首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Neuroscience最新文献

英文 中文
Is implicit memory associated with the hippocampus? 内隐记忆与海马体有关吗?
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-18 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2024.2315816
Ashley C Steinkrauss, Scott D Slotnick

According to the traditional memory-systems view, the hippocampus is critical during explicit (conscious) long-term memory, whereas other brain regions support implicit (nonconscious) memory. In the last two decades, some fMRI studies have reported hippocampal activity during implicit memory tasks. The aim of the present discussion paper was to identify whether any implicit memory fMRI studies have provided convincing evidence that the hippocampus is associated with nonconscious processes without being confounded by conscious processes. Experimental protocol and analysis parameters included the stimulus type(s), task(s), measures of subjective awareness, explicit memory accuracy, the relevant fMRI contrast(s) or analysis, and confound(s). A systematic review was conducted to identify implicit memory studies that reported fMRI activity in the hippocampus. After applying exclusion criteria, 13 articles remained for analysis. We found that there were no implicit memory fMRI studies where subjective awareness was absent, explicit memory performance was at chance, and there were no confounds that could have driven the observed hippocampal activity. The confounds included explicit memory (including false memory), imbalanced attentional states between conditions (yielding activation of the default-mode network), imbalanced stimuli between conditions, and differential novelty. As such, not a single fMRI study provided convincing evidence that implicit memory was associated with the hippocampus. Neuropsychological evidence was also considered, and implicit memory deficits were caused by factors known to disrupt brain regions beyond the hippocampus, such that the behavioral effects could not be attributed to this region. The present results indicate that implicit memory is not associated with the hippocampus.

根据传统的记忆系统观点,海马体在显性(有意识的)长期记忆中至关重要,而其他脑区则支持内隐(非有意识的)记忆。在过去二十年中,一些 fMRI 研究报告了内隐记忆任务中的海马活动。本讨论文章旨在确定是否有内隐记忆 fMRI 研究提供了令人信服的证据,证明海马与非意识过程相关,而不会被意识过程所混淆。实验方案和分析参数包括刺激类型、任务、主观意识测量、显性记忆准确性、相关的 fMRI 对比或分析以及混淆因素。我们进行了一项系统性回顾,以确定报告海马区 fMRI 活动的内隐记忆研究。在应用排除标准后,剩下 13 篇文章可供分析。我们发现,没有任何内隐记忆 fMRI 研究不存在主观意识,显性记忆的表现是偶然的,也没有任何混杂因素可能导致观察到的海马活动。混杂因素包括显性记忆(包括虚假记忆)、不同条件下的不平衡注意状态(导致默认模式网络的激活)、不同条件下的不平衡刺激以及不同的新颖性。因此,没有一项 fMRI 研究提供了内隐记忆与海马相关的令人信服的证据。此外,还考虑了神经心理学证据,内隐记忆缺陷是由已知会破坏海马以外大脑区域的因素造成的,因此行为效应不能归因于该区域。本研究结果表明,内隐记忆与海马体无关。
{"title":"Is implicit memory associated with the hippocampus?","authors":"Ashley C Steinkrauss, Scott D Slotnick","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2315816","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2315816","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to the traditional memory-systems view, the hippocampus is critical during explicit (conscious) long-term memory, whereas other brain regions support implicit (nonconscious) memory. In the last two decades, some fMRI studies have reported hippocampal activity during implicit memory tasks. The aim of the present discussion paper was to identify whether any implicit memory fMRI studies have provided convincing evidence that the hippocampus is associated with nonconscious processes without being confounded by conscious processes. Experimental protocol and analysis parameters included the stimulus type(s), task(s), measures of subjective awareness, explicit memory accuracy, the relevant fMRI contrast(s) or analysis, and confound(s). A systematic review was conducted to identify implicit memory studies that reported fMRI activity in the hippocampus. After applying exclusion criteria, 13 articles remained for analysis. We found that there were no implicit memory fMRI studies where subjective awareness was absent, explicit memory performance was at chance, and there were no confounds that could have driven the observed hippocampal activity. The confounds included explicit memory (including false memory), imbalanced attentional states between conditions (yielding activation of the default-mode network), imbalanced stimuli between conditions, and differential novelty. As such, not a single fMRI study provided convincing evidence that implicit memory was associated with the hippocampus. Neuropsychological evidence was also considered, and implicit memory deficits were caused by factors known to disrupt brain regions beyond the hippocampus, such that the behavioral effects could not be attributed to this region. The present results indicate that implicit memory is not associated with the hippocampus.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"56-70"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139899447","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The hippocampus and implicit memory. 海马体与内隐记忆
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2024.2354706
Scott D Slotnick

