首页 > 最新文献

Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology最新文献

英文 中文
Effect of processing speed and memory performance on classification accuracy of the dot counting test in a mixed neuropsychiatric sample. 处理速度和记忆能力对混合神经精神病样本点计数测试分类准确性的影响。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-07 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2363978
Matthew S Phillips, Amanda M Wisinger, Brian M Cerny, Humza Khan, Fini Chang, Ka Yin Phoebe Tse, Gabriel P Ovsiew, Zachary J Resch, Greg Shapiro, Jason R Soble, Kyle J Jennette

Objective: This study examined the impact of impairment in two specific cognitive abilities, processing speed and memory, on Dot Counting Test (DCT) classification accuracy by evaluating performance validity classification accuracy across cognitively unimpaired, single-domain impairment, and multidomain impairment subgroups within a mixed clinical sample.

Method: Cross-sectional data were analyzed from 348 adult outpatients classified as valid (n = 284) or invalid (n = 64) based on four independent criterion performance validity tests (PVTs). Unimpaired (n = 164), single-domain processing speed impairment (n = 24), single-domain memory impairment (n = 53), and multidomain processing speed and memory impairment (n = 43) clinical subgroups were established among the valid group. Both the traditional DCT E-score and unrounded E-score were examined.

Results: Overall, the DCT demonstrated acceptable to excellent classification accuracy across the unimpaired (area under the curve [AUC] traditional E-score=.855; unrounded E-score=.855) and single-domain impairment groups (traditional E-score AUCs = .690-.754; unrounded E-score AUCs = .692-747). However, it did not reliably discriminate the multidomain processing speed and memory impairment group from the invalid performers (traditional and unrounded E-scores AUC = .557).

Conclusions: Findings support the DCT as a non-memory-based freestanding PVT for use with single-domain cognitive impairment, with traditional E-score ≥17 (unrounded E-score ≥16.95) recommended for those with memory impairment and traditional E-score ≥19 (unrounded ≥18.08) with processing speed impairment. Moreover, results replicated previously established optimal cutoffs for unimpaired groups using both the traditional (≥14) and unrounded (≥13.84) E-scores. However, the DCT did not reliably discriminate between invalid performance and multidomain cognitive impairment, indicating caution is warranted when using the DCT with patients suspected of greater cognitive impairment.

研究目的本研究通过评估混合临床样本中认知能力未受损、单领域受损和多领域受损亚组的表现有效性分类准确性,研究了处理速度和记忆力这两种特定认知能力受损对点数计数测验(DCT)分类准确性的影响:对 348 名成年门诊患者的横断面数据进行分析,根据四项独立的标准性能效度测试 (PVT) 将其分为有效(284 人)或无效(64 人)。在有效组中建立了未受损(164 人)、单域处理速度受损(24 人)、单域记忆受损(53 人)和多域处理速度和记忆受损(43 人)临床亚组。对传统的 DCT E 分数和非整数 E 分数进行了研究:总体而言,DCT 在未受损组(曲线下面积[AUC]传统 E-score=.855;未四舍五入 E-score=.855)和单域受损组(传统 E-score AUC=.690-.754;未四舍五入 E-score AUC=.692-747)的分类准确性可接受到极佳。然而,它并不能可靠地区分多域处理速度和记忆障碍组与无效表现组(传统和非整型E-score AUC = .557):研究结果支持将 DCT 作为一种非基于记忆的独立 PVT 用于单域认知障碍,建议有记忆障碍的人使用传统 E-score≥17(未四舍五入 E-score≥16.95),有处理速度障碍的人使用传统 E-score≥19(未四舍五入 E-score≥18.08)。此外,使用传统 E 分数(≥14)和非四舍五入 E 分数(≥13.84)得出的结果与之前确定的未受损群体的最佳临界值相同。然而,DCT 并不能可靠地区分无效表现和多域认知障碍,这表明在对疑似有较大认知障碍的患者使用 DCT 时需要谨慎。
{"title":"Effect of processing speed and memory performance on classification accuracy of the dot counting test in a mixed neuropsychiatric sample.","authors":"Matthew S Phillips, Amanda M Wisinger, Brian M Cerny, Humza Khan, Fini Chang, Ka Yin Phoebe Tse, Gabriel P Ovsiew, Zachary J Resch, Greg Shapiro, Jason R Soble, Kyle J Jennette","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2363978","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2363978","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Objective: </strong>This study examined the impact of impairment in two specific cognitive abilities, processing speed and memory, on Dot Counting Test (DCT) classification accuracy by evaluating performance validity classification accuracy across cognitively unimpaired, single-domain impairment, and multidomain impairment subgroups within a mixed clinical sample.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Cross-sectional data were analyzed from 348 adult outpatients classified as valid (<i>n</i> = 284) or invalid (<i>n</i> = 64) based on four independent criterion performance validity tests (PVTs). Unimpaired (<i>n</i> = 164), single-domain processing speed impairment (<i>n</i> = 24), single-domain memory impairment (<i>n</i> = 53), and multidomain processing speed and memory impairment (<i>n</i> = 43) clinical subgroups were established among the valid group. Both the traditional DCT E-score and unrounded E-score were examined.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Overall, the DCT demonstrated acceptable to excellent classification accuracy across the unimpaired (area under the curve [AUC] traditional E-score=.855; unrounded E-score=.855) and single-domain impairment groups (traditional E-score AUCs = .690-.754; unrounded E-score AUCs = .692-747). However, it did not reliably discriminate the multidomain processing speed and memory impairment group from the invalid performers (traditional and unrounded E-scores AUC = .557).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Findings support the DCT as a non-memory-based freestanding PVT for use with single-domain cognitive impairment, with traditional E-score ≥17 (unrounded E-score ≥16.95) recommended for those with memory impairment and traditional E-score ≥19 (unrounded ≥18.08) with processing speed impairment. Moreover, results replicated previously established optimal cutoffs for unimpaired groups using both the traditional (≥14) and unrounded (≥13.84) E-scores. However, the DCT did not reliably discriminate between invalid performance and multidomain cognitive impairment, indicating caution is warranted when using the DCT with patients suspected of greater cognitive impairment.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"522-534"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141283849","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Extended ambulatory assessment of executive function: within-person reliability of working memory and inhibitory control tasks. 执行功能门诊延伸评估:工作记忆和抑制控制任务的人际可靠性。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-13 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2364396
Gezelle Dali, Antoinette Poulton, Li Peng Evelyn Chen, Robert Hester

Introduction: Ambulatory assessment of executive function - particularly in the form working memory (WM) - is increasingly common. Few studies to date, however, have also incorporated ambulatory measures of inhibitory control. Critically, the extended within-person reliability of ambulatory tasks tapping each of these constructs has been largely overlooked.

