首页 > 最新文献

Applied Animal Ethology最新文献

英文 中文
The behaviour of beef suckler cattle (Bos taurus) 肉用乳牛(Bos taurus)的行为
Pub Date : 1984-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(84)90036-1
Y. Ruckebusch
{"title":"The behaviour of beef suckler cattle (Bos taurus)","authors":"Y. Ruckebusch","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(84)90036-1","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(84)90036-1","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(84)90036-1","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86927733","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Changes in activity, spatial pattern and social behavior in calves after grouping 小牛犊分组后活动、空间格局和社会行为的变化
Pub Date : 1984-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(84)90028-2
Seiji Kondo, Noboru Kawakami, Hideaki Kohama, Susumu Nishino

The behavior of 2 groups of 6 calves that were managed in 2 different ways were observed, and the development of their behavioral patterns, spatial patterns and social behavior in the groups were discussed, together with the effect on these processes of their social experience before grouping.

Twelve 5-month-old Holstein steers, reared alone from one week of age, were divided into 2 groups of 6 calves each; Groups A and B. Calves in Group A were penned together in a 14.4 × 28.8 m pen, and the position and behavior of individuals was observed throughout a continuous 153-h period at 15-min intervals. Aggressive behavior between calves was also recorded. The position of calves was recorded as coordinates on a grid on the pen floor. There were 8 × 16 squares in the grid, and each square measured 1.8 × 1.8 m.

Group B was further divided into 3 groups of 2 calves each, which were kept in 9.6 × 14.4 m pens. The pair-mates were changed every 3 days in order to combine all possible pairs in the group. After this treatment, calves were all housed together in a 14.4 × 28.8 m pen, and observed using the same methods and for the same duration as Group A.

The lying pattern of Group A was not diurnal until the latter half of the observation, while calves in Group B showed clear-cut diurnal variations from the initial stages. Concerning the spatial patterns; the mean distance to nearest neighbor in Group A tended to decrease and become stable. The ratio between the mean distance to nearest neighbor and the distance expected in random distribution also declined from 0.7 to a significant aggregation of 0.5 (P < 0.05). For Group B, a similar change in the mean distance to the nearest neighbor was observed, although this showed significant aggregation from the first 24-h period (P < 0.05). The mean area occupied by a group in each 24-h period, for both groups, declined for 72 h after grouping and became stable at around 18 m2, for both groups, thereafter. The frequency of aggression in Group A declined from 23 times in the first 24-h period to 9 times at the end of the observation period. For Group B, the aggression frequency was 53 times in the first 24-h period, thereafter decreasing rapidly.

