首页 > 最新文献

Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Deficits in the pupillary response associated with abnormal visuospatial attention allocation in mild traumatic brain injury. 与轻度脑外伤视觉空间注意力分配异常相关的瞳孔反应缺陷。
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-18 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2314727
Mohammed M Alnawmasi, Sieu K Khuu

Introduction: The ability to allocate visual attention is known to be impaired in patients with mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). In the present study, we investigated a possible neural correlate of this cognitive deficit by examining the pupil response of patients with mTBI whilst performing a modified Posner visual search task.

Method: Two experiments were conducted in which the target location was either not cued (Experiment 1) or cued (Experiment 2). Additionally, in Experiment 2, the type of cue (endogenous vs exogenous cue) and cue validity were treated as independent variables. In both experiments, search efficiency was varied by changing shape similarity between target and distractor patterns. The reaction time required to judge whether the target was present or absent and pupil dilation metrics, particularly the pupil dilation latency (PDL) and amplitude (PDA), were measured. Thirteen patients with chronic mTBI and 21 age-, sex-, and IQ -matched controls participated in the study.

Results: In Experiment 1, patients with mTBI displayed a similar PDA for both efficient and inefficient search conditions, while control participants had a significantly larger PDA in inefficient search conditions compared to efficient search conditions. As cognitive load is positively correlated with PDA, our findings suggest that mTBI patients were unable to apply more mental effort whilst performing visual search, particularly if the task is difficult when visual search is inefficient. In Experiment 2, when the target location was cued, patients with mTBI displayed no significant pupil dilation response to the target regardless of the efficiency of the search, nor whether the cue was valid or invalid. These results contrasted with control participants, who were additionally sensitive to the validity of the cue in which PDA was smaller for cue-valid conditions than invalid conditions, particularly for efficient search conditions.

Conclusion: Pupillometry provided further evidence of attention allocation deficits following mTBI.

简介众所周知,轻度脑外伤(mTBI)患者的视觉注意力分配能力会受到损害。在本研究中,我们通过检测轻度脑损伤患者在执行改良的波斯纳视觉搜索任务时的瞳孔反应,研究了这种认知缺陷可能与神经有关:方法:进行了两次实验,目标位置要么没有提示(实验1),要么有提示(实验2)。此外,在实验 2 中,线索类型(内源性线索与外源性线索)和线索有效性被视为自变量。在这两项实验中,搜索效率都是通过改变目标图案和分心图案之间的形状相似度来改变的。实验还测量了判断目标是否存在所需的反应时间和瞳孔放大指标,特别是瞳孔放大潜伏期(PDL)和振幅(PDA)。13 名慢性 mTBI 患者和 21 名年龄、性别和智商相匹配的对照组参加了研究:在实验 1 中,mTBI 患者在高效和低效搜索条件下显示出相似的 PDA,而对照组参与者在低效搜索条件下的 PDA 明显大于高效搜索条件下的 PDA。由于认知负荷与PDA呈正相关,我们的研究结果表明,mTBI患者在进行视觉搜索时无法付出更多的脑力,尤其是在视觉搜索效率低下时,如果任务难度较大,则更是如此。在实验 2 中,当目标位置被提示时,无论搜索效率如何,也无论提示是有效还是无效,mTBI 患者对目标都没有表现出明显的瞳孔放大反应。这些结果与对照组受试者形成了鲜明对比,对照组受试者对线索的有效性更为敏感,在线索有效的条件下,瞳孔放大反应小于无效条件下,尤其是在高效搜索条件下:瞳孔测量法进一步证明了mTBI后的注意力分配缺陷。
{"title":"Deficits in the pupillary response associated with abnormal visuospatial attention allocation in mild traumatic brain injury.","authors":"Mohammed M Alnawmasi, Sieu K Khuu","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2314727","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2314727","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>The ability to allocate visual attention is known to be impaired in patients with mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI). In the present study, we investigated a possible neural correlate of this cognitive deficit by examining the pupil response of patients with mTBI whilst performing a modified Posner visual search task.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Two experiments were conducted in which the target location was either not cued (Experiment 1) or cued (Experiment 2). Additionally, in Experiment 2, the type of cue (endogenous vs exogenous cue) and cue validity were treated as independent variables. In both experiments, search efficiency was varied by changing shape similarity between target and distractor patterns. The reaction time required to judge whether the target was present or absent and pupil dilation metrics, particularly the pupil dilation latency (PDL) and amplitude (PDA), were measured. Thirteen patients with chronic mTBI and 21 age-, sex-, and IQ -matched controls participated in the study.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>In Experiment 1, patients with mTBI displayed a similar PDA for both efficient and inefficient search conditions, while control participants had a significantly larger PDA in inefficient search conditions compared to efficient search conditions. As cognitive load is positively correlated with PDA, our findings suggest that mTBI patients were unable to apply more mental effort whilst performing visual search, particularly if the task is difficult when visual search is inefficient. In Experiment 2, when the target location was cued, patients with mTBI displayed no significant pupil dilation response to the target regardless of the efficiency of the search, nor whether the cue was valid or invalid. These results contrasted with control participants, who were additionally sensitive to the validity of the cue in which PDA was smaller for cue-valid conditions than invalid conditions, particularly for efficient search conditions.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Pupillometry provided further evidence of attention allocation deficits following mTBI.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"855-873"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139899995","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Factor analysis of neuropsychological domains in a preschool clinic. 学前诊所神经心理学领域的因子分析。
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2314777
Amy K Heffelfinger, Erin T Kaseda, Daniel D Holliday, Lauren E Miller, Jennifer I Koop

Introduction: The clinical practice of preschool neuropsychology assumes that our assessment tools are measuring underlying neuropsychological functions, and that these functions are negatively impacted by early life neurological injury, disease, and disorder. This study hypothesized that general intellectual capacity and specific cognitive skills, both "broad" neuropsychological domains and "specific" subdomains within those broader clusters, would be differentiable in a preschool-age clinical population.