Method: Participants (N = 283, Mage = 23.74 years, SD = 9.04) received notifications every 3 days (for 4 weeks) to undertake ambulatory assessment versions of the n-Back and Stop-Signal Tasks (SST) via the smartphone application CheckCog. Within-person reliability of these measures was explored.

Results: Compliance ranged from 66% (for eight sessions) to 89% (for four sessions). Our results reveal significant changes in performance within the first two sessions for both the n-Back and SST, with performance remaining largely consistent across the remaining (two to eight) sessions. In terms of test-retest reliability, the ICC (C, 1) values ranged from .29 to .68 on the n-Back (with overall accuracy being .51) and .31-.73 on the SST (with stop-signal reaction time being .53).

Conclusion: The results of the current study contribute to the literature by demonstrating the reliability of brief measures of executive function - in the form of inhibitory control and WM - delivered using smartphones in participants' natural environments. Based on our findings, the CheckCog app reliability tracks baseline systematic changes in WM and response inhibition across multiple time points and for an extended period in healthy individuals.

简介:对执行功能,尤其是工作记忆(WM)的非卧床评估越来越普遍:对执行功能(尤其是工作记忆(WM))的动态评估越来越常见。然而,迄今为止,很少有研究将抑制控制的流动测量纳入其中。重要的是,对上述每种结构进行的流动任务的人内扩展可靠性在很大程度上被忽视了:方法:参与者(N = 283,年龄 = 23.74 岁,SD = 9.04)每 3 天收到一次通知(持续 4 周),通过智能手机应用程序 CheckCog 进行 n-Back 和停止信号任务 (SST) 的流动评估。对这些测量方法的人际可靠性进行了探讨:符合率从 66%(8 次)到 89%(4 次)不等。我们的结果表明,在前两次测试中,n-Back 和 SST 的成绩都发生了显著变化,而在其余(2 至 8 次)测试中,成绩基本保持一致。在测试重复可靠性方面,n-Back 的 ICC(C,1)值在 0.29 至 0.68 之间(总体准确率为 0.51),SST 的 ICC(C,1)值在 0.31 至 0.73 之间(停止信号反应时间为 0.53):本研究的结果为相关文献做出了贡献,证明了在参与者的自然环境中使用智能手机对执行功能(以抑制控制和WM的形式)进行简短测量的可靠性。根据我们的研究结果,CheckCog 应用程序可以在多个时间点和一段较长的时间内追踪健康人在 WM 和反应抑制方面的基线系统性变化。
{"title":"Extended ambulatory assessment of executive function: within-person reliability of working memory and inhibitory control tasks.","authors":"Gezelle Dali, Antoinette Poulton, Li Peng Evelyn Chen, Robert Hester","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2364396","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2364396","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Ambulatory assessment of executive function - particularly in the form working memory (WM) - is increasingly common. Few studies to date, however, have also incorporated ambulatory measures of inhibitory control. Critically, the extended within-person reliability of ambulatory tasks tapping each of these constructs has been largely overlooked.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Participants (<i>N</i> = 283, <i>M</i><sub><i>age</i></sub> = 23.74 years, <i>SD</i> = 9.04) received notifications every 3 days (for 4 weeks) to undertake ambulatory assessment versions of the <i>n</i>-Back and Stop-Signal Tasks (SST) via the smartphone application CheckCog. Within-person reliability of these measures was explored.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Compliance ranged from 66% (for eight sessions) to 89% (for four sessions). Our results reveal significant changes in performance within the first two sessions for both the <i>n</i>-Back and SST, with performance remaining largely consistent across the remaining (two to eight) sessions. In terms of test-retest reliability, the ICC (C, 1) values ranged from .29 to .68 on the <i>n</i>-Back (with overall accuracy being .51) and .31-.73 on the SST (with stop-signal reaction time being .53).</p><p><strong>Conclusion: </strong>The results of the current study contribute to the literature by demonstrating the reliability of brief measures of executive function - in the form of inhibitory control and WM - delivered using smartphones in participants' natural environments. Based on our findings, the CheckCog app reliability tracks baseline systematic changes in WM and response inhibition across multiple time points and for an extended period in healthy individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"436-448"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141310807","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of false cognitive feedback on subsequent cognitive task performance. 错误认知反馈对后续认知任务表现的影响
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2360229
Piper M Clark, Amy B Brunell, Melissa T Buelow

Introduction: Previous research has found beliefs about oneself and one's own abilities may have the potential to affect subsequent performance on a particular task. Additionally, providing false feedback about a particular characteristic or even about overall cognitive abilities may also affect performance on later tasks. However, it is unclear to what extent false positive or negative feedback about cognition will affect subsequent executive function task performance. In the present series of studies, we examined whether receiving negative false feedback about cognition would affect subsequent decision making and other executive function task performance.

Method: In Study 1, the participants (n = 115) received false feedback that they were either high or low in creative intelligence before completing a series of decision making tasks. In Study 2, the participants (n = 146) completed a similar false feedback paradigm before completing assessments of a range of executive functions.

Results: Across studies, we found limited evidence of a consistent pattern of how false feedback affects subsequent cognitive task performance, although receiving positive and negative feedback affected specific tasks.

