首页 > 最新文献

Cerebral cortex最新文献

英文 中文
When emotion and time meet from human and rodent perspectives: a central role for the amygdala? 从人类和啮齿动物的角度看情感与时间的交汇:杏仁核的核心作用?
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae454
Valérie Doyère, Sylvie Droit-Volet

Initiated by a long stay of Valérie Doyère in the laboratory of Joseph LeDoux, a Franco-American collaborative group was formed around the topic of emotion and time perception in a comparative perspective between humans and non-human animals. Here, we discuss results from our studies on the mechanisms underlying time distortion under 2 conditions, timing of a threatening stimulus and timing of a neutral stimulus in the context of fear, with insights from neurodevelopment. Although the type of temporal distortion depends on the experimental situations, in both humans and rodents a high-arousal emotion automatically triggers acceleration of an "internal clock" system, an effect that may rely on the early maturing amygdala. Our studies, particularly in humans, also point to the role of attention and self-awareness in regulating the effect of fear on timing, relying on the prefrontal cortex, a late maturing structure. Thus, in line with LeDoux, while the amygdala may process all characteristics of events (including time) necessary to quickly trigger appropriate survival behaviors, some type of time distortions may rely on higher-order processing, some specific to humans. The extent of the network underlying threat-related time distortions remains to be explored, with species comparisons being a promising means of investigation.

瓦莱丽-多耶尔(Valérie Doyère)曾长期在约瑟夫-勒杜的实验室工作,在她的倡议下,一个法美合作小组围绕情绪和时间感知这一主题,从人类和非人类动物的比较角度展开了研究。在此,我们将结合神经发育的启示,讨论在两种情况下时间扭曲的机制研究结果,即在恐惧背景下对威胁性刺激的计时和对中性刺激的计时。虽然时间扭曲的类型取决于实验情境,但在人类和啮齿类动物中,高唤醒情绪会自动触发 "内部时钟 "系统加速,这种效应可能依赖于早期成熟的杏仁核。我们的研究,尤其是对人类的研究,还指出了注意力和自我意识在调节恐惧对时间的影响方面所起的作用,这种作用依赖于前额叶皮层这一晚熟结构。因此,与勒杜(LeDoux)的观点一致,虽然杏仁核可能会处理所有必要的事件特征(包括时间),以迅速触发适当的生存行为,但某些类型的时间扭曲可能依赖于高阶处理,其中一些是人类特有的。与威胁有关的时间扭曲的基础网络的范围仍有待探索,物种比较是一种很有前景的研究手段。
{"title":"When emotion and time meet from human and rodent perspectives: a central role for the amygdala?","authors":"Valérie Doyère, Sylvie Droit-Volet","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae454","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae454","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Initiated by a long stay of Valérie Doyère in the laboratory of Joseph LeDoux, a Franco-American collaborative group was formed around the topic of emotion and time perception in a comparative perspective between humans and non-human animals. Here, we discuss results from our studies on the mechanisms underlying time distortion under 2 conditions, timing of a threatening stimulus and timing of a neutral stimulus in the context of fear, with insights from neurodevelopment. Although the type of temporal distortion depends on the experimental situations, in both humans and rodents a high-arousal emotion automatically triggers acceleration of an \"internal clock\" system, an effect that may rely on the early maturing amygdala. Our studies, particularly in humans, also point to the role of attention and self-awareness in regulating the effect of fear on timing, relying on the prefrontal cortex, a late maturing structure. Thus, in line with LeDoux, while the amygdala may process all characteristics of events (including time) necessary to quickly trigger appropriate survival behaviors, some type of time distortions may rely on higher-order processing, some specific to humans. The extent of the network underlying threat-related time distortions remains to be explored, with species comparisons being a promising means of investigation.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142692784","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in functional connectivity during suppression of imagined threat. 抑制想象威胁时功能连接的个体差异。
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae458
Marlon Westhoff, Christoph Vogelbacher, Verena Schuster, Stefan G Hofmann

Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies typically rely on between-person analyses. To examine individual differences in functional connectivity, we used Group Iterative Multiple Model Estimation and its subgrouping function to analyze functional magnetic resonance imaging data of 54 participants who were suppressing imagined future threat. A two-stage random-effects meta-analytic approach was employed to examine individual differences. In addition to generalizable connections between brain regions, we identified individual differences in personalized models suggesting different pathways through which individuals suppress future threat. Two subgroups with distinct connectivity patterns emerged: One subgroup (n = 29; 53.70%), characterized by an additional lagged connection from the right to the left posterior cingulate cortex, exhibited comparatively higher anxiety and less brain connectivity, whereas the other subgroup (n = 25; 46.30%), showing an additional connection from the left posterior cingulate cortex to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, was associated with lower anxiety levels and greater connectivity. This study points to individual differences in functional connectivity during emotion regulation.

