首页 > 最新文献

Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Patterns of prospective memory errors differ in persons with multiple sclerosis. 多发性硬化症患者的前瞻性记忆错误模式有所不同。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348775
Caitlyn A Nguyen, Sarah A Raskin, Aaron P Turner, Zaenab Dhari, Lindsay O Neto, Elizabeth S Gromisch

Introduction: Prospective memory (PM) deficits have been documented in multiple sclerosis (MS). This study aimed to explore the specific types of errors made by persons with MS (PwMS), including differences between PwMS and healthy controls (HC) and PwMS who do and do not have impairments in processing speed and/or verbal learning and memory.

Method: PwMS (n = 111) and HC (n = 75) completed the Memory for Intentions Test (MIST), an objective measure of PM that has five types of errors that can be coded (PM failure, task substitution, loss of content, loss of time, and random errors). The number and types of PM errors were calculated for the overall MIST and six subscales, which break down performance by types of delay (2-Minute and 15-Minute), cue (Time and Event), and response (Verbal and Action). Impairment was defined as performing < 1.5 SD on either the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) or Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT). Bivariate analyses were used to examine group differences, with post-hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections.

Results: Nearly 93% of PwMS made at least one PM error, compared to 76% of HC (V = .24, p = .001). The most commonly made PM error by PwMS was loss of content errors (45.0%). PwMS made significantly more task substitution errors (26.4% vs. 7.6%, p < .001) and fewer loss of time errors (9.5% vs. 21.2%, p < .001) than HC. Impaired PwMS made more errors than non-impaired PwMS, specifically PM failures on time-based tasks.

Conclusions: PM errors are common in PwMS, particularly when there are longer delays and time-based cues. Not only do PwMS make more errors than demographically similar HC, but they exhibit different cognitive process failures.

导言:多发性硬化症(MS)患者存在前瞻性记忆(PM)缺陷。本研究旨在探讨多发性硬化症患者(PwMS)所犯错误的具体类型,包括多发性硬化症患者与健康对照组(HC)之间的差异,以及多发性硬化症患者在处理速度和/或言语学习与记忆方面有无障碍:PwMS(n=111)和HC(n=75)完成了 "意图记忆测试"(MIST),这是一种客观的PM测量方法,有五种可编码的错误类型(PM失败、任务替代、内容丢失、时间丢失和随机错误)。PM 错误的数量和类型是根据总体 MIST 和六个子量表计算得出的,这六个子量表按延迟类型(2 分钟和 15 分钟)、提示(时间和事件)和反应(语言和动作)进行了细分。障碍被定义为表现结果:近 93% 的 PwMS 至少出现过一次 PM 错误,而 HC 的这一比例为 76% (V = .24, p = .001)。残疾人最常犯的 PM 错误是内容丢失错误(45.0%)。PwMS 犯的任务替代错误要多得多(26.4% 对 7.6%,p p 结论:PM 错误在 PwMS 中很常见,尤其是在有较长的延迟和基于时间的提示时。与人口统计学上相似的 HC 相比,PwMS 不仅会犯更多的错误,而且会表现出不同的认知过程失误。
{"title":"Patterns of prospective memory errors differ in persons with multiple sclerosis.","authors":"Caitlyn A Nguyen, Sarah A Raskin, Aaron P Turner, Zaenab Dhari, Lindsay O Neto, Elizabeth S Gromisch","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348775","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348775","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Prospective memory (PM) deficits have been documented in multiple sclerosis (MS). This study aimed to explore the specific types of errors made by persons with MS (PwMS), including differences between PwMS and healthy controls (HC) and PwMS who do and do not have impairments in processing speed and/or verbal learning and memory.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>PwMS (<i>n</i> = 111) and HC (<i>n</i> = 75) completed the Memory for Intentions Test (MIST), an objective measure of PM that has five types of errors that can be coded (PM failure, task substitution, loss of content, loss of time, and random errors). The number and types of PM errors were calculated for the overall MIST and six subscales, which break down performance by types of delay (2-Minute and 15-Minute), cue (Time and Event), and response (Verbal and Action). Impairment was defined as performing < 1.5 SD on either the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) or Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT). Bivariate analyses were used to examine group differences, with post-hoc pairwise comparisons with Bonferroni corrections.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Nearly 93% of PwMS made at least one PM error, compared to 76% of HC (<i>V</i> = .24, <i>p</i> = .001). The most commonly made PM error by PwMS was loss of content errors (45.0%). PwMS made significantly more task substitution errors (26.4% vs. 7.6%, <i>p</i> < .001) and fewer loss of time errors (9.5% vs. 21.2%, <i>p</i> < .001) than HC. Impaired PwMS made more errors than non-impaired PwMS, specifically PM failures on time-based tasks.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>PM errors are common in PwMS, particularly when there are longer delays and time-based cues. Not only do PwMS make more errors than demographically similar HC, but they exhibit different cognitive process failures.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"329-340"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140859659","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring cultural contributions to the neuropsychology of social cognition: the advanced clinical solutions. 探索社会认知神经心理学的文化贡献:高级临床解决方案。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348212
Paul G Nestor, Ashley-Ann Woodhull

Introduction: Culture and social cognition are deeply intertwined, yet how this rich intersectionality is expressed neuropsychologically remains an important question.