The traditional memory-systems view is that explicit (conscious) long-term memory is associated with the hippocampus and implicit (nonconscious) memory is associated with non-hippocampal brain regions. This special issue of Cognitive Neuroscience focuses on whether the hippocampus is associated with implicit memory. An empirical fMRI paper by Miller, Kennard, Gowland, Antoniades, and Rosenthal (this issue) evaluated recognition memory performance of autobiographical amnesia patients with bilateral damage to hippocampal sub-region CA3 and found they had greater than chance recognition memory performance for spatial sequence learning, spatial item learning-same location, and non-spatial item learning, but chance performance for non-spatial sequence learning and spatial item learning-different location. These results are at odds with the view that the hippocampus is generally involved in sequence learning and complex event recognition. A discussion paper by Steinkrauss and Slotnick (this issue) considered whether fMRI studies have provided evidence that the hippocampus is associated with implicit memory. It was argued that all previous studies have been confounded by explicit memory, attentional states, stimuli, or novelty. This discussion paper is followed by commentaries from Hannula (this issue), Henke and Ruch (this issue), Rosenthal (this issue), Spaak (this issue), Thakral et al. (this issue), and Züst (this issue). The articles in this special issue illustrate that the association between the hippocampus and implicit memory is under active investigation and debate. It is hoped that the evidence and discourse in this issue will provide directions for future research.

传统的记忆系统观点认为,显性(有意识的)长期记忆与海马体有关,而内隐(非有意识的)记忆与非海马体的脑区有关。本期《认知神经科学》(Cognitive Neuroscience)特刊主要探讨海马体是否与内隐记忆有关。Miller、Kennard、Gowland、Antoniades 和 Rosenthal(本期)发表的一篇实证性 fMRI 论文评估了双侧海马亚区 CA3 受损的自传性遗忘症患者的识别记忆表现,发现他们在空间序列学习、空间项目学习-相同位置和非空间项目学习方面的识别记忆表现高于偶然表现,但在非空间序列学习和空间项目学习-不同位置方面的偶然表现高于偶然表现。这些结果与海马一般参与序列学习和复杂事件识别的观点不符。Steinkrauss 和 Slotnick(本期)的一篇讨论论文探讨了 fMRI 研究是否提供了海马体与内隐记忆相关的证据。他们认为,之前的所有研究都受到了显性记忆、注意状态、刺激或新颖性的干扰。在这篇讨论文章之后,Hannula(本期)、Henke 和 Ruch(本期)、Rosenthal(本期)、Spaak(本期)、Thakral 等人(本期)和 Züst(本期)分别发表了评论。本特刊中的文章表明,海马体与内隐记忆之间的联系正受到积极的研究和讨论。希望本期的证据和论述能为今后的研究提供方向。
{"title":"The hippocampus and implicit memory.","authors":"Scott D Slotnick","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2354706","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2354706","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The traditional memory-systems view is that explicit (conscious) long-term memory is associated with the hippocampus and implicit (nonconscious) memory is associated with non-hippocampal brain regions. This special issue of <i>Cognitive Neuroscience</i> focuses on whether the hippocampus is associated with implicit memory. An empirical fMRI paper by Miller, Kennard, Gowland, Antoniades, and Rosenthal (this issue) evaluated recognition memory performance of autobiographical amnesia patients with bilateral damage to hippocampal sub-region CA3 and found they had greater than chance recognition memory performance for spatial sequence learning, spatial item learning-same location, and non-spatial item learning, but chance performance for non-spatial sequence learning and spatial item learning-different location. These results are at odds with the view that the hippocampus is generally involved in sequence learning and complex event recognition. A discussion paper by Steinkrauss and Slotnick (this issue) considered whether fMRI studies have provided evidence that the hippocampus is associated with implicit memory. It was argued that all previous studies have been confounded by explicit memory, attentional states, stimuli, or novelty. This discussion paper is followed by commentaries from Hannula (this issue), Henke and Ruch (this issue), Rosenthal (this issue), Spaak (this issue), Thakral et al. (this issue), and Züst (this issue). The articles in this special issue illustrate that the association between the hippocampus and implicit memory is under active investigation and debate. It is hoped that the evidence and discourse in this issue will provide directions for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"25-26"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141064915","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Counterfactual imagination impairs memory for true actions: EEG and behavioural evidence. 反事实想象会损害对真实行动的记忆:脑电图和行为学证据
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2024.2315814
Phot Dhammapeera, Chloe Brunskill, Robin Hellerstedt, Zara M Bergström