通过对两组6头不同管理方式的小牛的行为进行观察,探讨了其行为模式、空间模式和群体内社会行为的发展,以及群体前社会经验对这些过程的影响。选取12头5月龄荷斯坦阉牛,自1周龄起单独饲养,分为2组,每组6头;A组和b组分别圈养在14.4 × 28.8 m的圈内,每隔15 min观察个体的位置和行为,连续观察153 h。小牛之间的攻击行为也被记录下来。小牛的位置以坐标记录在围栏地板上的网格上。网格中有8 × 16个正方形,每个正方形的尺寸为1.8 × 1.8 m。B组再分为3组,每组2头,饲养在9.6 × 14.4 m的圈内。每3天更换一次配对,以便组合组中所有可能的配对。试验结束后,将所有犊牛集中饲养在14.4 × 28.8 m的围栏内,采用与a组相同的方法和时间进行观察。观察后半段,a组犊牛的躺卧模式为非日卧模式,而B组犊牛的躺卧模式从一开始就有明显的日卧变化。关于空间格局;A组与最近邻的平均距离趋于减小并趋于稳定。平均到最近邻居的距离与随机分布中期望距离的比值也从0.7下降到显著的0.5 (P <0.05)。对于B组,观察到与最近邻居的平均距离也有类似的变化,尽管这表明从第一个24小时开始就有显著的聚集(P <0.05)。两组每24 h平均占地面积在分组后72 h下降,此后稳定在18 m2左右。A组攻击次数由观察开始24 h时的23次下降到观察结束时的9次。B组攻击次数在前24 h为53次,之后迅速下降。
{"title":"Changes in activity, spatial pattern and social behavior in calves after grouping","authors":"Seiji Kondo,&nbsp;Noboru Kawakami,&nbsp;Hideaki Kohama,&nbsp;Susumu Nishino","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(84)90028-2","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(84)90028-2","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The behavior of 2 groups of 6 calves that were managed in 2 different ways were observed, and the development of their behavioral patterns, spatial patterns and social behavior in the groups were discussed, together with the effect on these processes of their social experience before grouping.</p><p>Twelve 5-month-old Holstein steers, reared alone from one week of age, were divided into 2 groups of 6 calves each; Groups A and B. Calves in Group A were penned together in a 14.4 × 28.8 m pen, and the position and behavior of individuals was observed throughout a continuous 153-h period at 15-min intervals. Aggressive behavior between calves was also recorded. The position of calves was recorded as coordinates on a grid on the pen floor. There were 8 × 16 squares in the grid, and each square measured 1.8 × 1.8 m.</p><p>Group B was further divided into 3 groups of 2 calves each, which were kept in 9.6 × 14.4 m pens. The pair-mates were changed every 3 days in order to combine all possible pairs in the group. After this treatment, calves were all housed together in a 14.4 × 28.8 m pen, and observed using the same methods and for the same duration as Group A.</p><p>The lying pattern of Group A was not diurnal until the latter half of the observation, while calves in Group B showed clear-cut diurnal variations from the initial stages. Concerning the spatial patterns; the mean distance to nearest neighbor in Group A tended to decrease and become stable. The ratio between the mean distance to nearest neighbor and the distance expected in random distribution also declined from 0.7 to a significant aggregation of 0.5 (<em>P</em> &lt; 0.05). For Group B, a similar change in the mean distance to the nearest neighbor was observed, although this showed significant aggregation from the first 24-h period (<em>P</em> &lt; 0.05). The mean area occupied by a group in each 24-h period, for both groups, declined for 72 h after grouping and became stable at around 18 m<sup>2</sup>, for both groups, thereafter. The frequency of aggression in Group A declined from 23 times in the first 24-h period to 9 times at the end of the observation period. For Group B, the aggression frequency was 53 times in the first 24-h period, thereafter decreasing rapidly.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1984-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(84)90028-2","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"91548661","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 46
Fowl sociometry: Social discrimination and the behaviour of domestic hens during food competition 家禽社会计量学:社会歧视与家母鸡在食物竞争中的行为
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90124-4
G.J. Syme , L.A. Syme , D.R. Barnes

A stable food competition order was established in a flock of 8 hens. Each hen was then provided with a choice of competitor by allowing it to choose between 2 of its group-mates in 10 consecutive competitive trials. All hens were given a choice of every possible pairing of the remaining 7 birds in the flock. Many statistically significant social discriminations were recorded. Although there was a low correlation between the total number of choices for each bird and competitive rank, there was a significant tendency for birds to avoid higher-ranking hens when individual “dominant-subordinate” choices were analysed. Subjects could also assess relative rank rather than merely distinguishing between birds more or less dominant than themselves. Implications of these findings for the interpretation of competitive orders in domestic fowl are discussed.

在一群8只母鸡中建立了稳定的食物竞争秩序。然后,每只母鸡可以选择竞争对手,在连续10次的竞争试验中,允许它从2个同伴中选择。所有的母鸡都可以选择鸡群中剩下的7只鸟中的每一对。记录了许多具有统计意义的社会歧视。虽然每只鸡的选择总数与竞争等级之间的相关性不高,但当分析个体“支配-从属”选择时,鸟类有明显的倾向避免更高等级的母鸡。受试者还可以评估相对等级,而不仅仅是区分比自己更占优势或更弱势的鸟类。本文讨论了这些发现对家禽竞争顺序解释的意义。
{"title":"Fowl sociometry: Social discrimination and the behaviour of domestic hens during food competition","authors":"G.J. Syme ,&nbsp;L.A. Syme ,&nbsp;D.R. Barnes","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90124-4","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90124-4","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A stable food competition order was established in a flock of 8 hens. Each hen was then provided with a choice of competitor by allowing it to choose between 2 of its group-mates in 10 consecutive competitive trials. All hens were given a choice of every possible pairing of the remaining 7 birds in the flock. Many statistically significant social discriminations were recorded. Although there was a low correlation between the total number of choices for each bird and competitive rank, there was a significant tendency for birds to avoid higher-ranking hens when individual “dominant-subordinate” choices were analysed. Subjects could also assess relative rank rather than merely distinguishing between birds more or less dominant than themselves. Implications of these findings for the interpretation of competitive orders in domestic fowl are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90124-4","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87109621","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Escape-reaction of captive young red-legged partridges (Alectoris rufa) reared with or without visual contact with man 圈养幼红腿鹧鸪(Alectoris rufa)的逃跑反应,饲养时是否与人有视觉接触
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90125-6
Davide Csermely , Danilo Mainardi , Silvio Spanò