Method: In a convenience sample of 128 young adults (mean age = 24.9 years) recruited from a majority-minority urban university, we examined performance-based neuropsychological measures of social cognition, the Advanced Clinical Solutions-Social Perception (ACS-SP), in relation to both cultural orientation, as assessed by the Individualism-Collectivism Scale (ICS) and spoken English language, as assessed by the oral word pronunciation measure of the Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT4).

Results: Results indicated higher WRAT4 scores correlated with better performance across all ACS-SP measures of social cognition. Controlling for these associations in spoken English, partial correlations linked lower scores across both prosody interpretation and affect naming ACS-SP tasks with a propensity to view social relationships vertically, irrespective of individualistic or collectivistic orientations. Hierarchical regression results showed that cultural orientation and English-language familiarity each specifically and uniquely contributed to ACS-SP performance for matching prosody with facial expressions.

Conclusions: These findings underscore the importance of incorporating and prioritizing both language and cultural factors in neuropsychological studies of social cognition. They may be viewed as offering strong support for expanding the boundaries of the construct of social cognition beyond its current theoretical framework of one that privileges Western, educated, industralized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) values, customs, and epistemologies.

引言文化与社会认知深深地交织在一起,然而这种丰富的交叉性如何在神经心理学上表现出来仍然是一个重要的问题:方法:我们从一所少数族裔占多数的城市大学中方便抽取了 128 名年轻人(平均年龄为 24.9 岁)作为样本,研究了基于神经心理学表现的社会认知测量--高级临床解决方案-社会认知(ACS-SP)--与文化取向(由个人主义-集体主义量表(ICS)评估)和英语口语(由广域成就测验-4(WRAT4)的口语单词发音测量评估)之间的关系:结果表明,WRAT4 分数越高,在 ACS-SP 的所有社会认知测量中的表现就越好。在控制了英语口语中的这些相关性后,部分相关性将ACS-SP任务中较低的拟声词解释和情感命名得分与纵向看待社会关系的倾向联系起来,而与个人主义或集体主义取向无关。分层回归结果表明,文化取向和英语语言熟悉程度分别对ACS-SP中面部表情与拟声词匹配的成绩有特殊和独特的影响:这些发现强调了在社会认知的神经心理学研究中纳入语言和文化因素并将其置于优先地位的重要性。这些研究结果可以被看作是对扩大社会认知结构边界的有力支持,而不是局限于其当前的理论框架,即西方的、受过教育的、工业化的、富裕的和民主的(WEIRD)价值观、习俗和认识论。
{"title":"Exploring cultural contributions to the neuropsychology of social cognition: the advanced clinical solutions.","authors":"Paul G Nestor, Ashley-Ann Woodhull","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348212","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348212","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Culture and social cognition are deeply intertwined, yet how this rich intersectionality is expressed neuropsychologically remains an important question.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In a convenience sample of 128 young adults (mean age = 24.9 years) recruited from a majority-minority urban university, we examined performance-based neuropsychological measures of social cognition, the Advanced Clinical Solutions-Social Perception (ACS-SP), in relation to both cultural orientation, as assessed by the Individualism-Collectivism Scale (ICS) and spoken English language, as assessed by the oral word pronunciation measure of the Wide Range Achievement Test-4 (WRAT4).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results indicated higher WRAT4 scores correlated with better performance across all ACS-SP measures of social cognition. Controlling for these associations in spoken English, partial correlations linked lower scores across both prosody interpretation and affect naming ACS-SP tasks with a propensity to view social relationships vertically, irrespective of individualistic or collectivistic orientations. Hierarchical regression results showed that cultural orientation and English-language familiarity each specifically and uniquely contributed to ACS-SP performance for matching prosody with facial expressions.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings underscore the importance of incorporating and prioritizing both language and cultural factors in neuropsychological studies of social cognition. They may be viewed as offering strong support for expanding the boundaries of the construct of social cognition beyond its current theoretical framework of one that privileges Western, educated, industralized, rich and democratic (WEIRD) values, customs, and epistemologies.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"303-315"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140876566","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toward a cross-cultural understanding of intraindividual variability metrics. 实现对个体内部变异度量的跨文化理解。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2328870
Bonnie M Scott, Donald R Royall, Jared F Benge, Robin C Hilsabeck

Objective: Compare the construct validity and predictive utility of cognitive intraindividual variability (IIV) in a sample of community-dwelling Hispanic and non-Hispanic white (NHW) older adults.

Methods: The present study included annual data from 651 older adult control participants (Hispanic = 293; NHW = 358) enrolled in the Texas Alzheimer's Research and Care Consortium for at least 5 years. Mean composite z-scores were calculated for attention, language, memory, and executive domains. IIV was calculated as was the standard deviation both within (IIV-Within) and between (IIV-Between) these domains.