Imagined events can be misremembered as experienced, leading to memory distortions. However, less is known regarding how imagining counterfactual versions of past events can impair existing memories. We addressed this issue, and used EEG to investigate the neurocognitive processes involved when retrieving memories of true events that are associated with a competing imagined event. Participants first performed simple actions with everyday objects (e.g., rolling dice). A week later, they were shown pictures of some of the objects and either imagined the same action they had originally performed, or imagined a counterfactual action (e.g., stacking the dice). Subsequent tests showed that memory for performed actions was reduced after counterfactual imagination when compared to both veridical imagination and a baseline condition that had not been imagined at all, providing novel evidence that counterfactual imagination impairs true memories beyond simple forgetting over time. ERPs and EEG oscillations showed evidence of separate processes associated with memory retrieval versus post-retrieval processes that were recruited to support recall of memories that were challenging to access. The findings show that counterfactual imagination can cause impairments to sensorimotor-rich event memories, and provide new evidence regarding the neurocognitive mechanisms that are recruited when people need to distinguish memories of imagined versus true events.

想象中的事件可能会被误记为经历过的事件,从而导致记忆失真。然而,人们对想象过去事件的反事实版本如何损害现有记忆却知之甚少。我们针对这一问题,利用脑电图研究了在检索与想象事件相关的真实事件记忆时所涉及的神经认知过程。参与者首先用日常物品完成简单的动作(如掷骰子)。一周后,向他们展示其中一些物品的图片,并让他们想象与最初所做动作相同的动作,或想象一个反事实的动作(如将骰子叠起来)。随后的测试表明,与真实想象和完全没有想象的基线条件相比,反事实想象后对所做动作的记忆会减弱,这提供了新的证据,证明反事实想象会损害真实记忆,而不仅仅是随着时间的推移而简单遗忘。ERPs和脑电图振荡显示了与记忆检索相关的独立过程和检索后过程的证据,这些过程被招募来支持回忆那些具有挑战性的记忆。研究结果表明,反事实想象会对感官运动丰富的事件记忆造成损害,并提供了新的证据,说明当人们需要区分想象中的记忆和真实事件的记忆时,会动用哪些神经认知机制。
{"title":"Counterfactual imagination impairs memory for true actions: EEG and behavioural evidence.","authors":"Phot Dhammapeera, Chloe Brunskill, Robin Hellerstedt, Zara M Bergström","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2315814","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2315814","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Imagined events can be misremembered as experienced, leading to memory distortions. However, less is known regarding how imagining counterfactual versions of past events can impair existing memories. We addressed this issue, and used EEG to investigate the neurocognitive processes involved when retrieving memories of true events that are associated with a competing imagined event. Participants first performed simple actions with everyday objects (e.g., rolling dice). A week later, they were shown pictures of some of the objects and either imagined the same action they had originally performed, or imagined a counterfactual action (e.g., stacking the dice). Subsequent tests showed that memory for performed actions was reduced after counterfactual imagination when compared to both veridical imagination and a baseline condition that had not been imagined at all, providing novel evidence that counterfactual imagination impairs true memories beyond simple forgetting over time. ERPs and EEG oscillations showed evidence of separate processes associated with memory retrieval versus post-retrieval processes that were recruited to support recall of memories that were challenging to access. The findings show that counterfactual imagination can cause impairments to sensorimotor-rich event memories, and provide new evidence regarding the neurocognitive mechanisms that are recruited when people need to distinguish memories of imagined versus true events.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"12-23"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139740586","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Examination of the Neural Responses to Emotional and Movement-Related Images. 对情绪和运动相关图像的神经反应的事件相关电位(ERP)检查。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2024-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-16 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2024.2313597
Christine E MacKay, Amy S Desroches, Stephen D Smith

Previous research has suggested that the perception of emotional images may also activate brain regions related to the preparation of motoric plans. However, little research has investigated whether these emotion-movement interactions occur at early or later stages of visual perception. In the current research, event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to examine the time course of the independent - and combined - effects of perceiving emotions and implied movement. Twenty-five participants viewed images from four categories: 1) emotional with implied movement, 2) emotional with no implied movement, 3) neutral with implied movement, and 4) neutral with no implied movement. Both emotional stimuli and movement-related stimuli led to larger N200 (200-300 ms) waveforms. Furthermore, at frontal sites, there was a marginal interaction between emotion and implied movement, such that negative stimuli showed greater N200 amplitudes vs. neutral stimuli, but only for images with implied movement. At posterior sites, a similar effect was observed for images without implied movement. The late positive potential (LPP; 500-1000 ms) was significant for emotion (at frontal sites) and movement (at frontal, central, and posterior sites), as well as for their interaction (at parietal sites), with larger LPPs for negative vs. neutral images with movement only. Together, these results suggest that the perception of emotion and movement interact at later stages of visual perception.