It was noted that adult birds of Alectoris rufa, hatched and reared in captivity, cannot survive in nature after their release for restocking for hunting. An attempt was made to ascertain what effect various kinds of visual experiences with man during the first 2 months of life might have on the escape-response of young individuals.

It was observed that absence of visual contact with man during the 48 h after hatching causes fear of human beings in those individuals when 2 months old. Conversely, subjects which were in visual contact with man during the 48 h after hatching did not show this tendency to escape in front of him.

The possible links between this finding and the phenomenon of imprinting, together with the need for new rearing techniques, are discussed.

有人指出,在圈养环境中孵化和饲养的成年鹿角鸟在被放归饲养用于狩猎后,无法在自然界生存。我们试图确定在生命的头2个月里,各种与人的视觉体验对年轻个体的逃跑反应有什么影响。我们观察到,在孵化后的48小时内没有与人的视觉接触,导致这些个体在2个月大时对人类产生恐惧。相反,在孵化后48小时内与人类有视觉接触的实验对象没有表现出在人类面前逃跑的倾向。讨论了这一发现与印记现象之间的可能联系,以及对新饲养技术的需要。
{"title":"Escape-reaction of captive young red-legged partridges (Alectoris rufa) reared with or without visual contact with man","authors":"Davide Csermely ,&nbsp;Danilo Mainardi ,&nbsp;Silvio Spanò","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90125-6","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90125-6","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>It was noted that adult birds of <em>Alectoris rufa</em>, hatched and reared in captivity, cannot survive in nature after their release for restocking for hunting. An attempt was made to ascertain what effect various kinds of visual experiences with man during the first 2 months of life might have on the escape-response of young individuals.</p><p>It was observed that absence of visual contact with man during the 48 h after hatching causes fear of human beings in those individuals when 2 months old. Conversely, subjects which were in visual contact with man during the 48 h after hatching did not show this tendency to escape in front of him.</p><p>The possible links between this finding and the phenomenon of imprinting, together with the need for new rearing techniques, are discussed.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90125-6","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73703021","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 28
Ingestion of earthworms, Pontoscolex corethrurus, by village pigs, Sus scrofa papuensis, in the highlands of Papua New Guinea 巴布亚新几内亚高地,村猪Sus scrofa papuensis摄食蚯蚓pontocolex corethrurus
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90122-0
C.J. Rose , W.T. Williams

In a short (28-day) experiment, weanling village pigs, Sus scrofa papuensis, gained weight at the rate of 147 g daily when foraging on tethers in harvested sweet potato gardens, Impomoea batatas (L) Lam., and fed additional raw sweet potato tubers. A comparable group of pigs that had no access to soil lost weight and exhibited signs of protein malnourishment. The highly significant difference in growth rate was thought to be due to the foraging pigs consuming earthworms, Pontoscolex corethrurus, which were abundant in the soil.

Ten foraging pigs were closely observed over a period of 3 months. Daily weight gains increased to 190 g, and it was recorded that each pig ate between 414 and 1224 earthworms daily (half the daily amount being eaten in the first hour of foraging), and that this activity was predominant. As the day progressed, the pigs spent more time lying down, standing doing nothing, or eating green vegetation. These observations are discussed in the context of local pig husbandry.