Results: At baseline, NHW individuals obtained significantly higher mean scores in each domain than their Hispanic counterparts. They also showed significantly greater variability within and between domains, except for IIV-Within the language domain which was significantly larger in the Hispanic group. IIV-Between domains was driven primarily by IIV-Within the executive function domain in the NHW cohort and by IIV-Within the language domain in the Hispanic cohort. In both groups, the addition of IIV-Within and IIV-Between cognitive domains at baseline significantly improved prediction of global cognitive status after 5 years above and beyond demographic characteristics, genetic and cardiovascular risk. However, IIV-Between domains was the strongest predictor in the NHW group, while IIV-Within the attention domain was the strongest predictor in the Hispanic group.

Conclusions: Findings suggest that, while IIV-Between domains is a promising adjunctive method for predicting future cognitive decline, its construct validity and predictive utility varies based on ethnic group.

目的比较在社区居住的西班牙裔和非西班牙裔白人(NHW)老年人样本中认知个体内变异性(IIV)的构建有效性和预测效用:本研究纳入了 651 名老年人对照组参与者(西班牙裔 = 293 人;非西班牙裔白人 = 358 人)的年度数据,这些参与者已加入德克萨斯州阿尔茨海默氏症研究与护理联合会至少 5 年。计算了注意力、语言、记忆和执行领域的平均综合 Z 值。计算了这些领域内(IIV-Within)和领域间(IIV-Between)的标准偏差:结果:基线时,非华裔女性在每个领域的平均得分都明显高于西班牙裔女性。他们在各领域内和领域之间的变异性也明显更大,只有语言领域内的 IIV 变异性在西班牙裔群体中明显更大。在非华裔组群中,执行功能领域内的 IIV 主要驱动了领域间的 IIV,而在西班牙裔组群中,语言领域内的 IIV 主要驱动了领域间的 IIV。在这两组人群中,在基线时增加 IIV-Within 和 IIV-Between 认知功能域可显著提高对 5 年后总体认知状况的预测能力,而不局限于人口统计学特征、遗传和心血管风险。然而,IIV-Between 领域对非高危人群的预测作用最强,而 IIV-Within 的注意力领域对西班牙裔人群的预测作用最强:研究结果表明,虽然 IIV-Between domains 是预测未来认知能力下降的一种很有前景的辅助方法,但其构建有效性和预测效用因种族群体而异。
{"title":"Toward a cross-cultural understanding of intraindividual variability metrics.","authors":"Bonnie M Scott, Donald R Royall, Jared F Benge, Robin C Hilsabeck","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2328870","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2328870","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Compare the construct validity and predictive utility of cognitive intraindividual variability (IIV) in a sample of community-dwelling Hispanic and non-Hispanic white (NHW) older adults.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>The present study included annual data from 651 older adult control participants (Hispanic = 293; NHW = 358) enrolled in the Texas Alzheimer's Research and Care Consortium for at least 5 years. Mean composite z-scores were calculated for attention, language, memory, and executive domains. IIV was calculated as was the standard deviation both within (IIV-Within) and between (IIV-Between) these domains.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>At baseline, NHW individuals obtained significantly higher mean scores in each domain than their Hispanic counterparts. They also showed significantly greater variability within and between domains, except for IIV-Within the language domain which was significantly larger in the Hispanic group. IIV-Between domains was driven primarily by IIV-Within the executive function domain in the NHW cohort and by IIV-Within the language domain in the Hispanic cohort. In both groups, the addition of IIV-Within and IIV-Between cognitive domains at baseline significantly improved prediction of global cognitive status after 5 years above and beyond demographic characteristics, genetic and cardiovascular risk. However, IIV-Between domains was the strongest predictor in the NHW group, while IIV-Within the attention domain was the strongest predictor in the Hispanic group.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings suggest that, while IIV-Between domains is a promising adjunctive method for predicting future cognitive decline, its construct validity and predictive utility varies based on ethnic group.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"382-392"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140119620","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prospective memory performance in veterans with and without histories of mild traumatic brain injury: effect of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 genotype. 有轻度脑外伤史和无轻度脑外伤史退伍军人的前瞻性记忆表现:载脂蛋白 E (APOE) ε4基因型的影响。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-08 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2351205
Jennifer S Adler, Erin D Ozturk, Victoria C Merritt, Lisa Delano-Wood, Dawn M Schiehser, Mark W Bondi, Monica T Ly, Adan Ton-Loy, Scott F Sorg

Objective: Identifying factors that moderate cognitive outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) is crucial. Prospective memory (PM) is a cognitive domain of interest in mTBI recovery as it may be especially sensitive to TBI-related changes. Since studies show that genetic status - particularly possession of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele - can modify PM performance, we investigated associations between mTBI status and APOE-ε4 genotype on PM performance in a well-characterized sample of Veterans with neurotrauma histories.

Methods: 59 Veterans (mTBI = 33, Military Controls [MCs] = 26; age range: 24-50; average years post-injury = 10.41) underwent a structured clinical interview, neuropsychological assessment, and genotyping. The Memory for Intentions Test (MIST) measured PM across multiple subscales. ANCOVAs, adjusting for age and posttraumatic stress symptoms, tested the effects of mTBI status (mTBI vs. MC) and ε4 status (ε4+ vs. ε4-) on MIST scores.