以往的研究表明,对情绪图像的感知也可能激活与准备运动计划有关的大脑区域。然而,很少有研究调查这些情绪与运动的相互作用是发生在视觉感知的早期阶段还是后期阶段。在目前的研究中,我们使用了事件相关电位(ERPs)来检测感知情绪和隐含运动的独立效应和联合效应的时间过程。25 名参与者观看了四个类别的图像:1)有隐含动作的情绪图像;2)无隐含动作的情绪图像;3)有隐含动作的中性图像;4)无隐含动作的中性图像。情绪刺激和运动相关刺激都会导致较大的 N200(200-300 毫秒)波形。此外,在额叶部位,情绪和隐含运动之间存在边际交互作用,即负面刺激与中性刺激相比,N200 波形幅度更大,但仅限于有隐含运动的图像。在后部位点,对没有暗示动作的图像也观察到类似的效应。晚期正电位(LPP;500-1000 毫秒)对情绪(在额叶部位)和运动(在额叶、中央和后部部位)以及它们之间的交互作用(在顶叶部位)都有显著影响,其中仅有运动的负性图像与中性图像相比,晚期正电位更大。这些结果表明,在视觉感知的后期阶段,情绪感知和运动感知是相互影响的。
{"title":"An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Examination of the Neural Responses to Emotional and Movement-Related Images.","authors":"Christine E MacKay, Amy S Desroches, Stephen D Smith","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2313597","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2024.2313597","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has suggested that the perception of emotional images may also activate brain regions related to the preparation of motoric plans. However, little research has investigated whether these emotion-movement interactions occur at early or later stages of visual perception. In the current research, event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to examine the time course of the independent - and combined - effects of perceiving emotions and implied movement. Twenty-five participants viewed images from four categories: 1) emotional with implied movement, 2) emotional with no implied movement, 3) neutral with implied movement, and 4) neutral with no implied movement. Both emotional stimuli and movement-related stimuli led to larger N200 (200-300 ms) waveforms. Furthermore, at frontal sites, there was a marginal interaction between emotion and implied movement, such that negative stimuli showed greater N200 amplitudes vs. neutral stimuli, but only for images with implied movement. At posterior sites, a similar effect was observed for images without implied movement. The late positive potential (LPP; 500-1000 ms) was significant for emotion (at frontal sites) and movement (at frontal, central, and posterior sites), as well as for their interaction (at parietal sites), with larger LPPs for negative vs. neutral images with movement only. Together, these results suggest that the perception of emotion and movement interact at later stages of visual perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"1-11"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139740585","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
TMS must not harm participants: guidelines for evaluating TMS protocol safety. TMS不得伤害参与者:TMS协议安全性评估指南。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2259553
Scott D Slotnick

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can modulate a targeted brain region to assess whether that region is involved in a cognitive process. When TMS is employed in cognitive neuroscience, participants are typically healthy volunteers, and the technique is described as noninvasive. However, TMS parameters can be set such that stimulation produces long-lasting effects. Critically, TMS effects that have any possibility of lasting beyond a participant's time in the lab are potentially harmful. In this editorial, evidence is considered that indicates a 20-Hz multi-day TMS protocol has long-lasting effects, and a continuous theta-burst stimulation protocol needs further testing before it is deemed noninvasive. The following guidelines are provided for TMS protocol evaluation: 1) Effects must be shown to completely dissipate before participants leave the lab by testing well beyond the expected duration. 2) Participants should complete a cognitive test battery before TMS and after the effects are expected to dissipate. 3) Protocols should not be employed that produce effects lasting longer than the time in the lab. 4) The number of participants should ensure error bars are small, and results generalize to the population. 5) Results should be assessed at the group and individual-participant level, and effects should dissipate for every participant. 6) Bayesian analysis should be conducted to evaluate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. 7) Effects should be assessed in multiple cortical regions. It is hoped that these guidelines will be employed to ensure the continued use of TMS as a valuable tool in the field of cognitive neuroscience.