功能磁共振成像研究通常依赖于人与人之间的分析。为了研究功能连通性的个体差异,我们使用了群体迭代多重模型估计及其分组功能,分析了 54 名抑制想象的未来威胁的参与者的功能磁共振成像数据。我们采用了两阶段随机效应荟萃分析方法来研究个体差异。除了大脑区域之间可通用的连接外,我们还发现了个性化模型中的个体差异,这表明个体抑制未来威胁的途径不同。我们发现了两个具有不同连接模式的亚组:一个亚组(n = 29;53.70%)的特点是从右侧到左侧扣带回后皮层的额外滞后连接,表现出相对较高的焦虑水平和较低的大脑连接性;而另一个亚组(n = 25;46.30%)则表现出从左侧扣带回后皮层到腹内侧前额叶皮层的额外连接,与较低的焦虑水平和较高的连接性相关。这项研究表明,情绪调节过程中的功能连接存在个体差异。
{"title":"Individual differences in functional connectivity during suppression of imagined threat.","authors":"Marlon Westhoff, Christoph Vogelbacher, Verena Schuster, Stefan G Hofmann","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae458","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae458","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies typically rely on between-person analyses. To examine individual differences in functional connectivity, we used Group Iterative Multiple Model Estimation and its subgrouping function to analyze functional magnetic resonance imaging data of 54 participants who were suppressing imagined future threat. A two-stage random-effects meta-analytic approach was employed to examine individual differences. In addition to generalizable connections between brain regions, we identified individual differences in personalized models suggesting different pathways through which individuals suppress future threat. Two subgroups with distinct connectivity patterns emerged: One subgroup (n = 29; 53.70%), characterized by an additional lagged connection from the right to the left posterior cingulate cortex, exhibited comparatively higher anxiety and less brain connectivity, whereas the other subgroup (n = 25; 46.30%), showing an additional connection from the left posterior cingulate cortex to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, was associated with lower anxiety levels and greater connectivity. This study points to individual differences in functional connectivity during emotion regulation.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142692783","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introspective psychophysics for the study of subjective experience. 研究主观体验的内省心理物理学。
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-21 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae455
Megan A K Peters

Studying subjective experience is hard. We believe that pain is not identical to nociception, nor pleasure a computational reward signal, nor fear the activation of "threat circuitry". Unfortunately, introspective self-reports offer our best bet for accessing subjective experience, but many still believe that introspection is "unreliable" and "unverifiable". But which of introspection's faults do we find most damning? Is it that introspection provides imperfect access to brain processes (e.g. perception, memory)? That subjective experience is not objectively verifiable? That it is hard to isolate from non-subjective processing capacity? Here, I argue none of these prevents us from building a meaningful, impactful psychophysical research program that treats subjective experience as a valid empirical target through precisely characterizing relationships among environmental variables, brain processes and behavior, and self-reported phenomenology. Following recent similar calls by Peters (Towards characterizing the canonical computations generating phenomenal experience. 2022. Neurosci Biobehav Rev: 142, 104903), Kammerer and Frankish (What forms could introspective systems take? A research programme. 2023. J Conscious Stud 30:13-48), and Fleming (Metacognitive psychophysics in humans, animals, and AI. 2023. J Conscious Stud 30:113-128), "introspective psychophysics" thus treats introspection's apparent faults as features, not bugs-just as the noise and distortions linking environment to behavior inspired Fechner's psychophysics over 150 years ago. This next generation of psychophysics will establish a powerful tool for building and testing precise explanatory models of phenomenology across many dimensions-urgency, emotion, clarity, vividness, confidence, and more.