Methods: Using neuropsychological data from 580 children (6 and 71 months) seen for a clinical neuropsychological evaluation in the Preschool and Infant Neuropsychological Testing (PINT) Clinic, exploratory factor analyses (EFA) were conducted. Results: A one-factor model provided a good fit when considering verbal, nonverbal, and adaptive functions. Consideration of one- versus two-factor solutions for broad neuropsychological domains indicated that a 2-factor solution provided a significantly better fit for the data. Factor 1 was defined by motor, language, and nonverbal reasoning abilities; Factor 2 was defined by inhibitory control and attention. Further consideration of specific neuropsychological functions also supported a 2-factor solution. Factor 1 ("thinking") was defined by nonverbal reasoning, receptive language, and expressive language; Factor 2 ("processing") was defined by impulse control, inhibitory control, inattention, visual-motor integration, and visuo-constructional abilities. Motor skills cross-loaded onto both factors. Secondary analyses suggest these models provide the best fit for preschool-aged children with > 70 overall intellectual functioning and no comorbid medical diagnosis.

Conclusions: In a clinical sample of preschool-age children, neuropsychological assessment data appears to assess a general level of intellectual capacity or functioning. Further differentiation between assessing "thinking" (knowledge and reasoning skills) and "processing" (cognitive attention and processing of information) can be considered clinically. Next steps include more recent clinical sample replication, consideration of whether neuropsychological profiles are detectable in the preschool-age range and whether the results of early life assessment are predictive of future functioning.

引言:学前神经心理学的临床实践假定,我们的评估工具能够测量潜在的神经心理学功能,而这些功能会受到生命早期神经损伤、疾病和失调的负面影响。本研究假设,在学龄前临床人群中,一般智力能力和特定认知技能,包括 "广义 "神经心理学领域和这些广义领域中的 "特定 "子领域,是可以区分的:使用学龄前和婴幼儿神经心理测试(PINT)诊所中接受临床神经心理评估的 580 名儿童(6 个月和 71 个月)的神经心理数据,进行探索性因子分析(EFA)。结果显示在考虑言语、非言语和适应功能时,单因素模型具有良好的拟合效果。考虑到神经心理领域的单因素和双因素解决方案,双因素解决方案对数据的拟合效果明显更好。因子 1 由运动、语言和非语言推理能力定义;因子 2 由抑制控制和注意力定义。对特定神经心理功能的进一步考虑也支持双因素解决方案。因子 1("思维")由非语言推理、接受性语言和表达性语言定义;因子 2("处理")由冲动控制、抑制控制、注意力不集中、视觉-运动整合和视觉-结构能力定义。运动技能在这两个因子上有交叉负载。二次分析表明,这些模型最适合总体智力功能大于 70 且无合并症的学龄前儿童:结论:在学龄前儿童的临床样本中,神经心理评估数据似乎可以评估智力或功能的总体水平。临床上可以考虑进一步区分评估 "思维"(知识和推理能力)和 "处理"(认知注意力和信息处理能力)。下一步的工作包括:复制更多最新的临床样本,考虑在学龄前阶段是否可以检测到神经心理学特征,以及早期生活评估的结果是否可以预测未来的功能。
{"title":"Factor analysis of neuropsychological domains in a preschool clinic.","authors":"Amy K Heffelfinger, Erin T Kaseda, Daniel D Holliday, Lauren E Miller, Jennifer I Koop","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2314777","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2314777","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>The clinical practice of preschool neuropsychology assumes that our assessment tools are measuring underlying neuropsychological functions, and that these functions are negatively impacted by early life neurological injury, disease, and disorder. This study hypothesized that general intellectual capacity and specific cognitive skills, both \"broad\" neuropsychological domains and \"specific\" subdomains within those broader clusters, would be differentiable in a preschool-age clinical population.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Using neuropsychological data from 580 children (6 and 71 months) seen for a clinical neuropsychological evaluation in the Preschool and Infant Neuropsychological Testing (PINT) Clinic, exploratory factor analyses (EFA) were conducted. Results: A one-factor model provided a good fit when considering verbal, nonverbal, and adaptive functions. Consideration of one- versus two-factor solutions for broad neuropsychological domains indicated that a 2-factor solution provided a significantly better fit for the data. Factor 1 was defined by motor, language, and nonverbal reasoning abilities; Factor 2 was defined by inhibitory control and attention. Further consideration of specific neuropsychological functions also supported a 2-factor solution. Factor 1 (\"thinking\") was defined by nonverbal reasoning, receptive language, and expressive language; Factor 2 (\"processing\") was defined by impulse control, inhibitory control, inattention, visual-motor integration, and visuo-constructional abilities. Motor skills cross-loaded onto both factors. Secondary analyses suggest these models provide the best fit for preschool-aged children with > 70 overall intellectual functioning and no comorbid medical diagnosis.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>In a clinical sample of preschool-age children, neuropsychological assessment data appears to assess a general level of intellectual capacity or functioning. Further differentiation between assessing \"thinking\" (knowledge and reasoning skills) and \"processing\" (cognitive attention and processing of information) can be considered clinically. Next steps include more recent clinical sample replication, consideration of whether neuropsychological profiles are detectable in the preschool-age range and whether the results of early life assessment are predictive of future functioning.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"890-904"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139931387","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Voluntary imitation of dynamic facial expressions in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a facial-behavior analysis. 注意缺陷多动障碍患者对动态面部表情的自主模仿:面部行为分析。
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2320464
Esen Yildirim Demİrdöğen, Bahadır Turan, Murat Gülşen, Hikmet Can Çubukçu, Onur Burak Dursun

Objective: The difficulties involved in social interaction among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have been shown in many studies. Based on the knowledge that the imitation of facial expressions is a key factor in social interaction and functionality, the focus of prior studies has been on the evaluation of facial expressions in individuals with ADHD. However, little is known about voluntary facial mimicry in individuals with ADHD. In this context, we aimed to evaluate the voluntary-facial-imitation intensity of dynamic facial expressions in children with ADHD.