经颅磁刺激(TMS)可以调节靶向大脑区域,以评估该区域是否参与认知过程。当TMS用于认知神经科学时,参与者通常是健康的志愿者,该技术被描述为非侵入性的。然而,TMS参数可以设置为使得刺激产生持久的效果。至关重要的是,TMS效应有可能持续超过参与者在实验室的时间,这是潜在的有害影响。在这篇社论中,有证据表明,20赫兹的多日TMS方案具有持久的效果,而连续θ突发刺激方案在被认为是非侵入性的之前需要进一步测试。为TMS方案评估提供了以下指南:1)在参与者离开实验室之前,必须通过测试远远超过预期持续时间来证明效果完全消散。2) 参与者应在TMS之前和预期效果消散之后完成认知测试。3) 不应使用产生比实验室时间更长的效果的协议。4)参与者的数量应确保误差条较小,结果应推广到人群中。5) 结果应在小组和个人参与者层面进行评估,影响应在每个参与者身上消散。6) 应该进行贝叶斯分析来评估有利于零假设的证据。7) 应评估多个皮质区域的影响。希望这些指南将被用于确保TMS作为认知神经科学领域的一种有价值的工具的持续使用。
{"title":"TMS must not harm participants: guidelines for evaluating TMS protocol safety.","authors":"Scott D Slotnick","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259553","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259553","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) can modulate a targeted brain region to assess whether that region is involved in a cognitive process. When TMS is employed in cognitive neuroscience, participants are typically healthy volunteers, and the technique is described as noninvasive. However, TMS parameters can be set such that stimulation produces long-lasting effects. Critically, TMS effects that have any possibility of lasting beyond a participant's time in the lab are potentially harmful. In this editorial, evidence is considered that indicates a 20-Hz multi-day TMS protocol has long-lasting effects, and a continuous theta-burst stimulation protocol needs further testing before it is deemed noninvasive. The following guidelines are provided for TMS protocol evaluation: 1) Effects must be shown to completely dissipate before participants leave the lab by testing well beyond the expected duration. 2) Participants should complete a cognitive test battery before TMS and after the effects are expected to dissipate. 3) Protocols should not be employed that produce effects lasting longer than the time in the lab. 4) The number of participants should ensure error bars are small, and results generalize to the population. 5) Results should be assessed at the group and individual-participant level, and effects should dissipate for every participant. 6) Bayesian analysis should be conducted to evaluate evidence in favor of the null hypothesis. 7) Effects should be assessed in multiple cortical regions. It is hoped that these guidelines will be employed to ensure the continued use of TMS as a valuable tool in the field of cognitive neuroscience.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"121-126"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10233906","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No convincing evidence the hippocampus is associated with working memory. 没有令人信服的证据表明海马体与工作记忆有关。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2223919
Scott D Slotnick

In a previous discussion paper , twenty-six working memory fMRI studies that reported activity in the hippocampus were systematically analyzed. None of these studies provided convincing evidence that the hippocampus was active during the late delay phase, the only period in which working memory can be isolated from long-term memory processes. Based on these results, it was concluded that working memory does not activate the hippocampus. Six commentaries on the discussion paper were received from Courtney (2022), Kessels and Bergmann (2022), Peters and Reithler (2022), Rose and Chao (2022), Stern and Hasselmo (2022), and Wood et al. (2022). Based on these commentaries, the present response paper considered whether there is evidence of sustained hippocampal activity during the working memory delay period based on depth-electrode recording, whether there are activity-silent working memory mechanisms in the hippocampus, and whether there is hippocampal lesion evidence indicating this region is important for working memory. There was no convincing electrophysiological or neuropsychological evidence that the hippocampus is associated with working memory maintenance, and activity-silent mechanisms were arguably speculative. Given that only a small fraction (approximately 5%) of working memory fMRI studies have reported hippocampal activity and lesion evidence indicates the hippocampus is not necessary for working memory, the burden of proof is on proponents of the view that the hippocampus is important for working memory to provide compelling evidence to support their position. To date, from my perspective, there is no convincing evidence that the hippocampus is associated with working memory.