Conclusions: Our results indicate that the influence of false feedback on task performance is variable and may depend on factors such as the specific task or executive function assessed. In clinical work, it is important to consider how patients may internalize feedback about their cognitive abilities, as the feedback, coupled with other factors such as level of insight, apathy, disinhibition, or prior perceptions regarding a diagnosis, may influence interpretations.

前言以往的研究发现,对自己和自己能力的信念可能会影响以后在特定任务中的表现。此外,对特定特征甚至整体认知能力的错误反馈也可能会影响以后任务的表现。然而,目前还不清楚对认知能力的错误正面或负面反馈会在多大程度上影响后续执行功能任务的表现。在本系列研究中,我们考察了收到关于认知的错误负面反馈是否会影响后续决策和其他执行功能任务的表现:在研究 1 中,被试(n = 115)在完成一系列决策任务之前收到了错误的反馈信息,即他们的创造性智力高或低。在研究2中,参与者(n = 146)在完成一系列执行功能评估之前,也完成了类似的虚假反馈范式:结果:在各项研究中,我们发现错误反馈如何影响后续认知任务表现的一致模式证据有限,尽管接受积极和消极反馈会影响特定任务:我们的研究结果表明,错误反馈对任务表现的影响是多变的,可能取决于具体任务或评估的执行功能等因素。在临床工作中,重要的是要考虑患者如何将有关其认知能力的反馈内化,因为反馈加上其他因素,如洞察力水平、冷漠、抑制或之前对诊断的看法,可能会影响解释。
{"title":"The effect of false cognitive feedback on subsequent cognitive task performance.","authors":"Piper M Clark, Amy B Brunell, Melissa T Buelow","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2360229","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2360229","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Previous research has found beliefs about oneself and one's own abilities may have the potential to affect subsequent performance on a particular task. Additionally, providing false feedback about a particular characteristic or even about overall cognitive abilities may also affect performance on later tasks. However, it is unclear to what extent false positive or negative feedback about cognition will affect subsequent executive function task performance. In the present series of studies, we examined whether receiving negative false feedback about cognition would affect subsequent decision making and other executive function task performance.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>In Study 1, the participants (<i>n</i> = 115) received false feedback that they were either high or low in creative intelligence before completing a series of decision making tasks. In Study 2, the participants (<i>n</i> = 146) completed a similar false feedback paradigm before completing assessments of a range of executive functions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Across studies, we found limited evidence of a consistent pattern of how false feedback affects subsequent cognitive task performance, although receiving positive and negative feedback affected specific tasks.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our results indicate that the influence of false feedback on task performance is variable and may depend on factors such as the specific task or executive function assessed. In clinical work, it is important to consider how patients may internalize feedback about their cognitive abilities, as the feedback, coupled with other factors such as level of insight, apathy, disinhibition, or prior perceptions regarding a diagnosis, may influence interpretations.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"422-435"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141173936","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of tDCS on emotion recognition and brain oscillations. tDCS 对情绪识别和大脑振荡的影响
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2364403
Saliha Şahintürk, Erol Yıldırım

Introduction: Emotion recognition, the ability to interpret the emotional state of individuals by looking at their facial expressions, is essential for healthy social interactions and communication. There is limited research on the effects of tDCS on emotion recognition in the literature. This study aimed to investigate the effects of anodal stimulation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), a key region for emotion recognition from facial expressions, on emotion recognition and brain oscillations.

Method: A single-blind randomized-controlled study was conducted with 54 healthy participants. Before and after brain stimulation emotion recognition tasks were administered and resting-state EEG were recorded. The changes in task performances and brain oscillations were analyzed using repeated-measures two-way ANOVA analysis.

Results: There was no significant difference in the emotion recognition tasks between groups in pre-post measurements. The changes in delta, theta, alpha, beta and gamma frequency bands in the frontal, temporal, and posterio-occipital regions, which were determined as regions of interest in resting state EEG data before and after tDCS, were compared between groups. The results showed that there was a significant difference between groups only in delta frequency before and after tDCS in the frontal and temporal regions. While an increase in delta activity was observed in the experimental group in the frontal and temporal regions, a decrease was observed in the control group.

Conclusions: The tDCS may not have improved emotion recognition because it may not have had the desired effect on the vmPFC, which is in the lower part of the prefrontal lobe. The changes in EEG frequencies observed section tDCS may be similar to those seen in some pathological processes, which could explain the lack of improvement in emotion recognition. Future studies to be carried out for better understand this effect are important.