Method: Forty-one children with ADHD and 53 typically developing children were included in the study. Participants were presented with a video of six basic emotions and neutral facial expressions selected from the EU-Emotion Stimulus Set via a screen. After each emotion, the instruction "now imitate it" was given. While the children watched the video, their faces were recorded with a webcam. The intensity of the children's voluntary facial imitations was examined with a computer vision program (Openface) that performs facial analysis on recorded videos.

Results: There was no significant difference between the groups in terms of facial emotion recognition accuracy. In group comparisons of voluntary facial mimicry, children with ADHD showed a significantly higher imitation intensity after emotional expressions of sadness, surprise and fear. There was no difference between the groups after the emotions of happiness, anger and disgust.

Conclusion: This non-obtrusive, noninvasive, and cost-effective method allowed us to measure the quantitative differences in facial mimicry between children with ADHD and typically developing children. Our results contribute new information to the literature by indicating which emotions can be used in the evaluation of social communication skills, as well as intervention targets for these skills, in children with ADHD.

研究目的许多研究表明,注意力缺陷多动障碍(ADHD)儿童在社交互动方面存在困难。基于对面部表情的模仿是社会交往和功能发挥的关键因素这一认知,以往研究的重点是评估多动症患者的面部表情。然而,人们对多动症患者的自愿面部模仿却知之甚少。在这种情况下,我们旨在评估多动症儿童动态面部表情的自愿面部模仿强度:研究对象包括 41 名多动症儿童和 53 名发育正常的儿童。参与者通过屏幕观看一段视频,视频中包含从欧盟情绪刺激集中挑选的六种基本情绪和中性面部表情。在每种情绪之后,都会发出 "现在模仿它 "的指令。在孩子们观看视频的同时,网络摄像头记录下了他们的表情。通过对录制的视频进行面部分析的计算机视觉程序(Openface)对儿童自愿面部模仿的强度进行了检测:结果:在面部情绪识别准确率方面,各组之间没有明显差异。在自愿面部模仿的分组比较中,多动症儿童在表现出悲伤、惊讶和恐惧的情绪后,模仿强度明显更高。在快乐、愤怒和厌恶等情绪表达后,各组之间没有差异:结论:这种非侵入性、非侵害性和经济有效的方法使我们能够测量多动症儿童和发育正常儿童在面部模仿方面的定量差异。我们的研究结果为文献提供了新的信息,指出了哪些情绪可用于评估多动症儿童的社会交往技能,以及这些技能的干预目标。
{"title":"Voluntary imitation of dynamic facial expressions in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a facial-behavior analysis.","authors":"Esen Yildirim Demİrdöğen, Bahadır Turan, Murat Gülşen, Hikmet Can Çubukçu, Onur Burak Dursun","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2320464","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2320464","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>The difficulties involved in social interaction among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have been shown in many studies. Based on the knowledge that the imitation of facial expressions is a key factor in social interaction and functionality, the focus of prior studies has been on the evaluation of facial expressions in individuals with ADHD. However, little is known about voluntary facial mimicry in individuals with ADHD. In this context, we aimed to evaluate the voluntary-facial-imitation intensity of dynamic facial expressions in children with ADHD.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Forty-one children with ADHD and 53 typically developing children were included in the study. Participants were presented with a video of six basic emotions and neutral facial expressions selected from the EU-Emotion Stimulus Set via a screen. After each emotion, the instruction \"now imitate it\" was given. While the children watched the video, their faces were recorded with a webcam. The intensity of the children's voluntary facial imitations was examined with a computer vision program (Openface) that performs facial analysis on recorded videos.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>There was no significant difference between the groups in terms of facial emotion recognition accuracy. In group comparisons of voluntary facial mimicry, children with ADHD showed a significantly higher imitation intensity after emotional expressions of sadness, surprise and fear. There was no difference between the groups after the emotions of happiness, anger and disgust.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>This non-obtrusive, noninvasive, and cost-effective method allowed us to measure the quantitative differences in facial mimicry between children with ADHD and typically developing children. Our results contribute new information to the literature by indicating which emotions can be used in the evaluation of social communication skills, as well as intervention targets for these skills, in children with ADHD.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"915-927"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139912732","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Process scores on measures of learning and memory: issue 2. 学习和记忆测量的过程得分:问题 2。
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-19 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2307218
Matthew Calamia, Dustin B Hammers
{"title":"Process scores on measures of learning and memory: issue 2.","authors":"Matthew Calamia, Dustin B Hammers","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2307218","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2307218","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"759-762"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139905740","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A comparison of diagnostic performance of word-list and story recall tests for biomarker-determined Alzheimer's disease. 比较单词表和故事回忆测试对生物标志物确定的阿尔茨海默病的诊断效果。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-12 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2023.2240060
Davide Bruno, Ainara Jauregi Zinkunegi, Gwendlyn Kollmorgen, Margherita Carboni, Norbert Wild, Cynthia Carlsson, Barbara Bendlin, Ozioma Okonkwo, Nathaniel Chin, Bruce P Hermann, Sanjay Asthana, Kaj Blennow, Rebecca Langhough, Sterling C Johnson, Nunzio Pomara, Henrik Zetterberg, Kimberly D Mueller

Background: Wordlist and story recall tests are routinely employed in clinical practice for dementia diagnosis. In this study, our aim was to establish how well-standard clinical metrics compared to process scores derived from wordlist and story recall tests in predicting biomarker determined Alzheimer's disease, as defined by CSF ptau/Aβ42 ratio.

Methods: Data from 295 participants (mean age = 65 ± 9.) were drawn from the University of Wisconsin - Madison Alzheimer's Disease Research Center (ADRC) and Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention (WRAP). Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT; wordlist) and Logical Memory Test (LMT; story) data were used. Bayesian linear regression analyses were carried out with CSF ptau/Aβ42 ratio as outcome. Sensitivity analyses were carried out with logistic regressions to assess diagnosticity.