Results: Veterans with mTBI history performed more poorly compared to MCs on the MIST 15-min delay (p=.002, ηp2 =.160), Time Cue (p = .003, ηp2 =.157), and PM Total (p = .016, ηp2 =.102). Those with at least one copy of the ε4 allele performed more poorly compared to ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay (p = .011, ηp2 =.113) and PM Total (p = .048, ηp2 = .071). No significant interactions were observed between mTBI and APOE-ε4 status on MIST outcomes (ps>.25). Within the mTBI group, APOE-ε4+ Veterans performed worse than APOE-ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay subscale (p = .031, ηp2 = .150).

Conclusions: mTBI history and APOE-ε4 genotype status were independently associated with worse PM performance compared to those without head injury histories or possession of the APOE-e4 genotype. Performance on the MIST 15-min delay was worse in Veterans with both risk factors (mTBI history and APOE-ε4 positivity). Findings suggest that genetic status may modify outcomes even in relatively young Veterans with mTBI histories. Future research examining longitudinal associations and links to neuroimaging and biomarker data are needed.

目的:确定影响轻度创伤性脑损伤(mTBI)后认知结果的因素至关重要。前瞻性记忆(PM)是轻微创伤性脑损伤恢复中一个值得关注的认知领域,因为它可能对创伤性脑损伤相关的变化特别敏感。由于研究表明遗传状态--尤其是拥有脂蛋白E(APOE)ε4等位基因--会改变前瞻性记忆的表现,因此我们在具有神经创伤史的退伍军人样本中调查了mTBI状态和APOE-ε4基因型对前瞻性记忆表现的影响。方法:59名退伍军人(mTBI=33人,军事对照组[MCs]=26人;年龄范围:24-50岁;伤后平均年数=10.41年)接受了结构化临床访谈、神经心理学评估和基因分型。意向记忆测试(MIST)通过多个分量表测量 PM。在对年龄和创伤后应激症状进行调整后,方差分析检验了mTBI状态(mTBI vs. MC)和ε4状态(ε4+ vs. ε4-)对MIST得分的影响:与 MCs 相比,有 mTBI 史的退伍军人在 MIST 15 分钟延迟(p=.002,ηp2 =.160)、时间线索(p=.003,ηp2 =.157)和 PM 总分(p=.016,ηp2 =.102)上的表现更差。与ε4-退伍军人相比,至少有一个ε4等位基因拷贝的人在MIST 15分钟延迟(p = .011,ηp2 = .113)和PM总分(p = .048,ηp2 = .071)上的表现更差。mTBI 和 APOE-ε4 状态之间在 MIST 结果上没有观察到明显的交互作用(ps>.25)。在mTBI组中,APOE-ε4+退伍军人在MIST 15分钟延迟分量表上的表现比APOE-ε4-退伍军人差(p = .031,ηp2 = .150)。结论:与没有头部损伤史或拥有APOE-e4基因型的退伍军人相比,mTBI史和APOE-ε4基因型状态与较差的PM表现独立相关。同时具有这两种风险因素(mTBI史和APOE-ε4阳性)的退伍军人在MIST 15分钟延迟中的表现更差。研究结果表明,即使是有 mTBI 史的相对年轻的退伍军人,其遗传状况也可能会改变其结果。未来需要对纵向关联以及与神经影像学和生物标记物数据的联系进行研究。
{"title":"Prospective memory performance in veterans with and without histories of mild traumatic brain injury: effect of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 genotype.","authors":"Jennifer S Adler, Erin D Ozturk, Victoria C Merritt, Lisa Delano-Wood, Dawn M Schiehser, Mark W Bondi, Monica T Ly, Adan Ton-Loy, Scott F Sorg","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2351205","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2351205","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>Identifying factors that moderate cognitive outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) is crucial. Prospective memory (PM) is a cognitive domain of interest in mTBI recovery as it may be especially sensitive to TBI-related changes. Since studies show that genetic status - particularly possession of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) ε4 allele - can modify PM performance, we investigated associations between mTBI status and APOE-ε4 genotype on PM performance in a well-characterized sample of Veterans with neurotrauma histories.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>59 Veterans (mTBI = 33, Military Controls [MCs] = 26; age range: 24-50; average years post-injury = 10.41) underwent a structured clinical interview, neuropsychological assessment, and genotyping. The Memory for Intentions Test (MIST) measured PM across multiple subscales. ANCOVAs, adjusting for age and posttraumatic stress symptoms, tested the effects of mTBI status (mTBI vs. MC) and ε4 status (ε4+ vs. ε4-) on MIST scores.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Veterans with mTBI history performed more poorly compared to MCs on the MIST 15-min delay (<i>p</i>=.002, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.160), Time Cue (<i>p</i> = .003, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.157), and PM Total (<i>p</i> = .016, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.102). Those with at least one copy of the ε4 allele performed more poorly compared to ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay (<i>p</i> = .011, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> =.113) and PM Total (<i>p</i> = .048, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = .071). No significant interactions were observed between mTBI and APOE-ε4 status on MIST outcomes (<i>p</i>s>.25). Within the mTBI group, APOE-ε4+ Veterans performed worse than APOE-ε4- Veterans on the MIST 15-min delay subscale (<i>p</i> = .031, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = .150).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>mTBI history and APOE-ε4 genotype status were independently associated with worse PM performance compared to those without head injury histories or possession of the APOE-e4 genotype. Performance on the MIST 15-min delay was worse in Veterans with both risk factors (mTBI history and APOE-ε4 positivity). Findings suggest that genetic status may modify outcomes even in relatively young Veterans with mTBI histories. Future research examining longitudinal associations and links to neuroimaging and biomarker data are needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"352-363"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140876567","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development and validation of an episodic memory measure in the Mobile Toolbox (MTB): Arranging Pictures. 开发并验证移动工具箱(MTB)中的外显记忆测量方法:排列图片。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2353945
Stephanie Ruth Young, Elizabeth M Dworak, Miriam A Novack, Aaron J Kaat, Hubert Adam, Cindy J Nowinski, Zahra Hosseinian, Jerry Slotkin, Jordan Stoeger, Saki Amagai, Maria Varela Diaz, Anyelo Almonte Correa, Keith Alperin, Larsson Omberg, Michael Kellen, Monica R Camacho, Bernard Landavazo, Rachel L Nosheny, Michael W Weiner, Richard Gershon