研究主观体验很难。我们认为,疼痛并不等同于痛觉,快感也不是计算奖励信号,恐惧也不是 "威胁回路 "的激活。不幸的是,内省式自我报告为我们提供了获取主观体验的最佳途径,但许多人仍然认为内省 "不可靠"、"无法验证"。但是,我们认为内省的哪些缺点是最有害的呢?是内省提供的大脑过程(如感知、记忆)并不完美吗?主观经验无法客观验证?主观经验很难与非主观处理能力相分离?在这里,我认为这些都不妨碍我们建立一个有意义、有影响的心理物理学研究计划,通过精确描述环境变量、大脑过程和行为以及自我报告的现象学之间的关系,将主观体验作为一个有效的实证目标。彼得斯(Peters)最近也发出了类似的呼吁(《走向描述产生现象体验的典型计算》)。2022.Neurosci Biobehav Rev: 142, 104903)、卡默勒和弗兰基什(内省系统可以采取什么形式?研究计划。2023.J Conscious Stud 30:13-48)、Fleming(Metacognitive psychophysics in humans, animals, and AI.2023.J Conscious Stud 30:113-128),"内省心理物理学 "因此将内省的明显缺陷视为特征,而非缺陷--就像 150 多年前费希纳的心理物理学受到将环境与行为联系起来的噪音和扭曲的启发一样。下一代心理物理学将为建立和测试现象学的精确解释模型提供强有力的工具,这些模型涉及多个维度--急迫性、情感、清晰度、生动性、自信等等。
{"title":"Introspective psychophysics for the study of subjective experience.","authors":"Megan A K Peters","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae455","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae455","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Studying subjective experience is hard. We believe that pain is not identical to nociception, nor pleasure a computational reward signal, nor fear the activation of \"threat circuitry\". Unfortunately, introspective self-reports offer our best bet for accessing subjective experience, but many still believe that introspection is \"unreliable\" and \"unverifiable\". But which of introspection's faults do we find most damning? Is it that introspection provides imperfect access to brain processes (e.g. perception, memory)? That subjective experience is not objectively verifiable? That it is hard to isolate from non-subjective processing capacity? Here, I argue none of these prevents us from building a meaningful, impactful psychophysical research program that treats subjective experience as a valid empirical target through precisely characterizing relationships among environmental variables, brain processes and behavior, and self-reported phenomenology. Following recent similar calls by Peters (Towards characterizing the canonical computations generating phenomenal experience. 2022. Neurosci Biobehav Rev: 142, 104903), Kammerer and Frankish (What forms could introspective systems take? A research programme. 2023. J Conscious Stud 30:13-48), and Fleming (Metacognitive psychophysics in humans, animals, and AI. 2023. J Conscious Stud 30:113-128), \"introspective psychophysics\" thus treats introspection's apparent faults as features, not bugs-just as the noise and distortions linking environment to behavior inspired Fechner's psychophysics over 150 years ago. This next generation of psychophysics will establish a powerful tool for building and testing precise explanatory models of phenomenology across many dimensions-urgency, emotion, clarity, vividness, confidence, and more.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142680885","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining threat responses through a developmental lens. 从发展的角度审视威胁反应。
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-19 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae449
B J Casey, Yen-Chu Lin, Heidi C Meyer

Adolescence has been characterized by risk taking and fearlessness. Yet, the emergence of anxiety disorders that are associated with fear peaks during this developmental period. Moreover, adolescents show heightened sensitivity to stress relative to children and adults. To address inconsistencies between the common characterization of adolescents as fearless and the evidence of heightened anxiety and stress during this time, we build upon foundational discoveries of threat-related circuitry and behavior in adult rodents by Joseph LeDoux and colleagues. Specifically, the conservation of this circuitry across species has provided opportunities for identifying mechanisms underlying threat responses that we have extended to developing humans and rodents. We elucidate situations in which adolescents show heightened threat responses and others where they appear fearless and link them to developmental changes of threat circuitry during this period. We discuss the potential adaptiveness of these threat responses for survival of the individual and species but also the potential risks for anxiety and stress. We end by offering potential new ways in which behavioral treatments for youth with anxiety and stress-related disorders may be optimized to target the developing vs developed brain.