Results: LMT generally outperformed AVLT. Notably, the best predictors were primacy ratio, a process score indexing loss of information learned early during test administration, and recency ratio, which tracks loss of recently learned information. Sensitivity analyses confirmed this conclusion.

Conclusions: Our study shows that story recall tests may be better than wordlist tests for detection of dementia, especially when employing process scores alongside conventional clinical scores.

背景:单词表和故事回忆测试是临床诊断痴呆症的常规方法。在本研究中,我们的目的是确定标准临床指标与单词表和故事回忆测试得出的过程得分相比,在预测由 CSF ptau/Aβ42 比率确定的阿尔茨海默病生物标志物方面的效果如何:威斯康星大学麦迪逊分校阿尔茨海默病研究中心(ADRC)和威斯康星州阿尔茨海默病预防登记处(WRAP)收集了 295 名参与者(平均年龄为 65 ± 9 岁)的数据。研究使用了雷氏听觉言语学习测试(AVLT;单词表)和逻辑记忆测试(LMT;故事)的数据。以 CSF ptau/Aβ42 比值为结果进行贝叶斯线性回归分析。用逻辑回归进行了敏感性分析,以评估诊断性:结果:LMT 总体上优于 AVLT。值得注意的是,最好的预测指标是 "先验比 "和 "后验比"。"先验比 "是一个过程评分,用于衡量测试过程中早期学习信息的丢失情况,而 "后验比 "则用于追踪近期学习信息的丢失情况。敏感性分析证实了这一结论:我们的研究表明,在检测痴呆症方面,故事回忆测试可能比单词表测试更好,尤其是在采用过程评分和传统临床评分的情况下。
{"title":"A comparison of diagnostic performance of word-list and story recall tests for biomarker-determined Alzheimer's disease.","authors":"Davide Bruno, Ainara Jauregi Zinkunegi, Gwendlyn Kollmorgen, Margherita Carboni, Norbert Wild, Cynthia Carlsson, Barbara Bendlin, Ozioma Okonkwo, Nathaniel Chin, Bruce P Hermann, Sanjay Asthana, Kaj Blennow, Rebecca Langhough, Sterling C Johnson, Nunzio Pomara, Henrik Zetterberg, Kimberly D Mueller","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2240060","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2240060","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Wordlist and story recall tests are routinely employed in clinical practice for dementia diagnosis. In this study, our aim was to establish how well-standard clinical metrics compared to process scores derived from wordlist and story recall tests in predicting biomarker determined Alzheimer's disease, as defined by CSF ptau/Aβ42 ratio.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Data from 295 participants (mean age = 65 ± 9.) were drawn from the University of Wisconsin - Madison Alzheimer's Disease Research Center (ADRC) and Wisconsin Registry for Alzheimer's Prevention (WRAP). Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT; wordlist) and Logical Memory Test (LMT; story) data were used. Bayesian linear regression analyses were carried out with CSF ptau/Aβ42 ratio as outcome. Sensitivity analyses were carried out with logistic regressions to assess diagnosticity.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>LMT generally outperformed AVLT. Notably, the best predictors were primacy ratio, a process score indexing loss of information learned early during test administration, and recency ratio, which tracks loss of recently learned information. Sensitivity analyses confirmed this conclusion.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our study shows that story recall tests may be better than wordlist tests for detection of dementia, especially when employing process scores alongside conventional clinical scores.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"763-769"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10859550/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10332774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Primacy and recency effects in verbal memory are differentially associated with post-mortem frontal cortex p-tau 217 and 202 levels in a mixed sample of community-dwelling older adults. 在社区居住的老年人混合样本中,言语记忆中的首要效应和追忆效应与死后额叶皮层 p-tau 217 和 202 水平有不同的关联。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-13 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2023.2232583
Kristina M Gicas, William G Honer, Vladislav A Petyuk, Robert S Wilson, Patricia A Boyle, Sue E Leurgans, Julie A Schneider, Philip L De Jager, David A Bennett

Introduction: Serial position effects in verbal memory are associated with in vivo fluid biomarkers and neuropathological outcomes in Alzheimer's disease (AD). To extend the biomarker literature, associations between serial position scores and postmortem levels of brain phosphorylated tau (p-tau) were examined, in the context of Braak stage of neurofibrillary tangle progression.

Method: Participants were 1091 community-dwelling adults (Mage = 80.2, 68.9% female) from the Rush University Religious Orders Study and Memory and Aging Project who were non-demented at enrollment and followed for a mean of 9.2 years until death. The CERAD Word List Memory test administered at baseline and within 1 year of death was used to calculate serial position (primacy, recency) and total recall scores. Proteomic analyses quantified p-tau 217 and 202 from dorsolateral prefrontal cortex samples. Linear regressions assessed associations between cognitive scores and p-tau with Braak stage as a moderator.

Results: Cognitive status proximal to death indicated 34.7% were unimpaired, 26.2% met criteria for MCI, and 39.0% for dementia. Better baseline primacy recall, but not recency recall, was associated with lower p-tau 217 levels across Braak stages. Delayed recall showed a similar pattern as primacy. There was no main effect of immediate recall, but an interaction with Braak stages indicated a negative association with p-tau 217 level only in Braak V-VI. Within 1 year of death, there were no main effects for cognitive scores; however, recency, immediate and delayed recall scores interacted with Braak stage showing better recall was associated with lower p-tau 217 only in Braak V-VI. No associations were observed with p-tau 202.

Conclusions: Primacy recall measured in non-demented adults may be sensitive to emergent tau phosphorylation that occurs in the earliest stages of AD. Serial position scores may complement the routinely used delayed recall score and p-tau biomarkers to detect preclinical AD.