Introduction: Arranging Pictures is a new episodic memory test based on the NIH Toolbox (NIHTB) Picture Sequence Memory measure and optimized for self-administration on a personal smartphone within the Mobile Toolbox (MTB). We describe evidence from three distinct validation studies.

Method: In Study 1, 92 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures on study-provided smartphones in the lab and were administered external measures of similar and dissimilar constructs by trained examiners to assess validity under controlled circumstances. In Study 2, 1,021 participants completed the external measures in the lab and self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely on their personal smartphones to assess validity in real-world contexts. In Study 3, 141 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely twice with a two-week delay on personal iOS smartphones to assess test-retest reliability and practice effects.

Results: Internal consistency was good across samples (ρxx = .80 to .85, p < .001). Test-retest reliability was marginal (ICC = .49, p < .001) and there were significant practice effects after a two-week delay (ΔM = 3.21 (95% CI [2.56, 3.88]). As expected, correlations with convergent measures were significant and moderate to large in magnitude (ρ = .44 to .76, p < .001), while correlations with discriminant measures were small (ρ = .23 to .27, p < .05) or nonsignificant. Scores demonstrated significant negative correlations with age (ρ = -.32 to -.21, p < .001). Mean performance was slightly higher in the iOS compared to the Android group (MiOS = 18.80, NiOS = 635; MAndroid = 17.11, NAndroid = 386; t(757.73) = 4.17, p < .001), but device type did not significantly influence the psychometric properties of the measure. Indicators of potential cheating were mixed; average scores were significantly higher in the remote samples (F(2, 850) = 11.415, p < .001), but there were not significantly more perfect scores.

Conclusion: The MTB Arranging Pictures measure demonstrated evidence of reliability and validity when self-administered on personal device. Future research should examine the potential for cheating in remote settings and the properties of the measure in clinical samples.