青春期的特点是敢于冒险和无所畏惧。然而,与恐惧相关的焦虑症却在这一发育阶段达到高峰。此外,与儿童和成人相比,青少年对压力的敏感度更高。为了解决青少年无所畏惧的普遍特征与这一时期焦虑和压力增加的证据之间的矛盾,我们以约瑟夫-勒杜及其同事在成年啮齿动物中发现的威胁相关回路和行为的基础为依据。具体来说,这种回路在不同物种间的一致性为我们提供了识别威胁反应机制的机会,我们已将这种机制推广到发育中的人类和啮齿类动物身上。我们阐明了青少年在哪些情况下会表现出更强的威胁反应,以及在哪些情况下他们显得无所畏惧,并将它们与这一时期威胁回路的发育变化联系起来。我们讨论了这些威胁反应对个体和物种生存的潜在适应性,以及焦虑和压力的潜在风险。最后,我们提出了一些潜在的新方法,以优化针对患有焦虑症和压力相关疾病的青少年的行为治疗,使之针对发育中和发育后的大脑。
{"title":"Examining threat responses through a developmental lens.","authors":"B J Casey, Yen-Chu Lin, Heidi C Meyer","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae449","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae449","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adolescence has been characterized by risk taking and fearlessness. Yet, the emergence of anxiety disorders that are associated with fear peaks during this developmental period. Moreover, adolescents show heightened sensitivity to stress relative to children and adults. To address inconsistencies between the common characterization of adolescents as fearless and the evidence of heightened anxiety and stress during this time, we build upon foundational discoveries of threat-related circuitry and behavior in adult rodents by Joseph LeDoux and colleagues. Specifically, the conservation of this circuitry across species has provided opportunities for identifying mechanisms underlying threat responses that we have extended to developing humans and rodents. We elucidate situations in which adolescents show heightened threat responses and others where they appear fearless and link them to developmental changes of threat circuitry during this period. We discuss the potential adaptiveness of these threat responses for survival of the individual and species but also the potential risks for anxiety and stress. We end by offering potential new ways in which behavioral treatments for youth with anxiety and stress-related disorders may be optimized to target the developing vs developed brain.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142675292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Repetition of critical search features modulates EEG lateralized potentials in visual search. 关键搜索特征的重复会调节视觉搜索的脑电图侧化电位。
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae450
Matt Oxner, Veronica Mazza, Matthias M Müller

In visual search, the repetition of target and distractor colors enables both successful search and effective distractor handling. Nevertheless, the specific consequences of trial-to-trial feature repetition in different search contexts are poorly understood. Here, we investigated how feature repetition shapes the electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of target processing and distractor handling, testing theoretically informed predictions with single-trial mixed-effects modeling. In two experiments, the colors of a fixed-shape target and singleton distractor changed unpredictably across trials. Targets were color singletons in Experiment 1, allowing efficient search among pop-out items, but were not uniquely colored in Experiment 2, encouraging slower shape-feature search. Interference by the distractor occurred only in pop-out search but was reduced by repetition. This was paralleled by the contralateral electroencephalography (EEG) response: Following a search color change, the target-related N2pc was greatly reduced, and salient distractors elicited an N2pc followed by an enhanced PD. This biphasic response was absent in Experiment 2, where color was less useful to search. Overall, distractor positivities were not sensitive to feature repetition, suggesting that they are unrelated to preparatory suppression. Attention-related lateralization components are not universally elicited by target or distractor feature values but are driven specifically by expected features important to the search task.