在之前的一篇讨论论文中,系统地分析了26项工作记忆fMRI研究报告的海马体活动。这些研究都没有提供令人信服的证据,证明海马体在晚延迟阶段是活跃的,而晚延迟阶段是唯一可以将工作记忆与长期记忆过程分离的阶段。基于这些结果,我们得出结论,工作记忆不会激活海马体。我们收到了Courtney(2022)、Kessels和Bergmann(2022)、Peters和Reithler(2022)、Rose和Chao(2022)、Stern和Hasselmo(2022)以及Wood等人(2022)对讨论文件的六篇评论。基于这些评论,本论文考虑了基于深度电极记录的工作记忆延迟期海马是否有持续活动的证据,海马中是否存在活动-沉默的工作记忆机制,以及海马是否有病变证据表明该区域对工作记忆很重要。没有令人信服的电生理学或神经心理学证据表明海马体与工作记忆维持有关,活动沉默机制也可以说是推测性的。鉴于只有一小部分(约5%)的工作记忆fMRI研究报告了海马体的活动,而损伤证据表明海马体对工作记忆不是必需的,因此,海马体对工作记忆很重要的观点的支持者有责任提供令人信服的证据来支持他们的立场。到目前为止,从我的角度来看,没有令人信服的证据表明海马体与工作记忆有关。
{"title":"No convincing evidence the hippocampus is associated with working memory.","authors":"Scott D Slotnick","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2223919","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2223919","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a previous discussion paper , twenty-six working memory fMRI studies that reported activity in the hippocampus were systematically analyzed. None of these studies provided convincing evidence that the hippocampus was active during the late delay phase, the only period in which working memory can be isolated from long-term memory processes. Based on these results, it was concluded that working memory does not activate the hippocampus. Six commentaries on the discussion paper were received from Courtney (2022), Kessels and Bergmann (2022), Peters and Reithler (2022), Rose and Chao (2022), Stern and Hasselmo (2022), and Wood et al. (2022). Based on these commentaries, the present response paper considered whether there is evidence of sustained hippocampal activity during the working memory delay period based on depth-electrode recording, whether there are activity-silent working memory mechanisms in the hippocampus, and whether there is hippocampal lesion evidence indicating this region is important for working memory. There was no convincing electrophysiological or neuropsychological evidence that the hippocampus is associated with working memory maintenance, and activity-silent mechanisms were arguably speculative. Given that only a small fraction (approximately 5%) of working memory fMRI studies have reported hippocampal activity and lesion evidence indicates the hippocampus is not necessary for working memory, the burden of proof is on proponents of the view that the hippocampus is important for working memory to provide compelling evidence to support their position. To date, from my perspective, there is no convincing evidence that the hippocampus is associated with working memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 3","pages":"96-106"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9743570","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Spatiotemporal dynamics of selective attention and visual conflict monitoring using a Stroop task. 选择性注意的时空动力学和使用Stroop任务的视觉冲突监测。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2259554
Rawan Jarrar, Colleen Monahan, Johanna Shattuck, Peter Teale, Eugene Kronberg, Benzi M Kluger, Isabelle Buard

Selective attention and conflict monitoring are daily human phenomena, yet the spatial and temporal neurological underpinnings of these processes are not fully understood. Current literature suggests these executive functions occur via diverse and highly interconnected neural networks, including top-down, bottom-up, and conflict-control loops. To investigate the spatiotemporal activity of these processes, we collected neuromagnetic data using magnetoencephalography (MEG) in 28 healthy adults (age 19-36), while they performed a computerized Stroop task based on color naming. We focused on low-frequency oscillations in the context of top-down control and hypothesized that conflict monitoring-related activity would first be observed in the left anterior cingulate cortex, followed by the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and subsequently in the parietal and temporal lobes. Significant activity between 600-1000 ms post-stimulus onset was found for incongruent vs. congruent/neutral contrasts. Interestingly, spatiotemporal analysis did not provide evidence for a top-down pattern of activation, instead revealing a simultaneous pattern of activation in the frontal and temporal lobes. Most notable is the involvement of the left posterior inferior temporal cortex (pITC) and the left temporoparietal junction (TPJ), which have not conventionally been considered active players in attentional control. These results may be largely driven by alpha and beta oscillations from our sample population. Our findings challenge early theoretical models of top-down processing in the context of cognitive control from an attention perspective and also suggest a need to investigate attentional centers in the temporal lobe. Furthermore, the study highlights the valuable temporal data provided by MEG, which has been missing from previous studies.