在一项短期(28天)试验中,断奶村猪(Sus scrofa papuensis)在收获的甘薯园(imomoea batatas (L) Lam)的系绳上觅食时,以每天147克的速度增重。喂食额外的生红薯块茎。另一组没有接触土壤的猪体重下降,并表现出蛋白质营养不良的迹象。生长速度的显著差异被认为是由于觅食猪食用土壤中丰富的蚯蚓(pontocolex corethrurus)。对10头采食猪进行了为期3个月的密切观察。每天体重增加到190克,记录显示,每头猪每天吃414到1224条蚯蚓(每天觅食的第一个小时吃掉的蚯蚓量的一半),并且这种活动占主导地位。随着时间的推移,猪花更多的时间躺着,站着什么也不做,或者吃绿色的植物。这些观察结果在当地养猪业的背景下进行了讨论。
{"title":"Ingestion of earthworms, Pontoscolex corethrurus, by village pigs, Sus scrofa papuensis, in the highlands of Papua New Guinea","authors":"C.J. Rose ,&nbsp;W.T. Williams","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90122-0","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90122-0","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>In a short (28-day) experiment, weanling village pigs, <em>Sus scrofa papuensis</em>, gained weight at the rate of 147 g daily when foraging on tethers in harvested sweet potato gardens, <em>Impomoea batatas</em> (L) Lam., and fed additional raw sweet potato tubers. A comparable group of pigs that had no access to soil lost weight and exhibited signs of protein malnourishment. The highly significant difference in growth rate was thought to be due to the foraging pigs consuming earthworms, <em>Pontoscolex corethrurus</em>, which were abundant in the soil.</p><p>Ten foraging pigs were closely observed over a period of 3 months. Daily weight gains increased to 190 g, and it was recorded that each pig ate between 414 and 1224 earthworms daily (half the daily amount being eaten in the first hour of foraging), and that this activity was predominant. As the day progressed, the pigs spent more time lying down, standing doing nothing, or eating green vegetation. These observations are discussed in the context of local pig husbandry.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90122-0","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79710010","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
The buller syndrome in American bison bulls 美洲野牛的公牛综合症
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90126-8
Dale F. Lott

Homosexual mounting (buller syndrome) was observed incidentally during approximately 3900 h of field observations of 2 populations of American bison (Bison bison) in the course of an 11-year study of social and reproductive behaviour. Buller behaviour was regularly observed in intact, free-ranging males up to the age of 4 years. This suggests that some level of this behaviour, which is an economically significant problem in cattle husbandry, may be a natural trait of feed-lot steers.

在一项为期11年的社会和生殖行为研究过程中,对2个美洲野牛种群进行了大约3900小时的实地观察,偶然发现了同性恋增加(buller综合征)。在完整的、自由放养的雄性中,直到4岁,经常观察到公牛的行为。这表明,这种行为在某种程度上可能是饲养场牧牛者的一种自然特征,而这种行为在养牛业中是一个经济上的重大问题。
{"title":"The buller syndrome in American bison bulls","authors":"Dale F. Lott","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90126-8","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90126-8","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Homosexual mounting (buller syndrome) was observed incidentally during approximately 3900 h of field observations of 2 populations of American bison (<em>Bison bison</em>) in the course of an 11-year study of social and reproductive behaviour. Buller behaviour was regularly observed in intact, free-ranging males up to the age of 4 years. This suggests that some level of this behaviour, which is an economically significant problem in cattle husbandry, may be a natural trait of feed-lot steers.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90126-8","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78840213","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Floor-laying by domestic hens 家母鸡下蛋
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90134-7
M.C. Appleby, H.E. McRae
{"title":"Floor-laying by domestic hens","authors":"M.C. Appleby,&nbsp;H.E. McRae","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90134-7","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90134-7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90134-7","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75973888","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Homosexual behavior in feedlot steers: An aggression hypothesis 饲养场的同性恋行为:一种攻击假说
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90127-X
W.R. Klemm , C.J. Sherry , L.M. Schake , R.F. Sis

A common male homosexual vice in feedlot cattle is an activity where some steers, “bullers”, sexually attract other steers, “riders”. One theory of the cause is that bullers are excessively feminized and give off sexually stimulating pheromones. Because pheromones are detected in many mammals by the vomeronasal organ (VNO), we tested the role of this system by cauterizing duct openings which lead to the VNO. Cautery of non-bullers did statistically reduce bulling, but the magnitude of bulling was still high. Cautery of bullers revealed that they were indeed subjected to less mounting than were the non-cauterized bullers in the same herd.