在视觉搜索中,目标和干扰物颜色的重复既能使搜索成功,又能有效地处理干扰物。然而,人们对在不同搜索情境中试验到试验特征重复的具体后果还知之甚少。在这里,我们研究了特征重复如何影响目标处理和分心物处理的电生理和行为相关性,并通过单次试验混合效应建模检验了理论上的预测。在两个实验中,固定形状的目标和单个分心物的颜色在不同试验中发生了不可预测的变化。在实验 1 中,目标物的颜色是单一的,因此可以在弹出项目中进行有效搜索;但在实验 2 中,目标物的颜色并不是唯一的,因此鼓励进行较慢的形状特征搜索。干扰物的干扰只发生在弹出搜索中,但通过重复可以减少。这与对侧脑电图(EEG)的反应一致:在搜索颜色发生变化后,与目标相关的 N2pc 会大大降低,而突出的分心物会引起 N2pc,随后 PD 会增强。这种双相反应在实验 2 中并不存在,因为颜色对搜索的作用较小。总的来说,分心积极性对特征重复并不敏感,这表明它们与准备性抑制无关。与注意相关的侧化成分并非普遍由目标或分心特征值引起,而是特别由对搜索任务重要的预期特征驱动。
{"title":"Repetition of critical search features modulates EEG lateralized potentials in visual search.","authors":"Matt Oxner, Veronica Mazza, Matthias M Müller","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae450","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae450","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In visual search, the repetition of target and distractor colors enables both successful search and effective distractor handling. Nevertheless, the specific consequences of trial-to-trial feature repetition in different search contexts are poorly understood. Here, we investigated how feature repetition shapes the electrophysiological and behavioral correlates of target processing and distractor handling, testing theoretically informed predictions with single-trial mixed-effects modeling. In two experiments, the colors of a fixed-shape target and singleton distractor changed unpredictably across trials. Targets were color singletons in Experiment 1, allowing efficient search among pop-out items, but were not uniquely colored in Experiment 2, encouraging slower shape-feature search. Interference by the distractor occurred only in pop-out search but was reduced by repetition. This was paralleled by the contralateral electroencephalography (EEG) response: Following a search color change, the target-related N2pc was greatly reduced, and salient distractors elicited an N2pc followed by an enhanced PD. This biphasic response was absent in Experiment 2, where color was less useful to search. Overall, distractor positivities were not sensitive to feature repetition, suggesting that they are unrelated to preparatory suppression. Attention-related lateralization components are not universally elicited by target or distractor feature values but are driven specifically by expected features important to the search task.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":"34 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142680888","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Synesthesia is linked to large and extensive differences in brain structure and function as determined by whole-brain biomarkers derived from the HCP (Human Connectome Project) cortical parcellation approach. 根据 HCP(人类连接组计划)皮层划分方法得出的全脑生物标记确定,联觉与大脑结构和功能的巨大和广泛差异有关。
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae446
Jamie Ward, Julia Simner, Ivor Simpson, Charlotte Rae, Magda Del Rio, Jessica A Eccles, Chris Racey

There is considerable interest in understanding the developmental origins and health implications of individual differences in brain structure and function. In this pre-registered study we demonstrate that a hidden subgroup within the general population-people with synesthesia (e.g. who "hear" colors)-show a distinctive behavioral phenotype and wide-ranging differences in brain structure and function. We assess the performance of 13 different brain-based biomarkers (structural and functional MRI) for classifying synesthetes against general population samples, using machine learning models. The features in these models were derived from subject-specific parcellations of the cortex using the Human Connectome Project approach. All biomarkers performed above chance with intracortical myelin being a particularly strong predictor that has not been implicated in synesthesia before. Resting state data show widespread changes in the functional connectome (including less hub-based connectivity). These brain-based individual differences within the neurotypical population can be as large as those that differentiate neurotypical from clinical brain states.