选择性注意和冲突监测是人类日常现象,但这些过程的空间和时间神经基础尚未完全理解。目前的文献表明,这些执行功能是通过各种高度互联的神经网络实现的,包括自上而下、自下而上和冲突控制回路。为了研究这些过程的时空活动,我们使用脑磁图(MEG)收集了28名健康成年人(19-36岁)的神经磁数据,同时他们执行了基于颜色命名的计算机化Stroop任务。我们专注于自上而下控制背景下的低频振荡,并假设冲突监测相关活动将首先在左前扣带皮层中观察到,然后在左背外侧前额叶皮层,然后在顶叶和颞叶中观察到。600-1000之间的显著活动 刺激开始后ms发现不一致对比与一致/中性对比。有趣的是,时空分析并没有为自上而下的激活模式提供证据,而是揭示了额叶和颞叶同时激活的模式。最值得注意的是左后颞下皮质(pITC)和左颞顶叶交界处(TPJ)的参与,它们通常不被认为是注意力控制的积极参与者。这些结果可能在很大程度上是由我们样本群体的α和β振荡驱动的。我们的发现从注意力的角度挑战了认知控制背景下自上而下处理的早期理论模型,也表明有必要研究颞叶的注意力中心。此外,该研究强调了MEG提供的有价值的时间数据,而这些数据在以前的研究中是缺失的。
{"title":"Spatiotemporal dynamics of selective attention and visual conflict monitoring using a Stroop task.","authors":"Rawan Jarrar, Colleen Monahan, Johanna Shattuck, Peter Teale, Eugene Kronberg, Benzi M Kluger, Isabelle Buard","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259554","DOIUrl":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2259554","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Selective attention and conflict monitoring are daily human phenomena, yet the spatial and temporal neurological underpinnings of these processes are not fully understood. Current literature suggests these executive functions occur via diverse and highly interconnected neural networks, including top-down, bottom-up, and conflict-control loops. To investigate the spatiotemporal activity of these processes, we collected neuromagnetic data using magnetoencephalography (MEG) in 28 healthy adults (age 19-36), while they performed a computerized Stroop task based on color naming. We focused on low-frequency oscillations in the context of top-down control and hypothesized that conflict monitoring-related activity would first be observed in the left anterior cingulate cortex, followed by the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and subsequently in the parietal and temporal lobes. Significant activity between 600-1000 ms post-stimulus onset was found for incongruent vs. congruent/neutral contrasts. Interestingly, spatiotemporal analysis did not provide evidence for a top-down pattern of activation, instead revealing a simultaneous pattern of activation in the frontal and temporal lobes. Most notable is the involvement of the left posterior inferior temporal cortex (pITC) and the left temporoparietal junction (TPJ), which have not conventionally been considered active players in attentional control. These results may be largely driven by alpha and beta oscillations from our sample population. Our findings challenge early theoretical models of top-down processing in the context of cognitive control from an attention perspective and also suggest a need to investigate attentional centers in the temporal lobe. Furthermore, the study highlights the valuable temporal data provided by MEG, which has been missing from previous studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":" ","pages":"127-136"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10591874/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10287301","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Degree of abstraction rather than ambiguity is crucial for driving mentalizing involvement commentary on "A-EM: a neurocognitive model for understanding mixed and ambiguous emotions and morality". 在“a - em:一种理解混合和模糊情感和道德的神经认知模型”的评论中,抽象程度(而非模糊性)是驱动心智介入的关键。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2181322
Kris Baetens, Ning Ma
ABSTRACT Willems (this issue) proposes a neurocognitive model with a central role allotted to ambiguity in perceived morality and emotion in driving involvement of reflective/mentalizing processes. We argue that abstractness of representation has more explanatory power in this respect. We illustrate this with examples from the verbal and non-verbal domain showing a) concrete-ambiguous emotions processed through reflexive systems and b) abstract-unambiguous emotions processed through the mentalizing system, counter to MA-EM model predictions. However, due to the natural correlation between ambiguity and abstractness, both accounts will typically make convergent predictions.
Willems(本期)提出了一种神经认知模型,其核心作用是在感知道德和情感的模糊性中驱动反思/心智化过程的参与。我们认为表征的抽象性在这方面具有更强的解释力。我们用来自语言和非语言领域的例子来说明这一点:a)通过反射系统处理的具体-模糊情绪和b)通过心智化系统处理的抽象-明确情绪,这与MA-EM模型的预测相反。然而,由于模糊性和抽象性之间的自然关联,这两种解释通常会做出收敛的预测。
{"title":"Degree of abstraction rather than ambiguity is crucial for driving mentalizing involvement <i>commentary on \"A-EM: a neurocognitive model for understanding mixed and ambiguous emotions and morality\"</i>.","authors":"Kris Baetens,&nbsp;Ning Ma","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2181322","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2181322","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Willems (this issue) proposes a neurocognitive model with a central role allotted to ambiguity in perceived morality and emotion in driving involvement of reflective/mentalizing processes. We argue that abstractness of representation has more explanatory power in this respect. We illustrate this with examples from the verbal and non-verbal domain showing a) concrete-ambiguous emotions processed through reflexive systems and b) abstract-unambiguous emotions processed through the mentalizing system, counter to MA-EM model predictions. However, due to the natural correlation between ambiguity and abstractness, both accounts will typically make convergent predictions.","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 2","pages":"70-72"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9250382","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mixed and ambiguous emotions can be studied with verbal irony. 混合和模棱两可的情绪可以用言语反讽来研究。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2181320
Valeria A Pfeifer, Penny M Pexman