简介情绪识别是通过观察人的面部表情来解读其情绪状态的能力,对于健康的社会交往和沟通至关重要。有关 tDCS 对情绪识别影响的文献研究有限。本研究旨在探讨通过面部表情识别情绪的关键区域--腹内侧前额叶皮层(vmPFC)的阳极刺激对情绪识别和大脑振荡的影响:方法:对 54 名健康参与者进行了单盲随机对照研究。方法:对 54 名健康参与者进行了单盲随机对照研究,在脑刺激前后分别进行了情绪识别任务,并记录了静息状态脑电图。采用重复测量双向方差分析法分析了任务表现和大脑振荡的变化:结果:在情绪识别任务的前后测量中,各组之间没有明显差异。比较了 tDCS 前后各组间额叶、颞叶和枕后区的 delta、theta、alpha、β 和 gamma 频带的变化,这些频带是静息状态脑电图数据中确定的感兴趣区。结果显示,各组之间只有额叶和颞叶区域在 tDCS 前后的 delta 频率存在显著差异。实验组额叶和颞叶区域的δ活动有所增加,而对照组则有所减少:tDCS可能没有改善情绪识别能力,因为它可能没有对位于前额叶下部的vmPFC产生预期效果。在 tDCS 部分观察到的脑电图频率变化可能与某些病理过程中出现的频率变化相似,这可能是情绪识别能力未得到改善的原因。未来的研究对于更好地理解这种效应非常重要。
{"title":"Effects of tDCS on emotion recognition and brain oscillations.","authors":"Saliha Şahintürk, Erol Yıldırım","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2364403","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2364403","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Emotion recognition, the ability to interpret the emotional state of individuals by looking at their facial expressions, is essential for healthy social interactions and communication. There is limited research on the effects of tDCS on emotion recognition in the literature. This study aimed to investigate the effects of anodal stimulation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), a key region for emotion recognition from facial expressions, on emotion recognition and brain oscillations.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A single-blind randomized-controlled study was conducted with 54 healthy participants. Before and after brain stimulation emotion recognition tasks were administered and resting-state EEG were recorded. The changes in task performances and brain oscillations were analyzed using repeated-measures two-way ANOVA analysis.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>There was no significant difference in the emotion recognition tasks between groups in pre-post measurements. The changes in delta, theta, alpha, beta and gamma frequency bands in the frontal, temporal, and posterio-occipital regions, which were determined as regions of interest in resting state EEG data before and after tDCS, were compared between groups. The results showed that there was a significant difference between groups only in delta frequency before and after tDCS in the frontal and temporal regions. While an increase in delta activity was observed in the experimental group in the frontal and temporal regions, a decrease was observed in the control group.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The tDCS may not have improved emotion recognition because it may not have had the desired effect on the vmPFC, which is in the lower part of the prefrontal lobe. The changes in EEG frequencies observed section tDCS may be similar to those seen in some pathological processes, which could explain the lack of improvement in emotion recognition. Future studies to be carried out for better understand this effect are important.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"504-521"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141296192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effects of psilocybin on cognition and emotional processing in healthy adults and adults with depression: a systematic literature review. 迷幻药对健康成年人和抑郁症成年人认知和情绪处理的影响:系统性文献综述。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2363343
Laura Ramos, Selene G Vicente

Introduction: Psilocybin, a naturally occurring serotonergic agonist in some mushroom species, has shown promise as a novel, fast-acting pharmacotherapy seeking to overcome the limitations of conventional first-line antidepressants. Studying psilocybin effects on cognition and emotional processing may help to clarify the mechanisms underlying the therapeutic potential of psilocybin and may also support studies with people suffering from depression. Thus, this review aims to provide a comprehensive overview of the current literature regarding the effects of psilocybin on these two key areas in both healthy and depressed populations.

Method: A systematic search was performed on 29 January 2024, in the PubMed, EBSCOhost, Web of Science and SCOPUS databases. After duplicates removal, study selection was conducted considering pre-specified criteria. Data extraction was then performed. The quality assessment of the studies was carried out using the Cochrane Collaboration tools for randomized (RoB 2.0) and non-randomized (ROBINS-I) controlled trials.

Results: Twenty articles were included, with 18 targeting healthy adults and two adults with depression. Results point to impairments within attentional and inhibitory processes, and improvements in the domains of creativity and social cognition in healthy individuals. In the population with depression, only cognitive flexibility and emotional recognition were affected, both being enhanced. The comparison of outcomes from both populations proved limited.

Conclusions: Psilocybin acutely alters several cognitive domains, with a localized rather than global focus, in a dose- and time-dependent manner. However, the significant methodological constraints call for further research, in the context of depression and with standardized protocols, with longitudinal studies also imperative.

介绍:迷幻药是某些蘑菇物种中天然存在的一种血清素能激动剂,作为一种新型速效药物疗法,它有望克服传统一线抗抑郁药物的局限性。研究西洛赛宾对认知和情绪处理的影响有助于阐明西洛赛宾治疗潜力的内在机制,也有助于对抑郁症患者进行研究。因此,本综述旨在全面概述目前有关迷幻药对健康和抑郁人群这两个关键领域影响的文献:于 2024 年 1 月 29 日在 PubMed、EBSCOhost、Web of Science 和 SCOPUS 数据库中进行了系统检索。去除重复内容后,根据预先规定的标准对研究进行筛选。然后进行数据提取。采用 Cochrane 协作工具对随机对照试验(RoB 2.0)和非随机对照试验(ROBINS-I)进行了研究质量评估:共纳入 20 篇文章,其中 18 篇针对健康成人,2 篇针对抑郁症成人。结果表明,健康人的注意力和抑制过程会受到影响,而创造力和社会认知领域则会有所改善。在抑郁症患者中,只有认知灵活性和情绪识别能力受到影响,而这两项能力都得到了提高。事实证明,对这两种人群的结果进行比较是有局限性的:结论:迷幻药能以剂量和时间依赖的方式急性改变多个认知领域,重点在于局部而非整体。然而,由于在方法上存在很大的局限性,因此需要在抑郁症的背景下,通过标准化的方案开展进一步的研究,同时还必须开展纵向研究。
{"title":"The effects of psilocybin on cognition and emotional processing in healthy adults and adults with depression: a systematic literature review.","authors":"Laura Ramos, Selene G Vicente","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2363343","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2363343","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Psilocybin, a naturally occurring serotonergic agonist in some mushroom species, has shown promise as a novel, fast-acting pharmacotherapy seeking to overcome the limitations of conventional first-line antidepressants. Studying psilocybin effects on cognition and emotional processing may help to clarify the mechanisms underlying the therapeutic potential of psilocybin and may also support studies with people suffering from depression. Thus, this review aims to provide a comprehensive overview of the current literature regarding the effects of psilocybin on these two key areas in both healthy and depressed populations.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>A systematic search was performed on 29 January 2024, in the PubMed, EBSCOhost, Web of Science and SCOPUS databases. After duplicates removal, study selection was conducted considering pre-specified criteria. Data extraction was then performed. The quality assessment of the studies was carried out using the Cochrane Collaboration tools for randomized (RoB 2.0) and non-randomized (ROBINS-I) controlled trials.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Twenty articles were included, with 18 targeting healthy adults and two adults with depression. Results point to impairments within attentional and inhibitory processes, and improvements in the domains of creativity and social cognition in healthy individuals. In the population with depression, only cognitive flexibility and emotional recognition were affected, both being enhanced. The comparison of outcomes from both populations proved limited.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Psilocybin acutely alters several cognitive domains, with a localized rather than global focus, in a dose- and time-dependent manner. However, the significant methodological constraints call for further research, in the context of depression and with standardized protocols, with longitudinal studies also imperative.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"393-421"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141261061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multidimensional assessment of social cognition using non-immersive virtual reality in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: an exploratory study. 利用非沉浸式虚拟现实技术对路易体痴呆症和阿尔茨海默氏症患者的社会认知进行多维度评估:一项探索性研究。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2357362
Eva-Flore Msika, Marc Verny, Bénédicte Dieudonné, Nathalie Ehrlé, Alexandre Gaston-Bellegarde, Eric Orriols, Pascale Piolino, Pauline Narme