人们对了解大脑结构和功能的个体差异的发展起源和对健康的影响非常感兴趣。在这项预先登记的研究中,我们证明了普通人群中的一个隐性亚群--联觉患者(如 "听到 "颜色的人)--表现出独特的行为表型以及大脑结构和功能的广泛差异。我们利用机器学习模型评估了 13 种不同的脑部生物标记物(结构和功能磁共振成像)的性能,以对照普通人群样本对突触症患者进行分类。这些模型中的特征是利用人类连接组计划方法从特定对象的皮层细分中得出的。所有生物标志物的表现都超出了正常范围,其中皮层内髓鞘是一个特别强的预测因子,以前从未与突触相关联。静息状态数据显示了功能连接组的广泛变化(包括较少的基于枢纽的连接)。这些基于大脑的个体差异在神经畸形人群中可能与区分神经畸形和临床大脑状态的差异一样大。
{"title":"Synesthesia is linked to large and extensive differences in brain structure and function as determined by whole-brain biomarkers derived from the HCP (Human Connectome Project) cortical parcellation approach.","authors":"Jamie Ward, Julia Simner, Ivor Simpson, Charlotte Rae, Magda Del Rio, Jessica A Eccles, Chris Racey","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae446","DOIUrl":"10.1093/cercor/bhae446","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is considerable interest in understanding the developmental origins and health implications of individual differences in brain structure and function. In this pre-registered study we demonstrate that a hidden subgroup within the general population-people with synesthesia (e.g. who \"hear\" colors)-show a distinctive behavioral phenotype and wide-ranging differences in brain structure and function. We assess the performance of 13 different brain-based biomarkers (structural and functional MRI) for classifying synesthetes against general population samples, using machine learning models. The features in these models were derived from subject-specific parcellations of the cortex using the Human Connectome Project approach. All biomarkers performed above chance with intracortical myelin being a particularly strong predictor that has not been implicated in synesthesia before. Resting state data show widespread changes in the functional connectome (including less hub-based connectivity). These brain-based individual differences within the neurotypical population can be as large as those that differentiate neurotypical from clinical brain states.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":"34 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11567774/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142638485","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to: Partisans process policy-based and identity-based messages using dissociable neural systems. 更正为游击队员利用可分离的神经系统处理基于政策和基于身份的信息。
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae459
{"title":"Correction to: Partisans process policy-based and identity-based messages using dissociable neural systems.","authors":"","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae459","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae459","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":"34 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142680887","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural signatures of visual awareness independent of postperceptual processing. 视觉意识的神经特征与感知后处理无关
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae415
Michael A Cohen, Cole Dembski, Kevin Ortego, Clay Steinhibler, Michael Pitts

What are the neural processes associated with perceptual awareness that are distinct from preconscious sensory encoding and postperceptual processes such as reporting an experience? Using electroencephalography and a no-report visual masking paradigm, we manipulated stimulus visibility by varying the time between stimuli and masks in linear steps (17, 33, 50, 67, and 83 ms). Awareness increased nonlinearly, with stimuli never seen at the two shortest intervals, always seen at the two longest, and 50% seen at the intermediate interval. Separate report and no-report conditions were used to isolate awareness from task performance. Our results revealed a neural signal closely linked to perceptual awareness, independent of the task: a fronto-central event-related potential that we refer to as the N2 (~250 to 300 ms). Earlier event-related potential signals reflected the linear manipulation of stimulus strength, while later signals like P3b and temporal generalization of decoding were tied to task performance, appearing only in the report condition. Taken together, these findings inform current debates regarding theories of consciousness and offer new avenues for exploring the neural mechanisms supporting conscious processing.

与知觉意识相关的神经过程有哪些?这些过程有别于前意识感觉编码和知觉后过程(如报告体验)吗?我们使用脑电图和无报告视觉遮蔽范式,通过改变刺激和遮蔽之间的时间,以线性步骤(17、33、50、67 和 83 毫秒)来操纵刺激的可见性。认知度呈非线性增长,在两个最短的时间间隔内从未看到过刺激物,在两个最长的时间间隔内总是看到刺激物,在中间时间间隔内看到 50%的刺激物。为了将意识与任务表现分离开来,我们使用了单独的报告和无报告条件。我们的结果揭示了一个与知觉意识密切相关的神经信号,它与任务无关:一个前中央事件相关电位,我们称之为 N2(约 250 到 300 毫秒)。较早的事件相关电位信号反映了刺激强度的线性操作,而较晚的信号如 P3b 和解码的时间泛化则与任务表现有关,只出现在报告条件下。综上所述,这些发现为当前有关意识理论的争论提供了信息,并为探索支持意识处理的神经机制提供了新的途径。
{"title":"Neural signatures of visual awareness independent of postperceptual processing.","authors":"Michael A Cohen, Cole Dembski, Kevin Ortego, Clay Steinhibler, Michael Pitts","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae415","DOIUrl":"10.1093/cercor/bhae415","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>What are the neural processes associated with perceptual awareness that are distinct from preconscious sensory encoding and postperceptual processes such as reporting an experience? Using electroencephalography and a no-report visual masking paradigm, we manipulated stimulus visibility by varying the time between stimuli and masks in linear steps (17, 33, 50, 67, and 83 ms). Awareness increased nonlinearly, with stimuli never seen at the two shortest intervals, always seen at the two longest, and 50% seen at the intermediate interval. Separate report and no-report conditions were used to isolate awareness from task performance. Our results revealed a neural signal closely linked to perceptual awareness, independent of the task: a fronto-central event-related potential that we refer to as the N2 (~250 to 300 ms). Earlier event-related potential signals reflected the linear manipulation of stimulus strength, while later signals like P3b and temporal generalization of decoding were tied to task performance, appearing only in the report condition. Taken together, these findings inform current debates regarding theories of consciousness and offer new avenues for exploring the neural mechanisms supporting conscious processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":"34 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11558846/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142615460","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental encoding of natural sounds in the mouse auditory cortex. 小鼠听觉皮层对自然声音的发育编码
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae438
Stefano Zucca, Chiara La Rosa, Tommaso Fellin, Paolo Peretto, Serena Bovetti