简介排列图片是一种新的外显记忆测试,它以美国国立卫生研究院工具箱(NIHTB)图片序列记忆测量为基础,并经过优化,可在移动工具箱(MTB)内的个人智能手机上进行自我管理。我们介绍了三项不同验证研究的证据:在研究 1 中,92 名参与者在实验室中使用研究人员提供的智能手机进行了 "排列图片 "自我管理,并由训练有素的考官对相似和不相似的结构进行了外部测量,以评估在受控情况下的有效性。在研究 2 中,1,021 名参与者在实验室中完成了外部测量,并在个人智能手机上进行了 "排列图片 "的远程自我管理,以评估在真实环境中的有效性。在研究3中,141名参与者在个人iOS智能手机上进行了2次远程《排列图片》自测,每次延迟两周,以评估重测可靠性和练习效果:结果:不同样本的内部一致性良好(ρxx = .80 至 .85,p p p p iOS = 18.80,NiOS = 635;MAndroid = 17.11,NAndroid = 386;t(757.73) = 4.17,p p p 结论:MTB "排列图片 "测验的内部一致性良好:在个人设备上进行自我管理时,MTB 排列图片测量结果显示了可靠性和有效性。未来的研究应考察在远程环境中作弊的可能性以及该测量方法在临床样本中的特性。
{"title":"Development and validation of an episodic memory measure in the Mobile Toolbox (MTB): Arranging Pictures.","authors":"Stephanie Ruth Young, Elizabeth M Dworak, Miriam A Novack, Aaron J Kaat, Hubert Adam, Cindy J Nowinski, Zahra Hosseinian, Jerry Slotkin, Jordan Stoeger, Saki Amagai, Maria Varela Diaz, Anyelo Almonte Correa, Keith Alperin, Larsson Omberg, Michael Kellen, Monica R Camacho, Bernard Landavazo, Rachel L Nosheny, Michael W Weiner, Richard Gershon","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2353945","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2353945","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Arranging Pictures is a new episodic memory test based on the NIH Toolbox (NIHTB) Picture Sequence Memory measure and optimized for self-administration on a personal smartphone within the Mobile Toolbox (MTB). We describe evidence from three distinct validation studies.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In Study 1, 92 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures on study-provided smartphones in the lab and were administered external measures of similar and dissimilar constructs by trained examiners to assess validity under controlled circumstances. In Study 2, 1,021 participants completed the external measures in the lab and self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely on their personal smartphones to assess validity in real-world contexts. In Study 3, 141 participants self-administered Arranging Pictures remotely twice with a two-week delay on personal iOS smartphones to assess test-retest reliability and practice effects.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Internal consistency was good across samples (ρ<sub>xx</sub> = .80 to .85, <i>p</i> < .001). Test-retest reliability was marginal (ICC = .49, <i>p</i> < .001) and there were significant practice effects after a two-week delay (ΔM = 3.21 (95% CI [2.56, 3.88]). As expected, correlations with convergent measures were significant and moderate to large in magnitude (ρ = .44 to .76, <i>p</i> < .001), while correlations with discriminant measures were small (ρ = .23 to .27, <i>p</i> < .05) or nonsignificant. Scores demonstrated significant negative correlations with age (ρ = -.32 to -.21, <i>p</i> < .001). Mean performance was slightly higher in the iOS compared to the Android group (M<sub>iOS</sub> = 18.80, N<sub>iOS</sub> = 635; M<sub>Android</sub> = 17.11, N<sub>Android</sub> = 386; t(757.73) = 4.17, <i>p</i> < .001), but device type did not significantly influence the psychometric properties of the measure. Indicators of potential cheating were mixed; average scores were significantly higher in the remote samples (F(2, 850) = 11.415, <i>p</i> < .001), but there were not significantly more perfect scores.</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The MTB Arranging Pictures measure demonstrated evidence of reliability and validity when self-administered on personal device. Future research should examine the potential for cheating in remote settings and the properties of the measure in clinical samples.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"364-373"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11309919/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140957318","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality traits and personality problems in Korsakoff syndrome patients 科萨科夫综合征患者的人格特征和人格问题
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-19 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2344840
Ineke Roelfina Hendrika de Vries, Erik Oudman, Albert Postma
Personality research is of relevance because it provides insights into the psychological strengths and vulnerabilities of a person. Korsakoff’s syndrome (KS) is a severe neuropsychiatric disorder f...
人格研究之所以具有现实意义,是因为它能让我们深入了解一个人的心理优势和弱点。科萨科夫综合征(Korsakoff's Syndrome,简称KS)是一种严重的神经精神障碍,主要表现为...
{"title":"Personality traits and personality problems in Korsakoff syndrome patients","authors":"Ineke Roelfina Hendrika de Vries, Erik Oudman, Albert Postma","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2344840","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2024.2344840","url":null,"abstract":"Personality research is of relevance because it provides insights into the psychological strengths and vulnerabilities of a person. Korsakoff’s syndrome (KS) is a severe neuropsychiatric disorder f...","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":"70 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140630093","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Personality traits related to cognitive functioning in patients with functional neurological disorder 与功能性神经紊乱患者认知功能相关的人格特质
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-16 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2335599
Lars de Vroege, Timothy L. van Dijl, Jarinne E. Woudstra-de Jong, Arjan C. Videler, Willem J. Kop
Cognitive symptoms are prevalent in patients with functional neurological disorder (FND). Several studies have suggested that personality traits such as neuroticism may play a pivotal role in the d...
功能性神经紊乱(FND)患者普遍存在认知症状。一些研究表明,神经质等人格特质可能在功能性神经症的认知症状中起着关键作用。
{"title":"Personality traits related to cognitive functioning in patients with functional neurological disorder","authors":"Lars de Vroege, Timothy L. van Dijl, Jarinne E. Woudstra-de Jong, Arjan C. Videler, Willem J. Kop","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2335599","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2024.2335599","url":null,"abstract":"Cognitive symptoms are prevalent in patients with functional neurological disorder (FND). Several studies have suggested that personality traits such as neuroticism may play a pivotal role in the d...","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":"94 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140599707","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Ecological validity of common behavioral decision making tasks: evidence across two samples 常见行为决策任务的生态有效性:两个样本的证据
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2337759
Melissa T. Buelow, Bradley M. Okdie, Jennifer M. Kowalsky
Clinicians and scholars routinely use behavioral decision tasks to assess real-world decision making capabilities. However, many common behavioral decision making tasks lack data on the extent to w...
临床医生和学者经常使用行为决策任务来评估现实世界的决策能力。然而,许多常见的行为决策任务都缺乏有关...
{"title":"Ecological validity of common behavioral decision making tasks: evidence across two samples","authors":"Melissa T. Buelow, Bradley M. Okdie, Jennifer M. Kowalsky","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2337759","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2024.2337759","url":null,"abstract":"Clinicians and scholars routinely use behavioral decision tasks to assess real-world decision making capabilities. However, many common behavioral decision making tasks lack data on the extent to w...","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":"18 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140599714","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Atypical semantic cognition in schizotypal personality disorder and borderline personality disorder. 分裂型人格障碍和边缘型人格障碍的非典型语义认知。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-04 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2340813
Lea S Petersen, Martin Vestergaard, Maria W Meisner, Malene Foldager, Erik Simonsen