Introduction: Few studies have focused on social cognition in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), even though some brain structures being well known as underlying social cognitive processes are directly impacted in this disease. Furthermore, social cognition processes have been mostly studied independently using evaluations with poor ecological validity. We aimed at studying the ability of a new naturalistic and multidimensional social cognition task to reveal impairments in DLB patients. We chose to compare the profile of these patients with that of Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, for which social cognition is better preserved.

Method: Fifteen patients (DLB: n = 7; AD: n = 8) and 28 healthy controls underwent the REALSoCog task. They encountered several social situations (e.g. control versus transgressions) in a non-immersive virtual city environment allowing the assessment of moral cognition, cognitive and affective theory of mind (ToM), emotional empathy and behavioral intentions.

Results: The main results showed (i) a lower ability to detect transgressions in DLB patients, particularly conventional ones, whereas moral cognition seemed better preserved in AD patients; (ii) a cognitive ToM impairment in both DLB and AD patients, while affective ToM is impaired only in DLB patients; (iii) a decreased emotional empathy specifically observed in DLB patients; (iv) more inappropriate behavioral intentions, mainly in DLB patients, but also in some AD patients.

Conclusions: This study suggests the feasibility and potential interest of the REALSoCog task in revealing social cognition deficits, particularly for DLB patients by showing different social patterns as compared to AD patients. These results offer interesting clinical perspectives to develop more naturalistic tasks in such populations and for clinical differential diagnosis. Limitations and future perspectives are discussed.

引言尽管众所周知,路易体痴呆症(DLB)的某些大脑结构会直接影响患者的社会认知过程,但很少有研究关注这种疾病的社会认知过程。此外,对社会认知过程的研究大多采用生态效度较差的独立评估。我们的目的是研究一种新的自然多维社会认知任务揭示 DLB 患者损伤的能力。我们选择将这些患者的情况与阿尔茨海默病(AD)患者的情况进行比较,后者的社会认知能力保存得更好:15 名患者(DLB:n = 7;AD:n = 8)和 28 名健康对照者接受了 REALSoCog 任务。他们在非沉浸式虚拟城市环境中遇到了几种社会情境(如控制与过失),从而对道德认知、认知和情感心智理论(ToM)、情感共鸣和行为意图进行了评估:主要结果显示:(i) DLB 患者发现越轨行为的能力较低,尤其是传统越轨行为,而注意力缺失症患者的道德认知似乎保存得更好;(ii) DLB 和注意力缺失症患者的认知心智理论(ToM)均受损,而情感心智理论(ToM)仅在 DLB 患者中受损;(iii) DLB 患者的情感共鸣明显降低;(iv) 主要在 DLB 患者中,但在一些注意力缺失症患者中,不当行为意向较多:本研究表明,REALSoCog 任务在揭示社会认知缺陷方面具有可行性和潜在的兴趣,特别是通过显示与注意力缺失症患者不同的社会模式,揭示了 DLB 患者的社会认知缺陷。这些结果为在此类人群中开发更多自然任务和临床鉴别诊断提供了有趣的临床视角。本文还讨论了研究的局限性和未来展望。
{"title":"Multidimensional assessment of social cognition using non-immersive virtual reality in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: an exploratory study.","authors":"Eva-Flore Msika, Marc Verny, Bénédicte Dieudonné, Nathalie Ehrlé, Alexandre Gaston-Bellegarde, Eric Orriols, Pascale Piolino, Pauline Narme","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2357362","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2357362","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Few studies have focused on social cognition in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), even though some brain structures being well known as underlying social cognitive processes are directly impacted in this disease. Furthermore, social cognition processes have been mostly studied independently using evaluations with poor ecological validity. We aimed at studying the ability of a new naturalistic and multidimensional social cognition task to reveal impairments in DLB patients. We chose to compare the profile of these patients with that of Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, for which social cognition is better preserved.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>Fifteen patients (DLB: <i>n</i> = 7; AD: <i>n</i> = 8) and 28 healthy controls underwent the REALSoCog task. They encountered several social situations (e.g. control versus transgressions) in a non-immersive virtual city environment allowing the assessment of moral cognition, cognitive and affective theory of mind (ToM), emotional empathy and behavioral intentions.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The main results showed (i) a lower ability to detect transgressions in DLB patients, particularly conventional ones, whereas moral cognition seemed better preserved in AD patients; (ii) a cognitive ToM impairment in both DLB and AD patients, while affective ToM is impaired only in DLB patients; (iii) a decreased emotional empathy specifically observed in DLB patients; (iv) more inappropriate behavioral intentions, mainly in DLB patients, but also in some AD patients.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>This study suggests the feasibility and potential interest of the REALSoCog task in revealing social cognition deficits, particularly for DLB patients by showing different social patterns as compared to AD patients. These results offer interesting clinical perspectives to develop more naturalistic tasks in such populations and for clinical differential diagnosis. Limitations and future perspectives are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"488-503"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141174012","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Young children's performance on a design fluency task: longitudinal data on total number of designs, clustering and switching, and regression-based norms. 幼儿在设计流畅性任务中的表现:关于设计总数、分组和切换以及基于回归的标准的纵向数据。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2359744
Christine Resch, Esther Keulers, Rosa Martens, Gerard van Breukelen, Caroline M van Heugten, Wenying Hou, Petra P M Hurks

Introduction: Design fluency (DF) tasks are commonly used to assess executive functions such as attentional control, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring and strategy use. Next to the total number of correct designs, the standard outcome of a DF task, clustering and switching can help disentangle the processes underlying DF performance. We present the first longitudinal study of 4-8-year-old children's developmental DF trajectories.