Mice communicate through high-frequency ultrasonic vocalizations, which are crucial for social interactions such as courtship and aggression. Although ultrasonic vocalization representation has been found in adult brain areas along the auditory pathway, including the auditory cortex, no evidence is available on the neuronal representation of ultrasonic vocalizations early in life. Using in vivo two-photon calcium imaging, we analyzed auditory cortex layer 2/3 neuronal responses to USVs, pure tones (4 to 90 kHz), and high-frequency modulated sweeps from postnatal day 12 (P12) to P21. We found that ACx neurons are tuned to respond to ultrasonic vocalization syllables as early as P12 to P13, with an increasing number of responsive cells as the mouse age. By P14, while pure tone responses showed a frequency preference, no syllable preference was observed. Additionally, at P14, USVs, pure tones, and modulated sweeps activate clusters of largely nonoverlapping responsive neurons. Finally, we show that while cell correlation decreases with increasing processing of peripheral auditory stimuli, neurons responding to the same stimulus maintain highly correlated spontaneous activity after circuits have attained mature organization, forming neuronal subnetworks sharing similar functional properties.

小鼠通过高频超声波发声进行交流,这对求偶和攻击等社会互动至关重要。虽然在听觉通路的成年脑区(包括听觉皮层)发现了超声波发声表征,但没有证据表明超声波发声在生命早期的神经元表征。利用体内双光子钙成像技术,我们分析了听觉皮层第 2/3 层神经元对 USV、纯音(4 至 90 kHz)和高频调制扫频的反应。我们发现,ACx神经元早在出生后第12天至第13天就能对超声发声音节做出反应,随着小鼠年龄的增长,反应细胞的数量也在增加。到 P14 时,虽然纯音反应显示出频率偏好,但没有观察到音节偏好。此外,在 P14 时,USV、纯音和调制扫频激活的反应神经元群基本不重叠。最后,我们发现,虽然细胞相关性会随着外周听觉刺激处理的增加而降低,但对相同刺激做出反应的神经元在电路组织成熟后仍会保持高度相关的自发活动,形成具有相似功能特性的神经元子网络。
{"title":"Developmental encoding of natural sounds in the mouse auditory cortex.","authors":"Stefano Zucca, Chiara La Rosa, Tommaso Fellin, Paolo Peretto, Serena Bovetti","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae438","DOIUrl":"10.1093/cercor/bhae438","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mice communicate through high-frequency ultrasonic vocalizations, which are crucial for social interactions such as courtship and aggression. Although ultrasonic vocalization representation has been found in adult brain areas along the auditory pathway, including the auditory cortex, no evidence is available on the neuronal representation of ultrasonic vocalizations early in life. Using in vivo two-photon calcium imaging, we analyzed auditory cortex layer 2/3 neuronal responses to USVs, pure tones (4 to 90 kHz), and high-frequency modulated sweeps from postnatal day 12 (P12) to P21. We found that ACx neurons are tuned to respond to ultrasonic vocalization syllables as early as P12 to P13, with an increasing number of responsive cells as the mouse age. By P14, while pure tone responses showed a frequency preference, no syllable preference was observed. Additionally, at P14, USVs, pure tones, and modulated sweeps activate clusters of largely nonoverlapping responsive neurons. Finally, we show that while cell correlation decreases with increasing processing of peripheral auditory stimuli, neurons responding to the same stimulus maintain highly correlated spontaneous activity after circuits have attained mature organization, forming neuronal subnetworks sharing similar functional properties.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":"34 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11538960/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142582200","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of left ventrolateral prefrontal stimulation on forming and maintaining deep and shallow episodic traces. 刺激左侧前额叶外侧对形成和维持深浅情节痕迹的影响
IF 2.9 2区 医学 Q2 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhae437
Nan Peng, Wei Wu, Sergio Della Sala, Paul Hoffman