简介言语记忆中的序列位置效应与阿尔茨海默病(AD)的体内体液生物标志物和神经病理学结果有关。为了扩展生物标志物文献,我们结合神经纤维缠结发展的布拉克阶段,研究了序列位置得分与脑磷酸化 tau(p-tau)的尸检水平之间的关联:研究对象是拉什大学宗教团体研究和记忆与衰老项目中的1091名社区成年人(年龄=80.2,68.9%为女性),他们在入组时均无痴呆症,平均随访9.2年直至死亡。在基线和死亡后一年内进行的 CERAD 单词表记忆测试被用来计算序列位置(主要性、再现性)和总回忆分数。蛋白质组分析量化了背外侧前额叶皮层样本中的 p-tau 217 和 202。线性回归评估了认知评分与p-tau之间的关联,并以Braak阶段作为调节因子:结果:死亡前的认知状况显示,34.7%的人无认知障碍,26.2%的人符合 MCI 标准,39.0%的人符合痴呆标准。在不同的布拉克阶段,较好的基线原始回忆(而非回顾回忆)与较低的 p-tau 217 水平相关。延迟回忆的模式与基线回忆类似。即时回忆没有主效应,但与 Braak 阶段的交互作用表明,只有在 Braak V-VI 阶段与 p-tau 217 水平呈负相关。在死亡后 1 年内,认知评分没有主效应;但是,回忆、即时回忆和延迟回忆评分与 Braak 阶段的交互作用表明,只有在 Braak V-VI 阶段,较好的回忆与较低的 p-tau 217 相关。结论:结论:在非痴呆成人中测量的原始回忆可能对发生在AD早期阶段的新出现的tau磷酸化很敏感。序列位置评分可以补充常规使用的延迟回忆评分和p-tau生物标志物,以检测临床前AD。
{"title":"Primacy and recency effects in verbal memory are differentially associated with post-mortem frontal cortex p-tau 217 and 202 levels in a mixed sample of community-dwelling older adults.","authors":"Kristina M Gicas, William G Honer, Vladislav A Petyuk, Robert S Wilson, Patricia A Boyle, Sue E Leurgans, Julie A Schneider, Philip L De Jager, David A Bennett","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2232583","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2232583","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Serial position effects in verbal memory are associated with <i>in vivo</i> fluid biomarkers and neuropathological outcomes in Alzheimer's disease (AD). To extend the biomarker literature, associations between serial position scores and postmortem levels of brain phosphorylated tau (p-tau) were examined, in the context of Braak stage of neurofibrillary tangle progression.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants were 1091 community-dwelling adults (M<sub>age</sub> = 80.2, 68.9% female) from the Rush University Religious Orders Study and Memory and Aging Project who were non-demented at enrollment and followed for a mean of 9.2 years until death. The CERAD Word List Memory test administered at baseline and within 1 year of death was used to calculate serial position (primacy, recency) and total recall scores. Proteomic analyses quantified p-tau 217 and 202 from dorsolateral prefrontal cortex samples. Linear regressions assessed associations between cognitive scores and p-tau with Braak stage as a moderator.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Cognitive status proximal to death indicated 34.7% were unimpaired, 26.2% met criteria for MCI, and 39.0% for dementia. Better baseline primacy recall, but not recency recall, was associated with lower p-tau 217 levels across Braak stages. Delayed recall showed a similar pattern as primacy. There was no main effect of immediate recall, but an interaction with Braak stages indicated a negative association with p-tau 217 level only in Braak V-VI. Within 1 year of death, there were no main effects for cognitive scores; however, recency, immediate and delayed recall scores interacted with Braak stage showing better recall was associated with lower p-tau 217 only in Braak V-VI. No associations were observed with p-tau 202.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Primacy recall measured in non-demented adults may be sensitive to emergent tau phosphorylation that occurs in the earliest stages of AD. Serial position scores may complement the routinely used delayed recall score and p-tau biomarkers to detect preclinical AD.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"770-785"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10787031/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9835575","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparison of core and process scores on the California Verbal Learning Test-3 for Parkinson's disease and essential tremor patients. 帕金森病患者和本质性震颤患者在加州言语学习测试-3 中的核心得分和过程得分的比较。
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-28 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2023.2241653
Karen Torres

Introduction: Parkinson's disease (PD) and essential tremor (ET) are two disorders known to lead to executive dysfunction, presumably through distinct pathways to the frontal lobes via the striatum or cerebellum, respectively. Memory functioning in PD and ET patients has been previously suggested to be adversely impacted by executive dysfunction. The aims of this exploratory study were to compare memory performance between and within groups on the California Verbal Learning Test - 3 (CVLT-3) through the analysis of core and process scores and to understand the relationship of these scores with measures of executive functioning.

Method: Seventy PD and 54 ET patients completed comprehensive neuropsychological testing. Independent sample t-tests or Mann-Whitney tests were used to compare between group core and process scores on the CVLT-3. Within-subjects analyses were conducted via Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test due to nonparametric data. Spearman's correlations were conducted to explore the relationship between memory process scores and measures of executive functioning.

Results: The ET and PD samples were similar with regard to age, education, gender, and general cognitive functioning. PD patients made more repetition errors (U = 2391.50, p = .01) than ET patients and Normal Memory PD patients made more repetition errors than Low Memory PD patients (U= 711.00, p= .00). Correlational analyses revealed repetition errors were negatively associated with tests of inhibition, set shifting, and working memory (rs = -.293, -.232). ET patients demonstrated a preference for a serial cluster learning strategy (T = 861.00, p = .005), similar to PD patients (T= 1633.00, p = <.001).

Conclusions: The study revealed presence of higher repetition errors in the PD sample that was demonstrated to have a negative relationship with measures of executive functioning. Implications for investigating process ("qualitative") scores in memory performance to determine extent of executive involvement are discussed.