Bullers commonly participated in mounting and had aggression scores about twice that of riders. We believe that aggresssion is a key element in bulling behavior because: (1) most (over 80%) of the steers withdrew from the social contesting engaged in by both riders and bullers; (2) riders were challenged by bullers, in that bullers also mounted riders, as well as other bullers; (3) bullers were more aggressive than riders in the conventional measures of aggression; and (4) the amount of bulling was much greater during the periods of greatest social stress (such as just after the herds were formed and, in one pen, just after 50 new steers were added to the pen). Thus, we are led to advance a new hypothesis about bulling; namely, that it is a ritualized “game” based on social hierarchy contesting.

饲养场中常见的男性同性恋恶习是一些阉牛(bulllers)性吸引其他阉牛(riders)的行为。一种理论认为这是因为雄犬过度雌性化,并释放出性刺激信息素。由于许多哺乳动物通过犁鼻器官(VNO)检测信息素,我们通过烧灼通向VNO的导管开口来测试该系统的作用。烧灼非牛头确实在统计学上减少了牛头,但牛头的程度仍然很高。对公牛的灼烧表明,在同一群公牛中,他们确实比未灼烧的公牛受到更少的骑乘。Bullers通常参与骑乘,其攻击性得分约为骑手的两倍。我们认为攻击性是霸凌行为的一个关键因素,因为:(1)大多数(超过80%)的公牛退出了骑手和霸凌者参与的社会竞争;(2)骑手受到斗牛士的挑战,因为斗牛士也骑着骑手,也骑着其他的斗牛士;(3)在传统的攻击措施中,斗牛士比骑手更具攻击性;(4)在社会压力最大的时期(比如刚形成畜群之后,在一个猪圈里,刚加入50头新牛之后),公牛的数量要大得多。因此,我们提出了一个关于欺凌的新假设;也就是说,它是一种基于社会等级竞争的仪式化“游戏”。
{"title":"Homosexual behavior in feedlot steers: An aggression hypothesis","authors":"W.R. Klemm ,&nbsp;C.J. Sherry ,&nbsp;L.M. Schake ,&nbsp;R.F. Sis","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90127-X","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90127-X","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>A common male homosexual vice in feedlot cattle is an activity where some steers, “bullers”, sexually attract other steers, “riders”. One theory of the cause is that bullers are excessively feminized and give off sexually stimulating pheromones. Because pheromones are detected in many mammals by the vomeronasal organ (VNO), we tested the role of this system by cauterizing duct openings which lead to the VNO. Cautery of non-bullers did statistically reduce bulling, but the magnitude of bulling was still high. Cautery of bullers revealed that they were indeed subjected to less mounting than were the non-cauterized bullers in the same herd.</p><p>Bullers commonly participated in mounting and had aggression scores about twice that of riders. We believe that aggresssion is a key element in bulling behavior because: (1) most (over 80%) of the steers withdrew from the social contesting engaged in by both riders and bullers; (2) riders were challenged by bullers, in that bullers also mounted riders, as well as other bullers; (3) bullers were more aggressive than riders in the conventional measures of aggression; and (4) the amount of bulling was much greater during the periods of greatest social stress (such as just after the herds were formed and, in one pen, just after 50 new steers were added to the pen). Thus, we are led to advance a new hypothesis about bulling; namely, that it is a ritualized “game” based on social hierarchy contesting.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90127-X","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73955237","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 30
Spatial analysis of grazing animals: A critique of some methods 放牧动物的空间分析:对一些方法的批判
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90130-X
J.C. Eddison, D.G.M. Wood-Gush, R.I. Muetzelfeldt
{"title":"Spatial analysis of grazing animals: A critique of some methods","authors":"J.C. Eddison,&nbsp;D.G.M. Wood-Gush,&nbsp;R.I. Muetzelfeldt","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90130-X","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90130-X","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90130-X","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"105341885","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A practising veterinary surgeon's view of ethology 兽医师对动物行为学的看法
Pub Date : 1983-11-01 DOI: 10.1016/0304-3762(83)90135-9
P.B. Clarke
{"title":"A practising veterinary surgeon's view of ethology","authors":"P.B. Clarke","doi":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90135-9","DOIUrl":"10.1016/0304-3762(83)90135-9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":100106,"journal":{"name":"Applied Animal Ethology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1983-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90135-9","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82210501","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Applied Animal Ethology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1