Method: At initial enrollment, children (n = 228) were aged between 4.05 and 6.88 years (M = 5.18, SD = 0.59) and attended Dutch primary schools. The DF task was administered at three time points, each time point separated by approximately 1 year. Data were analyzed using mixed regression for total number of correct designs and switching, and mixed logistic regression analysis for clustering.

Results: The total number of correct designs increased linearly across the three time points. Across all time points, children made very few clusters, and most clusters consisted of only 3 designs. Clustering only increased at the third assessment compared to the two previous assessments. Switching increased up to the second assessment, but not after that. The number of switches was highly correlated with the total number of correct designs at all time points (r = 0.78 to r = 0.85). These developmental trajectories were similar for all children regardless of their baseline age. Normative data are given for the total number of correct designs and switching.

Conclusions: Children as of age 4 onwards can perform a DF task. For children as young as 4-8 years old, computing clustering, and switching measures is of limited value to study cognitive processes underlying DF performance, next to the total number of correct designs. There were no sex differences on any of the DF outcomes. Level of parental education (LPE) was positively associated with the total number of correct designs and switching.

简介:设计流畅性(DF)任务通常用于评估注意力控制、认知灵活性、自我监控和策略使用等执行功能:设计流畅性(DF)任务通常用于评估注意力控制、认知灵活性、自我监控和策略使用等执行功能。设计流畅性任务的标准结果是正确设计的总数,除此以外,聚类和转换也有助于厘清设计流畅性表现的基本过程。我们首次对 4-8 岁儿童的 DF 发展轨迹进行了纵向研究:最初入学时,儿童(n = 228)的年龄在 4.05 到 6.88 岁之间(M = 5.18,SD = 0.59),就读于荷兰小学。DF 任务在三个时间点进行,每个时间点相隔约 1 年。数据采用混合回归法对正确设计总数和转换进行分析,并采用混合逻辑回归分析法对数据进行聚类分析:结果:在三个时间点上,正确设计的总数呈线性增长。在所有时间点上,孩子们的聚类都很少,大多数聚类都只有 3 个设计。与前两次评估相比,聚类只在第三次评估时才有所增加。转换次数在第二次评估前有所增加,但之后就没有了。在所有时间点上,切换次数与正确设计总数都高度相关(r = 0.78 到 r = 0.85)。无论基线年龄如何,所有儿童的发展轨迹都相似。我们还给出了正确设计总数和转换的标准数据:结论:4 岁以上的儿童可以完成 DF 任务。对于 4-8 岁的儿童来说,计算聚类和转换测量对于研究 DF 表现的认知过程价值有限,仅次于正确设计总数。在任何DF结果上都没有性别差异。父母的教育水平(LPE)与设计正确总数和转换呈正相关。
{"title":"Young children's performance on a design fluency task: longitudinal data on total number of designs, clustering and switching, and regression-based norms.","authors":"Christine Resch, Esther Keulers, Rosa Martens, Gerard van Breukelen, Caroline M van Heugten, Wenying Hou, Petra P M Hurks","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2359744","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2359744","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Design fluency (DF) tasks are commonly used to assess executive functions such as attentional control, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring and strategy use. Next to the total number of correct designs, the standard outcome of a DF task, clustering and switching can help disentangle the processes underlying DF performance. We present the first longitudinal study of 4-8-year-old children's developmental DF trajectories.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>At initial enrollment, children (<i>n</i> = 228) were aged between 4.05 and 6.88 years (<i>M</i> = 5.18, SD = 0.59) and attended Dutch primary schools. The DF task was administered at three time points, each time point separated by approximately 1 year. Data were analyzed using mixed regression for total number of correct designs and switching, and mixed logistic regression analysis for clustering.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>The total number of correct designs increased linearly across the three time points. Across all time points, children made very few clusters, and most clusters consisted of only 3 designs. Clustering only increased at the third assessment compared to the two previous assessments. Switching increased up to the second assessment, but not after that. The number of switches was highly correlated with the total number of correct designs at all time points (<i>r</i> = 0.78 to <i>r</i> = 0.85). These developmental trajectories were similar for all children regardless of their baseline age. Normative data are given for the total number of correct designs and switching.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Children as of age 4 onwards can perform a DF task. For children as young as 4-8 years old, computing clustering, and switching measures is of limited value to study cognitive processes underlying DF performance, next to the total number of correct designs. There were no sex differences on any of the DF outcomes. Level of parental education (LPE) was positively associated with the total number of correct designs and switching.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"449-465"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141247756","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prospective memory is associated with aspects of disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy. 前瞻性记忆与癫痫患者的残疾和生活质量有关。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-02 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348213
Yosefa A Modiano, Steven Paul Woods

Introduction: Episodic memory disruptions in epilepsy stem from shared neurocircuitry. While prior research has focused on retrospective memory (RM), prospective memory (PM; i.e. remembering to remember) also deserves consideration given its critical role in the management of daily activities. The current investigation assessed whether PM is associated with disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy.

Methods: This cross-sectional, correlational study included a consecutive series of 50 people with epilepsy presenting for neuropsychological evaluation who completed the Royal Prince Alfred Prospective Memory Test (RPA) and Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) and 63 demographically comparable healthy adults. The participants with epilepsy also completed clinical measures of neuropsychological ability and questionnaires assessing disability and quality of life.

Results: People with epilepsy had significantly more frequent memory symptoms as compared to healthy adults at a very large effect size. Worse mood was associated with lower PM ability at a medium effect size and more frequent PM symptoms at a large effect size. A hierarchical linear regression indicated that PM explained 52% of the variance in disability and 43% of the variance in quality of life after accounting for RM ability.