The levels-of-processing framework, proposing that deep encoding enhances retention, plays a crucial role in episodic memory research. Neuroimaging evidence highlights that increased activity of the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during deep encoding predicts subsequent memory success. However, cognitive mechanisms underlying this region's involvement in establishing and consolidating deep and shallow traces remain unclear. In this preregistered study, we investigated whether repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex versus the vertex differentially modulates the formation and maintenance of deep and shallow traces. Trains of 20 Hz online repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation were delivered over the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex or vertex during tasks involving pleasantness (deep) and alphabetical order (shallow) judgments of words. Following encoding, two recognition tests assessed immediate and 24-h delayed recognition of words. Compared to the vertex control, ventrolateral prefrontal stimulation selectively disrupted the formation of episodic memory under deep encoding conditions, evidenced by increased response time at encoding and reduced immediate recognition in the deep but not shallow condition. Notably, forgetting rates across the 24-h delay were similar for disrupted deep, intact deep, and shallow items, implying that the rate of trace decay is independent of the strength of trace formation. The constant trace decay indicates that distinct mechanisms are involved in establishing and maintaining episodic traces.

处理水平框架提出深度编码能增强记忆的保持,这一框架在外显记忆研究中起着至关重要的作用。神经影像学证据表明,在深度编码过程中,左侧腹外侧前额叶皮层活动的增加预示着随后记忆的成功。然而,该区域参与建立和巩固深浅痕迹的认知机制仍不清楚。在这项预先登记的研究中,我们调查了重复经颅磁刺激左侧腹外侧前额叶皮层与顶点是否会对深浅痕迹的形成和维持产生不同的调节作用。在涉及单词愉快度(深层)和字母顺序(浅层)判断的任务中,对左侧腹外侧前额叶皮层或顶点进行一连串 20 赫兹的在线重复经颅磁刺激。编码后,两个识别测试分别评估了对单词的即时识别和 24 小时延迟识别。与顶点对照组相比,在深度编码条件下,前额叶外侧刺激选择性地破坏了外显记忆的形成,表现为编码时反应时间增加,以及在深度条件下即时识别率降低,而在浅度条件下则没有。值得注意的是,在24小时的延迟中,被破坏的深层、完整的深层和浅层项目的遗忘率相似,这意味着痕迹衰减的速度与痕迹形成的强度无关。恒定的痕迹衰减表明,在建立和维持情节性痕迹的过程中,有不同的机制参与其中。
{"title":"Effects of left ventrolateral prefrontal stimulation on forming and maintaining deep and shallow episodic traces.","authors":"Nan Peng, Wei Wu, Sergio Della Sala, Paul Hoffman","doi":"10.1093/cercor/bhae437","DOIUrl":"10.1093/cercor/bhae437","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The levels-of-processing framework, proposing that deep encoding enhances retention, plays a crucial role in episodic memory research. Neuroimaging evidence highlights that increased activity of the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during deep encoding predicts subsequent memory success. However, cognitive mechanisms underlying this region's involvement in establishing and consolidating deep and shallow traces remain unclear. In this preregistered study, we investigated whether repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex versus the vertex differentially modulates the formation and maintenance of deep and shallow traces. Trains of 20 Hz online repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation were delivered over the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex or vertex during tasks involving pleasantness (deep) and alphabetical order (shallow) judgments of words. Following encoding, two recognition tests assessed immediate and 24-h delayed recognition of words. Compared to the vertex control, ventrolateral prefrontal stimulation selectively disrupted the formation of episodic memory under deep encoding conditions, evidenced by increased response time at encoding and reduced immediate recognition in the deep but not shallow condition. Notably, forgetting rates across the 24-h delay were similar for disrupted deep, intact deep, and shallow items, implying that the rate of trace decay is independent of the strength of trace formation. The constant trace decay indicates that distinct mechanisms are involved in establishing and maintaining episodic traces.</p>","PeriodicalId":9715,"journal":{"name":"Cerebral cortex","volume":"34 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11538959/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142582202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cerebral cortex
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1