导言:帕金森病(PD)和本质性震颤(ET)是两种已知会导致执行功能障碍的疾病,它们可能分别通过纹状体或小脑通往额叶的不同路径。以前曾有研究表明,执行功能障碍会对帕金森病和ET患者的记忆功能产生不利影响。这项探索性研究的目的是通过分析核心和过程得分,比较组间和组内的加州言语学习测验-3(CVLT-3)记忆表现,并了解这些得分与执行功能测量的关系:70名PD和54名ET患者完成了综合神经心理学测试。采用独立样本 t 检验或曼-惠特尼检验来比较 CVLT-3 的组间核心和过程得分。由于采用的是非参数数据,因此通过 Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test 进行受试者内分析。斯皮尔曼相关分析用于探讨记忆过程得分与执行功能测量之间的关系:在年龄、教育程度、性别和一般认知功能方面,ET 和 PD 样本相似。记忆障碍患者的重复错误(U=2391.50,p=.01)多于ET患者,而正常记忆障碍患者的重复错误多于低记忆障碍患者(U=711.00,p=.00)。相关分析表明,重复错误与抑制测试、集合转移和工作记忆测试呈负相关(rs = -.293, -.232)。ET患者表现出对序列群学习策略的偏好(T = 861.00,p = .005),与PD患者相似(T = 1633.00,p = 结论):研究表明,帕金森病样本中存在较高的重复错误,这与执行功能的测量结果呈负相关。本研究讨论了调查记忆表现的过程("定性")分数以确定执行参与程度的意义。
{"title":"Comparison of core and process scores on the California Verbal Learning Test-3 for Parkinson's disease and essential tremor patients.","authors":"Karen Torres","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2241653","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2241653","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Parkinson's disease (PD) and essential tremor (ET) are two disorders known to lead to executive dysfunction, presumably through distinct pathways to the frontal lobes via the striatum or cerebellum, respectively. Memory functioning in PD and ET patients has been previously suggested to be adversely impacted by executive dysfunction. The aims of this exploratory study were to compare memory performance between and within groups on the California Verbal Learning Test - 3 (CVLT-3) through the analysis of core and process scores and to understand the relationship of these scores with measures of executive functioning.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Seventy PD and 54 ET patients completed comprehensive neuropsychological testing. Independent sample t-tests or Mann-Whitney tests were used to compare between group core and process scores on the CVLT-3. Within-subjects analyses were conducted via Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test due to nonparametric data. Spearman's correlations were conducted to explore the relationship between memory process scores and measures of executive functioning.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The ET and PD samples were similar with regard to age, education, gender, and general cognitive functioning. PD patients made more repetition errors (<i>U</i> = 2391.50, <i>p</i> = .01) than ET patients and Normal Memory PD patients made more repetition errors than Low Memory PD patients (<i>U</i>= 711.00, <i>p</i>= .00). Correlational analyses revealed repetition errors were negatively associated with tests of inhibition, set shifting, and working memory (<i>r<sub>s</sub> </i>= -.293, -.232). ET patients demonstrated a preference for a serial cluster learning strategy (<i>T</i> = 861.00, <i>p</i> = .005), similar to PD patients (<i>T</i>= 1633.00, <i>p</i> = <.001).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The study revealed presence of higher repetition errors in the PD sample that was demonstrated to have a negative relationship with measures of executive functioning. Implications for investigating process (\"qualitative\") scores in memory performance to determine extent of executive involvement are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"798-812"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10242556","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Serial position effects and mild cognitive impairment: a comparison of measures and scoring approaches. 序列位置效应与轻度认知障碍:测量和评分方法比较。
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-05-31 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2023.2214298
Daniel S Weitzner, Matthew Calamia

Introduction: Serial position effects (SPEs) have shown promise as predictors of future cognitive decline and conversion from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to Alzheimer's disease (AD), even when accounting for total learning and memory scores. However, conflicting results have been found in the literature, which may be at least partially related to the many ways in which SPEs are calculated. The current study aimed to address the discrepancies in the literature by examining whether one method of analyzing SPEs is more sensitive at distinguishing those with and without psychometrically defined MCI.

Method: 86 older adult participants (57 healthy comparison, 29 MCI) completed the California Verbal Learning Test, Third Edition (CVLT3) and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), along with measures assessing multiple cognitive domains. Each participant completed two visits, between 3 and 9 days apart, with a different memory measure administered on each day. The standard scoring approach and the regional scoring approach to calculating SPEs were compared.

Results: Results showed that, when significant differences were found, SPEs were always reduced in the MCI group compared to the healthy comparison group when using regional scoring; however, results were not as consistent when using standard scoring. Further, lower primacy than recency scores were only consistently seen in the MCI group when using the RAVLT but not the CVLT3. ROC analyses showed that only regional scoring of SPEs from delayed recall of the RAVLT and the CVLT3 accurately discriminated between those with and without MCI.

Conclusion: Regional scoring of SPEs may be more sensitive at identifying subtle cognitive decline compared to standard scoring. However, the specific measure that is used to analyze SPEs can impact the interpretation of findings.