Conclusions: PM is associated with poorer everyday functioning among people with epilepsy and shows evidence of incremental value beyond RM ability in that regard. Future studies are needed to understand the complex pathways from PM to functional limitations to inform clinical intervention.

简介癫痫患者的外显记忆障碍源于共同的神经回路。以前的研究主要集中在回顾性记忆(RM)上,而前瞻性记忆(PM,即记住要记住的事)在日常活动管理中起着至关重要的作用,因此也值得考虑。目前的调查评估了前瞻性记忆是否与癫痫患者的残疾和生活质量有关:这项横断面相关性研究连续收集了 50 名接受神经心理学评估的癫痫患者,他们完成了皇家阿尔弗雷德王子前瞻性记忆测试(RPA)和前瞻性与回顾性记忆问卷(PRMQ),以及 63 名在人口统计学上具有可比性的健康成年人。癫痫患者还完成了神经心理能力临床测量以及残疾和生活质量评估问卷:结果:与健康成年人相比,癫痫患者的记忆症状明显更频繁,影响程度非常大。在中等效应水平上,情绪不佳与较低的记忆能力有关,在较大效应水平上,与更频繁的记忆症状有关。分层线性回归表明,在考虑RM能力后,PM可解释52%的残疾差异和43%的生活质量差异:PM 与癫痫患者较差的日常功能相关,并有证据表明,PM 在这方面具有超越 RM 能力的增量价值。未来的研究需要了解从 PM 到功能限制的复杂路径,以便为临床干预提供依据。
{"title":"Prospective memory is associated with aspects of disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy.","authors":"Yosefa A Modiano, Steven Paul Woods","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348213","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348213","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Episodic memory disruptions in epilepsy stem from shared neurocircuitry. While prior research has focused on retrospective memory (RM), prospective memory (PM; i.e. remembering to remember) also deserves consideration given its critical role in the management of daily activities. The current investigation assessed whether PM is associated with disability and quality of life in people with epilepsy.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>This cross-sectional, correlational study included a consecutive series of 50 people with epilepsy presenting for neuropsychological evaluation who completed the Royal Prince Alfred Prospective Memory Test (RPA) and Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) and 63 demographically comparable healthy adults. The participants with epilepsy also completed clinical measures of neuropsychological ability and questionnaires assessing disability and quality of life.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>People with epilepsy had significantly more frequent memory symptoms as compared to healthy adults at a very large effect size. Worse mood was associated with lower PM ability at a medium effect size and more frequent PM symptoms at a large effect size. A hierarchical linear regression indicated that PM explained 52% of the variance in disability and 43% of the variance in quality of life after accounting for RM ability.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>PM is associated with poorer everyday functioning among people with epilepsy and shows evidence of incremental value beyond RM ability in that regard. Future studies are needed to understand the complex pathways from PM to functional limitations to inform clinical intervention.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"316-328"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140849174","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social cognition in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. 1 型神经纤维瘤病儿童的社会认知能力。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-28 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2348214
Julie Remaud, Jérémy Besnard, Sébastien Barbarot, Arnaud Roy

Introduction: Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) is a genetic pathology that can lead to impaired social functioning that has a negative impact on patients' quality of life. To date, although the hypothesis of impaired social cognition has been proposed as a potential explanation for these difficulties, very few studies have focused on theory of mind in children with NF1. Furthermore, other complex sociocognitive abilities have never been investigated. The aim of the present study was to assess theory of mind, moral reasoning, and social information processing in children with NF1 compared with a control group.

Method: We administered the Paediatric Evaluation of Emotions, Relationships and Socialization® to 38 children with NF1 aged between 8 years and 16 years 11 months (mean = 11.4, SD = 2.3) and 43 control children with comparable sociodemographic characteristics.

Results: Patients performed significantly worse than controls on moral reasoning and social information processing tests, but there was no significant difference on theory of mind.

Conclusions: These results seem to confirm the presence of social cognition difficulties in NF1 that could explain, at least in part, their social difficulties, although not all dimensions are concerned. The differences between the processes we assessed are discussed in relation to the methodologies used to measure them, and raises questions about the complementarity of traditional tools and more ecological assessments.

简介神经纤维瘤病 1 型(NF1)是一种遗传性疾病,可导致社交功能受损,对患者的生活质量产生负面影响。迄今为止,尽管人们提出了社会认知能力受损的假说,作为这些困难的潜在解释,但很少有研究关注 NF1 儿童的心智理论。此外,对其他复杂的社会认知能力也从未进行过调查。本研究旨在评估 NF1 儿童与对照组相比在心智理论、道德推理和社会信息处理方面的能力:我们对 38 名年龄在 8 岁至 16 岁 11 个月之间的 NF1 儿童(平均值 = 11.4,标准差 = 2.3)和 43 名社会人口学特征相当的对照组儿童进行了儿科情感、关系和社会化评估®:结果:患者在道德推理和社会信息处理测试中的表现明显差于对照组,但在思维理论方面没有明显差异:这些结果似乎证实了 NF1 患儿存在社会认知障碍,至少可以部分解释他们的社交障碍,尽管并非涉及所有方面。我们所评估的过程之间的差异与测量这些过程所使用的方法有关,并提出了传统工具与更生态的评估是否互补的问题。
{"title":"Social cognition in children with neurofibromatosis type 1.","authors":"Julie Remaud, Jérémy Besnard, Sébastien Barbarot, Arnaud Roy","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348214","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2348214","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) is a genetic pathology that can lead to impaired social functioning that has a negative impact on patients' quality of life. To date, although the hypothesis of impaired social cognition has been proposed as a potential explanation for these difficulties, very few studies have focused on theory of mind in children with NF1. Furthermore, other complex sociocognitive abilities have never been investigated. The aim of the present study was to assess theory of mind, moral reasoning, and social information processing in children with NF1 compared with a control group.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We administered the Paediatric Evaluation of Emotions, Relationships and Socialization® to 38 children with NF1 aged between 8 years and 16 years 11 months (mean = 11.4, <i>SD</i> = 2.3) and 43 control children with comparable sociodemographic characteristics.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Patients performed significantly worse than controls on moral reasoning and social information processing tests, but there was no significant difference on theory of mind.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These results seem to confirm the presence of social cognition difficulties in NF1 that could explain, at least in part, their social difficulties, although not all dimensions are concerned. The differences between the processes we assessed are discussed in relation to the methodologies used to measure them, and raises questions about the complementarity of traditional tools and more ecological assessments.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"374-381"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140859660","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An evaluation of the moderating effects of routine and busyness on the relationship between prospective memory and everyday functioning in older persons with HIV disease. 评估例行公事和繁忙程度对患有艾滋病的老年人的前瞻性记忆和日常功能之间关系的调节作用。
IF 1.8 4区 心理学 Q3 CLINICAL NEUROLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-04 DOI: 10.1080/13803395.2024.2350577
Dhruvi M Mehta, Steven Paul Woods, Nneka Akpotaire