Increased schizotypal traits have previously been associated with atypical semantic cognition in community samples. However, no study has yet examined whether adults diagnosed with schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) display atypical semantic fluency and memory. We hypothesized that 24 adults diagnosed with SPD would name more idiosyncratic words on the semantic fluency task and show decreased semantic recall for animal and fruit category words compared with 29 participants with borderline personality disorder (BPD) and a community sample of 96 age-matched controls. We examined whether atypical semantic cognition was specifically associated with disorganized and eccentric speech and thinking, or more broadly with pathological personality traits and personality functioning. Our main hypothesis was confirmed, as the SPD participants named more idiosyncratic words and recalled fewer semantically related words compared with controls. Surprisingly, participants with BPD likewise named more atypical words compared with controls. More idiosyncratic semantic fluency was associated with more eccentric speech and thinking. Increased idiosyncratic semantic fluency and reduced semantic recall were both coupled to increased detachment and lowered personality functioning, while reduced semantic recall further was related to increased interpersonal problems. Our findings suggest that persons with SPD, and to a lesser degree BPD, show atypical semantic cognition, which is associated with eccentric speech and thinking, and more broadly with impaired personality function, social withdrawal, and emotional flatness. The idiosyncratic semantic cognition may worsen difficulties with social reciprocity seen in SPD and BPD.

在社区样本中,精神分裂型人格特征的增加与非典型语义认知有关。然而,目前还没有研究表明被诊断患有精神分裂型人格障碍(SPD)的成年人是否会表现出非典型语义流畅性和记忆力。我们假设,与 29 名边缘型人格障碍(BPD)患者和 96 名年龄匹配的社区对照组样本相比,24 名被诊断患有分裂型人格障碍的成年人在语义流畅性任务中会说出更多的特异性词汇,并且对动物和水果类词汇的语义记忆会有所下降。我们研究了非典型语义认知是否特别与言语和思维的无序性和古怪性有关,或者更广泛地说,是否与病态人格特征和人格功能有关。我们的主要假设得到了证实,因为与对照组相比,SPD 参与者说出了更多的特异词,而回忆起的语义相关词却更少。令人惊讶的是,与对照组相比,患有 BPD 的参与者同样说出了更多非典型词语。更多的特异语义流畅性与更古怪的言语和思维有关。特异语义流畅性的增加和语义回忆的减少都与疏离感的增加和人格功能的降低有关,而语义回忆的减少又与人际关系问题的增加有关。我们的研究结果表明,SPD(其次是BPD)患者表现出非典型语义认知,这与古怪的言语和思维有关,更广泛地说,与人格功能受损、社交退缩和情感平淡有关。特异的语义认知可能会加重 SPD 和 BPD 患者的社会互惠困难。
{"title":"Atypical semantic cognition in schizotypal personality disorder and borderline personality disorder.","authors":"Lea S Petersen, Martin Vestergaard, Maria W Meisner, Malene Foldager, Erik Simonsen","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2340813","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2340813","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Increased schizotypal traits have previously been associated with atypical semantic cognition in community samples. However, no study has yet examined whether adults diagnosed with schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) display atypical semantic fluency and memory. We hypothesized that 24 adults diagnosed with SPD would name more idiosyncratic words on the semantic fluency task and show decreased semantic recall for animal and fruit category words compared with 29 participants with borderline personality disorder (BPD) and a community sample of 96 age-matched controls. We examined whether atypical semantic cognition was specifically associated with disorganized and eccentric speech and thinking, or more broadly with pathological personality traits and personality functioning. Our main hypothesis was confirmed, as the SPD participants named more idiosyncratic words and recalled fewer semantically related words compared with controls. Surprisingly, participants with BPD likewise named more atypical words compared with controls. More idiosyncratic semantic fluency was associated with more eccentric speech and thinking. Increased idiosyncratic semantic fluency and reduced semantic recall were both coupled to increased detachment and lowered personality functioning, while reduced semantic recall further was related to increased interpersonal problems. Our findings suggest that persons with SPD, and to a lesser degree BPD, show atypical semantic cognition, which is associated with eccentric speech and thinking, and more broadly with impaired personality function, social withdrawal, and emotional flatness. The idiosyncratic semantic cognition may worsen difficulties with social reciprocity seen in SPD and BPD.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"218-232"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855337","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between objective and subjective executive function in Parkinson's disease. 帕金森病患者客观和主观执行功能之间的关系。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-09 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2340812
Molly Split, Celina Pluim McDowell, Francesca V Lopez, Erin Almklov, J Vincent Filoteo, Stephanie Lessig, Irene Litvan, Dawn M Schiehser

Introduction: Difficulties in executive functioning (EF) are common in PD; however, the relationship between subjective and objective EF is unclear. Understanding this relationship could help guide clinical EF assessment. This study examined the relationship between subjective self-reported EF (SEF) and objective EF (OEF) and predictors of SEF-OEF discrepancies in PD.

Method: One-hundred and sixteen non-demented PD participants completed measures of OEF (i.e. problem-solving, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, and working memory) and SEF (Frontal Systems Behavior Scale-Self Executive Dysfunction Subscale). Pearson bivariate correlations and linear regressions were performed to examine the relationship between SEF and OEF and the non-motor symptoms (e.g. mood, fatigue), demographic, and PD characteristic (e.g. MCI status) predictors of discrepancies between OEF and SEF (|OEF minus SEF scores|). Correlates of under-, over-, and accurate-reporting were also explored.