导言:序列位置效应(SPEs)有望预测未来认知能力的下降以及从轻度认知障碍(MCI)到阿尔茨海默病(AD)的转变,即使考虑到总的学习和记忆分数也是如此。然而,文献中发现了相互矛盾的结果,这可能至少部分与 SPEs 的多种计算方法有关。方法:86 名老年参与者(57 名健康对比者,29 名 MCI 患者)完成了加利福尼亚言语学习测试第三版(CVLT3)和雷伊听觉言语学习测试(RAVLT),以及评估多个认知领域的测量。每位受试者完成两次访问,每次相隔 3 到 9 天,每天进行不同的记忆测量。对计算 SPE 的标准评分法和区域评分法进行了比较:结果显示,当发现显著差异时,采用区域计分法时,与健康对比组相比,MCI 组的 SPE 总是降低;但采用标准计分法时,结果并不一致。此外,只有在使用 RAVLT 而非 CVLT3 时,MCI 组的主要得分才会持续低于重复得分。ROC分析表明,只有对RAVLT和CVLT3延迟回忆中的SPEs进行区域评分,才能准确区分MCI患者和非MCI患者:结论:与标准评分相比,区域性 SPEs 评分在识别细微认知能力下降方面可能更加敏感。结论:与标准评分相比,区域评分在识别细微认知能力下降方面可能更加敏感。然而,用于分析 SPEs 的特定测量方法可能会影响对研究结果的解释。
{"title":"Serial position effects and mild cognitive impairment: a comparison of measures and scoring approaches.","authors":"Daniel S Weitzner, Matthew Calamia","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2214298","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2214298","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Serial position effects (SPEs) have shown promise as predictors of future cognitive decline and conversion from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to Alzheimer's disease (AD), even when accounting for total learning and memory scores. However, conflicting results have been found in the literature, which may be at least partially related to the many ways in which SPEs are calculated. The current study aimed to address the discrepancies in the literature by examining whether one method of analyzing SPEs is more sensitive at distinguishing those with and without psychometrically defined MCI.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>86 older adult participants (57 healthy comparison, 29 MCI) completed the California Verbal Learning Test, Third Edition (CVLT3) and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), along with measures assessing multiple cognitive domains. Each participant completed two visits, between 3 and 9 days apart, with a different memory measure administered on each day. The standard scoring approach and the regional scoring approach to calculating SPEs were compared.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results showed that, when significant differences were found, SPEs were always reduced in the MCI group compared to the healthy comparison group when using regional scoring; however, results were not as consistent when using standard scoring. Further, lower primacy than recency scores were only consistently seen in the MCI group when using the RAVLT but not the CVLT3. ROC analyses showed that only regional scoring of SPEs from delayed recall of the RAVLT and the CVLT3 accurately discriminated between those with and without MCI.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>Regional scoring of SPEs may be more sensitive at identifying subtle cognitive decline compared to standard scoring. However, the specific measure that is used to analyze SPEs can impact the interpretation of findings.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"813-824"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9547783","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Recognition subtests of the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status: evidence for a cortical vs. subcortical distinction. 用于评估神经心理状态的可重复电池的识别子测验:皮层与皮层下区分的证据。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-10-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2023.2259044
Julia V Vehar, Shervin Rahimpour, Paolo Moretti, Panagiotis Kassavetis, Jumana Alshaikh, John Rolston, Kevin Duff

Introduction: Within clinical neuropsychology, a classic diagnostic distinction is made between cortical and subcortical disorders, especially based on their memory profiles. Typically, this is based on the comparison of recall and recognition trials, where individuals with cortical conditions do not tend to benefit (i.e., score well) on recognition trials and individuals with subcortical conditions do. Although the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) is a widely used brief cognitive battery, there is a lack of evidence to support this measure's utility in distinguishing between the memory profiles of these conditions.

Method: Thirty-six mild Alzheimer's disease (AD), 55 Parkinson's disease (PD), and 105 essential tremor (ET) participants (N = 196) were administered the RBANS with additional Story and Figure Recognition subtests. Group differences on recall and recognition scores (Total Correct, Hits or True Positives, False Positive Errors, and discriminability index) were examined across the three groups, while controlling for the influence of age and gender.

Results: As expected, individuals with AD had poorer recognition scores compared to the other clinical groups across tasks (all p-values < .05), while the ET sample largely performed comparably to the PD sample. With the exception of comparable Figure Recognition and Recall in the PD sample, all groups exhibited significantly greater recognition Hit performance compared to Recall (all p-values < .05).

Conclusions: The group differences in performance across RBANS recognition subtests suggest support for traditional "cortical" and "subcortical" profiles. However, all groups, including the mild AD sample, demonstrated a benefit from recognition cues compared to free recall. Overall, these findings support the inclusion of the newly developed Story and Figure Recognition subtests in future clinical practice and research endeavors.

引言:在临床神经心理学中,皮层和皮层下疾病之间有一个经典的诊断区别,特别是基于它们的记忆特征。通常,这是基于回忆和识别试验的比较,其中皮层条件的个体在识别试验中往往不会受益(即得分良好),而皮层下条件的个体则会受益。尽管神经心理状态评估的可重复电池组(RBANS)是一个广泛使用的简短认知电池组,缺乏证据支持这一措施在区分这些情况的记忆特征方面的效用。方法:36名轻度阿尔茨海默病(AD)、55名帕金森病(PD)和105名原发性震颤(ET)参与者(N = 196)进行RBANS,并进行额外的故事和图形识别子测验。在控制年龄和性别的影响的同时,研究了三组在回忆和识别得分(总正确率、命中率或真阳性率、假阳性错误和可辨别性指数)方面的组间差异。结果:不出所料,与其他临床组相比,AD患者在任务中的识别得分较差(所有p值均<0.05),而ET样本的表现与PD样本相当。除了PD样本中可比较的图形识别和回忆外,与回忆相比,所有组都表现出显著更高的识别命中表现(所有p值均<.05)。结论:RBANS识别子测验中的组间表现差异表明支持传统的“皮层”和“皮层下”特征。然而,与自由回忆相比,包括轻度AD样本在内的所有组都证明了识别线索的益处。总的来说,这些发现支持将新开发的故事和图形识别子测验纳入未来的临床实践和研究工作。
{"title":"Recognition subtests of the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status: evidence for a cortical vs. subcortical distinction.","authors":"Julia V Vehar, Shervin Rahimpour, Paolo Moretti, Panagiotis Kassavetis, Jumana Alshaikh, John Rolston, Kevin Duff","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2259044","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2259044","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Within clinical neuropsychology, a classic diagnostic distinction is made between cortical and subcortical disorders, especially based on their memory profiles. Typically, this is based on the comparison of recall and recognition trials, where individuals with cortical conditions do not tend to benefit (i.e., score well) on recognition trials and individuals with subcortical conditions do. Although the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) is a widely used brief cognitive battery, there is a lack of evidence to support this measure's utility in distinguishing between the memory profiles of these conditions.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Thirty-six mild Alzheimer's disease (AD), 55 Parkinson's disease (PD), and 105 essential tremor (ET) participants (<i>N</i> = 196) were administered the RBANS with additional Story and Figure Recognition subtests. Group differences on recall and recognition scores (Total Correct, Hits or True Positives, False Positive Errors, and discriminability index) were examined across the three groups, while controlling for the influence of age and gender.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>As expected, individuals with AD had poorer recognition scores compared to the other clinical groups across tasks (all p-values < .05), while the ET sample largely performed comparably to the PD sample. With the exception of comparable Figure Recognition and Recall in the PD sample, all groups exhibited significantly greater recognition Hit performance compared to Recall (all p-values < .05).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The group differences in performance across RBANS recognition subtests suggest support for traditional \"cortical\" and \"subcortical\" profiles. However, all groups, including the mild AD sample, demonstrated a benefit from recognition cues compared to free recall. Overall, these findings support the inclusion of the newly developed Story and Figure Recognition subtests in future clinical practice and research endeavors.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"786-797"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10922284/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41112551","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between learning slopes and Alzheimer's Disease biomarkers in cognitively unimpaired participants with and without subjective memory concerns. 在有和没有主观记忆问题的认知未受损参与者中,学习斜率与阿尔茨海默病生物标志物之间的关系。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2023-09-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-07 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2023.2254444
Dustin B Hammers, Julian V Pentchev, Hee Jin Kim, Robert J Spencer, Liana G Apostolova