Introduction: People living with HIV (PLWH) often experience difficulties in everyday functioning, which can arise in part from deficits in the strategic/executive aspects of prospective memory (PM). Using Suchy's Contextually Valid Executive Assessment (ConVExA) framework, this study sought to determine whether the contextual factors of busyness and routine moderate the relationship between the strategic/executive aspects of PM and everyday functioning in older PLWH.

Methods: Participants in this cross-sectional analysis were 145 PLWH aged 50 years and older who had completed the Martin and Park Environmental Demands (MPED) questionnaire of routine and busyness, the performance-based Cambridge Test of Prospective Memory, and self-report measures of activities of daily living (ADLs) and cognitive symptoms in daily life.

Results: Multiple regression analyses covarying for relevant comorbidities showed that higher levels of busyness - but not routine - were associated with more frequent cognitive symptoms in daily life. Neither busyness nor routine interacted with PM in association with cognitive symptoms. However, routine and a strategic/executive measure of PM interacted in predicting ADLs; specifically, the association between time-based PM and ADLs was stronger in persons with higher levels of routine in their daily lives. Parallel analyses with less executively-demanding event-based PM were null and small.

Conclusions: Overall, findings provided mixed - and unexpected - evidence for the associations between contextual factors (i.e. routine and busyness), everyday functioning, and PM in this sample of older adults with HIV disease. Results and clinical implications are interpreted and discussed in the framework of the ConVExA model.

导言:艾滋病病毒感染者(PLWH)在日常生活中经常会遇到困难,这可能部分源于前瞻性记忆(PM)的策略/执行方面的缺陷。本研究采用 Suchy 的情境有效执行评估(ConVExA)框架,试图确定忙碌和日常事务等情境因素是否会缓和老年艾滋病病毒感染者前瞻性记忆的策略/执行方面与日常功能之间的关系:这项横断面分析的参与者是 145 名 50 岁及以上的 PLWH,他们完成了马丁和帕克环境需求(MPED)常规和忙碌程度问卷、基于表现的剑桥前瞻性记忆测试,以及日常生活活动(ADLs)和日常生活认知症状的自我报告测量:与相关合并症共同作用的多元回归分析表明,较高程度的忙碌与日常生活中更频繁出现的认知症状有关,但与常规症状无关。在认知症状与 PM 的关系方面,忙碌程度和日常工作都没有相互作用。然而,例行工作和策略性/执行性工作的衡量标准在预测日常生活自理能力方面是相互影响的;特别是,日常生活例行工作水平较高的人,其基于时间的工作与日常生活自理能力之间的关联性更强。对执行要求较低的基于事件的个人生活方式进行的平行分析结果为零,且结果很小:总体而言,研究结果提供了不同的--也是出乎意料的--证据,证明了在感染艾滋病病毒的老年人样本中,环境因素(即常规和繁忙程度)、日常功能和PM之间的关联。在 ConVExA 模型的框架内对结果和临床意义进行了解释和讨论。
{"title":"An evaluation of the moderating effects of routine and busyness on the relationship between prospective memory and everyday functioning in older persons with HIV disease.","authors":"Dhruvi M Mehta, Steven Paul Woods, Nneka Akpotaire","doi":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2350577","DOIUrl":"10.1080/13803395.2024.2350577","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Introduction: </strong>People living with HIV (PLWH) often experience difficulties in everyday functioning, which can arise in part from deficits in the strategic/executive aspects of prospective memory (PM). Using Suchy's Contextually Valid Executive Assessment (ConVExA) framework, this study sought to determine whether the contextual factors of busyness and routine moderate the relationship between the strategic/executive aspects of PM and everyday functioning in older PLWH.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Participants in this cross-sectional analysis were 145 PLWH aged 50 years and older who had completed the Martin and Park Environmental Demands (MPED) questionnaire of routine and busyness, the performance-based Cambridge Test of Prospective Memory, and self-report measures of activities of daily living (ADLs) and cognitive symptoms in daily life.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Multiple regression analyses covarying for relevant comorbidities showed that higher levels of busyness - but not routine - were associated with more frequent cognitive symptoms in daily life. Neither busyness nor routine interacted with PM in association with cognitive symptoms. However, routine and a strategic/executive measure of PM interacted in predicting ADLs; specifically, the association between time-based PM and ADLs was stronger in persons with higher levels of routine in their daily lives. Parallel analyses with less executively-demanding event-based PM were null and small.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Overall, findings provided mixed - and unexpected - evidence for the associations between contextual factors (i.e. routine and busyness), everyday functioning, and PM in this sample of older adults with HIV disease. Results and clinical implications are interpreted and discussed in the framework of the ConVExA model.</p>","PeriodicalId":15382,"journal":{"name":"Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology","volume":" ","pages":"341-351"},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11309909/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140855249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1