Results: Greater SEF complaints and worse OEF were significantly associated (β =.200, p = .009) and 64% of participants accurately identified their level of OEF abilities. Fewer years of education and greater symptoms of depression, anxiety, and fatigue significantly correlated with greater discrepancies between OEF and SEF. Fatigue was the best predictor of EF discrepancy in the overall sample (β = .281, p = .022). Exploratory analyses revealed apathy and fatigue associated with greater under-reporting, while anxiety associated with greater over-reporting.

Conclusions: SEF and OEF are significantly related in PD. Approximately 64% of non-demented persons with PD accurately reported their EF skill level, while 28% under-reported and 8% over-reported. SEF-OEF discrepancies were predicted by fatigue in the overall sample. Preliminary evidence suggests reduced apathy and fatigue symptoms relate to more under-reporting, while anxiety relates to greater over-reporting. Given the prevalence of these non-motor symptoms in PD, it is important to carefully consider them when assessing EF in PD.

前言执行功能(EF)障碍在帕金森病中很常见,但主观和客观 EF 之间的关系尚不清楚。了解这种关系有助于指导临床执行功能评估。本研究探讨了主观自我报告的EF(SEF)与客观EF(OEF)之间的关系,以及PD患者SEF-OEF差异的预测因素:116名非痴呆症患者完成了OEF(即解决问题、认知灵活性、抑制和工作记忆)和SEF(额叶系统行为量表-自我执行功能障碍分量表)的测量。为了研究 SEF 和 OEF 与非运动症状(如情绪、疲劳)、人口统计学和帕金森病特征(如 MCI 状态)之间的关系,对 OEF 和 SEF 之间的差异(|OEF 分数减 SEF 分数|)的预测因素进行了皮尔逊双变量相关和线性回归。研究还探讨了少报、多报和准确报告的相关性:结果:SEF投诉较多与OEF较差有显著相关性(β=.200,p=.009),64%的参与者能准确识别自己的OEF能力水平。受教育年限越短,抑郁、焦虑和疲劳症状越严重,与 OEF 和 SEF 之间的差异越大明显相关。在整个样本中,疲劳是预测EF差异的最佳指标(β = .281, p = .022)。探索性分析表明,冷漠和疲劳与更大程度的少报有关,而焦虑则与更大程度的多报有关:结论:SEF和OEF在帕金森病中具有显著相关性。约64%的非痴呆症患者准确报告了他们的EF技能水平,28%的患者报告不足,8%的患者报告过多。在整个样本中,SEF-OEF差异是由疲劳所预测的。初步证据表明,冷漠和疲劳症状的减轻与更多的低报有关,而焦虑则与更多的高报有关。鉴于这些非运动症状在帕金森病中的普遍性,在评估帕金森病患者的EF时仔细考虑这些症状非常重要。
{"title":"The relationship between objective and subjective executive function in Parkinson's disease.","authors":"Molly Split, Celina Pluim McDowell, Francesca V Lopez, Erin Almklov, J Vincent Filoteo, Stephanie Lessig, Irene Litvan, Dawn M Schiehser","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2340812","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2340812","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Difficulties in executive functioning (EF) are common in PD; however, the relationship between subjective and objective EF is unclear. Understanding this relationship could help guide clinical EF assessment. This study examined the relationship between subjective self-reported EF (SEF) and objective EF (OEF) and predictors of SEF-OEF discrepancies in PD.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>One-hundred and sixteen non-demented PD participants completed measures of OEF (i.e. problem-solving, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, and working memory) and SEF (Frontal Systems Behavior Scale-Self Executive Dysfunction Subscale). Pearson bivariate correlations and linear regressions were performed to examine the relationship between SEF and OEF and the non-motor symptoms (e.g. mood, fatigue), demographic, and PD characteristic (e.g. MCI status) predictors of discrepancies between OEF and SEF (|OEF minus SEF scores|). Correlates of under-, over-, and accurate-reporting were also explored.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Greater SEF complaints and worse OEF were significantly associated (<i>β</i> =.200, <i>p</i> = .009) and 64% of participants accurately identified their level of OEF abilities. Fewer years of education and greater symptoms of depression, anxiety, and fatigue significantly correlated with greater discrepancies between OEF and SEF. Fatigue was the best predictor of EF discrepancy in the overall sample (β = .281, <i>p</i> = .022). Exploratory analyses revealed apathy and fatigue associated with greater under-reporting, while anxiety associated with greater over-reporting.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>SEF and OEF are significantly related in PD. Approximately 64% of non-demented persons with PD accurately reported their EF skill level, while 28% under-reported and 8% over-reported. SEF-OEF discrepancies were predicted by fatigue in the overall sample. Preliminary evidence suggests reduced apathy and fatigue symptoms relate to more under-reporting, while anxiety relates to greater over-reporting. Given the prevalence of these non-motor symptoms in PD, it is important to carefully consider them when assessing EF in PD.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"207-217"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140898494","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1