Objective: Learning slopes represent serial acquisition of information during list-learning tasks. Although several calculations for learning slopes exist, the Learning Ratio (LR) has recently demonstrated the highest sensitivity toward changes in cognition and Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers. However, investigation of learning slopes in cognitively unimpaired individuals with subjective memory concerns (SMC) has been limited. The current study examines the association of learning slopes to SMC, and the role of SMC in the relationship between learning slopes and AD biomarkers in cognitively unimpaired individuals.

Method: Data from 950 cognitively unimpaired participants from the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (aged 55 to 89) were used to calculate learning slope metrics. Learning slopes among those with and without SMC were compared with demographic correction, and the relationships of learning slopes with AD biomarkers of bilateral hippocampal volume and β-amyloid pathology were determined.

Results: Learning slopes were consistently predictive of hippocampal atrophy and β-amyloid deposition. Results were heightened for LR relative to the other learning slopes. Additionally, interaction analyses revealed different associations between learning slopes and hippocampal volume as a function of SMC status.

Conclusions: Learning slopes appear to be sensitive to SMC and AD biomarkers, with SMC status influencing the relationship in cognitively unimpaired participants. These findings advance our knowledge of SMC, and suggest that LR - in particular - can be an important tool for the detection of AD pathology in both SMC and in AD clinical trials.

目标:学习斜率表示在列表学习任务中对信息的连续获取。尽管存在几种学习斜率的计算方法,但学习率(LR)最近对认知和阿尔茨海默病(AD)生物标志物的变化表现出最高的敏感性。然而,对具有主观记忆问题(SMC)的认知未受损个体的学习斜率的研究有限。目前的研究考察了学习斜率与SMC的关系,以及SMC在认知未受损个体的学习斜率和AD生物标志物之间关系中的作用。方法:使用来自阿尔茨海默病神经成像倡议的950名认知未受损参与者(年龄在55岁至89岁之间)的数据来计算学习斜率指标。将SMC患者和无SMC患者的学习斜率与人口统计学校正进行比较,并确定学习斜率与双侧海马体积AD生物标志物和β-淀粉样蛋白病理的关系。结果:学习斜率始终可以预测海马萎缩和β-淀粉样蛋白沉积。相对于其他学习斜率,LR的结果更高。此外,相互作用分析揭示了学习斜率和海马体积之间的不同关联,作为SMC状态的函数。结论:学习斜率似乎对SMC和AD生物标志物敏感,SMC状态影响认知未受损参与者的关系。这些发现提高了我们对SMC的认识,并表明LR——尤其是LR——可以成为SMC和AD临床试验中检测AD病理的重要工具。
{"title":"The relationship between learning slopes and Alzheimer's Disease biomarkers in cognitively unimpaired participants with and without subjective memory concerns.","authors":"Dustin B Hammers, Julian V Pentchev, Hee Jin Kim, Robert J Spencer, Liana G Apostolova","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2254444","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2023.2254444","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Learning slopes represent serial acquisition of information during list-learning tasks. Although several calculations for learning slopes exist, the Learning Ratio (LR) has recently demonstrated the highest sensitivity toward changes in cognition and Alzheimer's disease (AD) biomarkers. However, investigation of learning slopes in cognitively unimpaired individuals with subjective memory concerns (SMC) has been limited. The current study examines the association of learning slopes to SMC, and the role of SMC in the relationship between learning slopes and AD biomarkers in cognitively unimpaired individuals.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Data from 950 cognitively unimpaired participants from the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (aged 55 to 89) were used to calculate learning slope metrics. Learning slopes among those with and without SMC were compared with demographic correction, and the relationships of learning slopes with AD biomarkers of bilateral hippocampal volume and β-amyloid pathology were determined.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Learning slopes were consistently predictive of hippocampal atrophy and β-amyloid deposition. Results were heightened for LR relative to the other learning slopes. Additionally, interaction analyses revealed different associations between learning slopes and hippocampal volume as a function of SMC status.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Learning slopes appear to be sensitive to SMC and AD biomarkers, with SMC status influencing the relationship in cognitively unimpaired participants. These findings advance our knowledge of SMC, and suggest that LR - in particular - can be an important tool for the detection of AD pathology in both SMC and in AD clinical trials.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"727-743"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10916703/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10173547","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1