical activity was associated with faster processing speed in participants closer in time to their cancer diagnosis. These results emphasize the need for more research to understand when cancer survivors may benefit from physical activity and what aspects of cognition may be improved.
在乳腺癌幸存者中,与癌症相关的认知能力下降是癌症及其治疗常见的长期副作用。体育锻炼是导致认知能力下降的一个可调节的风险因素。然而,现有研究对认知与运动之间的关系以及癌症治疗对这种关系的影响缺乏共识。我们利用一项正在进行的随机临床试验的基线数据来研究自我报告和客观测量的认知与体育锻炼之间的关系。乳腺癌幸存者(253 人)完成了一系列神经认知测试、PROMIS 认知能力问卷调查、医疗图表治疗信息摘录,并在腰部佩戴 ActiGraph 加速计 7 天。参与者的平均年龄为 58.5 岁(SD = 8.88),入组前 3 年确诊(SD = 1.27),基线时 57% 接受化疗,80% 接受激素治疗。自述认知能力的提高与中度到剧烈运动(MVPA;β = 0.070,se = 0.028,p = 0.012)的分钟数增加有显著关系。与任何客观测量的认知领域均无明显关联。确诊后的时间(年)是 MVPA 和处理速度的重要调节因子(β = -0.103,se = 0.043,p = 0.017)。研究结果表明,体育锻炼与自我报告的认知能力有关,但与客观测量的认知能力无关。更多的体育锻炼与参与者更快的处理速度有关,因为参与者更接近癌症诊断。这些结果表明,有必要开展更多的研究,以了解癌症幸存者何时可从体育锻炼中获益,以及哪些方面的认知可能会得到改善。
{"title":"Relationship of physical activity and cognitive functioning among breast cancer survivors: a cross-sectional analysis","authors":"Sheri J Hartman, Rong W Zablocki, Rowena M Tam, Barton W. Palmer, Barbara A. Parker, Dorothy D. Sears, T. Ahles, Loki Natarajan","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1332960","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1332960","url":null,"abstract":"Cancer related cognitive decline is a common long-term side effect of cancer and its treatments among breast cancer survivors. Physical activity is a modifiable risk factor related to cognitive decline. However, existing research lacks consensus regarding the relationship between cognition and exercise as well as the impact of cancer treatments on this relationship. Baseline data from an ongoing randomized clinical trial was utilized to examine the relationship between self-reported and objectively measured cognition with physical activity. Exploratory analyses examined cancer treatments as potential moderators.Breast cancer survivors (N = 253) completed a battery of neurocognitive tests, the PROMIS Cognitive abilities questionnaire, medical charts abstracted for treatment information, and wore an ActiGraph accelerometer at the waist for 7 days. Data were analyzed using multiple linear regression models.Participants were on average 58.5 (SD = 8.88) years old, diagnosed 3 years prior to enrollment (SD = 1.27) with 57% treated with chemotherapy and 80% receiving hormone therapy at baseline. Better self-reported cognitive ability was significantly associated with greater min of moderate to vigorous physical activity (MVPA; β = 0.070, se = 0.028, p = 0.012). There were no significant associations with any objectively measured cognitive domains. Time since diagnosis (years) was a significant moderator of MVPA and Processing Speed (β = −0.103, se = 0.043, p = 0.017). Treatment with chemotherapy and/or hormones did not significantly moderate the relationship between MVPA and any of the cognitive measures or domains.Findings suggest that physical activity is related to self-reported cognition but not objectively measured cognition. Greater physical activity was associated with faster processing speed in participants closer in time to their cancer diagnosis. These results emphasize the need for more research to understand when cancer survivors may benefit from physical activity and what aspects of cognition may be improved.","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":" 48","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140996122","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Editorial: Cognitive-motor development and its consequences in children with neurodevelopmental disorders 社论:神经发育障碍儿童的认知运动发展及其后果
Pub Date : 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1417227
Yao-Chuen Li
{"title":"Editorial: Cognitive-motor development and its consequences in children with neurodevelopmental disorders","authors":"Yao-Chuen Li","doi":"10.3389/fcogn.2024.1417227","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3389/fcogn.2024.1417227","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":94013,"journal":{"name":"Frontiers in Cognition","volume":" 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140997892","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Frontiers in Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1