In this commentary we draw attention to a context involving mixed and ambiguous emotions: verbal irony. Irony is frequently used, evokes mixed emotional responses (e.g., criticism and amusement), and has been the focus of recent cognitive neuroscience research. Yet, irony has primarily been studied as a linguistic device, and has rarely been considered by emotion researchers. Similarly, linguistics has not considered mixed and ambiguous emotion when studying verbal irony. We argue that verbal irony offers rich opportunities to evoke and study mixed and ambiguous emotions, and might provide advantages for testing the MA-EM model.

在这篇评论中,我们提请注意一个涉及复杂和模棱两可的情感的语境:言语讽刺。反讽经常被使用,引起复杂的情绪反应(例如,批评和娱乐),并且一直是最近认知神经科学研究的焦点。然而,反讽主要是作为一种语言手段来研究的,很少被情感研究者所考虑。同样,语言学在研究言语反讽时也没有考虑到混杂和模棱两可的情感。我们认为言语反讽提供了丰富的机会来唤起和研究混合和模糊的情绪,并可能为测试MA-EM模型提供优势。
{"title":"Mixed and ambiguous emotions can be studied with verbal irony.","authors":"Valeria A Pfeifer,&nbsp;Penny M Pexman","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2181320","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2181320","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this commentary we draw attention to a context involving mixed and ambiguous emotions: verbal irony. Irony is frequently used, evokes mixed emotional responses (e.g., criticism and amusement), and has been the focus of recent cognitive neuroscience research. Yet, irony has primarily been studied as a linguistic device, and has rarely been considered by emotion researchers. Similarly, linguistics has not considered mixed and ambiguous emotion when studying verbal irony. We argue that verbal irony offers rich opportunities to evoke and study mixed and ambiguous emotions, and might provide advantages for testing the MA-EM model.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 2","pages":"65-67"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9250384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Can we distinguish mixed from ambiguous emotions and morality? 我们能区分混杂的、模棱两可的情感和道德吗?
IF 2 4区 医学 Q3 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/17588928.2023.2181321
Kobie van Krieken, Anneke de Graaf, Enny Das

The neurocognitive model of Mixed and Ambiguous Emotions and Morality (MA-EM) makes a relevant case for putting non-unidimensional emotions and morality more prominently on the research agenda. However, existing research challenges its assumptions about the distinction between mixed and ambiguous emotions and morality, and how they relate to reflective versus simulative processing routes, in three respects. First, the emotional state of being moved is generally conceptualized as a non-ambiguous rather than an ambiguous emotion. Second, mixed emotions have been found to elicit reflection rather than simulation. Third, the morality of narrative characters is typically perceived as mixed rather than ambiguous.

混合和模糊情绪和道德(MA-EM)的神经认知模型为将非单向度的情绪和道德放在研究议程的突出位置提供了相关的案例。然而,现有的研究在三个方面挑战了其关于混合和模糊情绪与道德之间区别的假设,以及它们如何与反思与模拟加工路线相关联。首先,被感动的情绪状态通常被概念化为一种非模棱两可的情绪,而不是一种模棱两可的情绪。其次,人们发现,复杂的情绪会引发反思,而不是模拟。第三,叙事角色的道德通常被认为是混合的,而不是模糊的。
{"title":"Can we distinguish mixed from ambiguous emotions and morality?","authors":"Kobie van Krieken,&nbsp;Anneke de Graaf,&nbsp;Enny Das","doi":"10.1080/17588928.2023.2181321","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2023.2181321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The neurocognitive model of Mixed and Ambiguous Emotions and Morality (MA-EM) makes a relevant case for putting non-unidimensional emotions and morality more prominently on the research agenda. However, existing research challenges its assumptions about the distinction between mixed and ambiguous emotions and morality, and how they relate to reflective versus simulative processing routes, in three respects. First, the emotional state of <i>being moved</i> is generally conceptualized as a non-ambiguous rather than an ambiguous emotion. Second, mixed emotions have been found to elicit reflection rather than simulation. Third, the morality of narrative characters is typically perceived as mixed rather than ambiguous.</p>","PeriodicalId":10413,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Neuroscience","volume":"14 2","pages":"68-69"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9620414","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Neuroscience
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1