首页 > 最新文献

Molecular Autism最新文献

英文 中文
Brainstem white matter microstructure is associated with hyporesponsiveness and overall sensory features in autistic children. 自闭症儿童脑干白质微观结构与低反应性和整体感觉特征有关。
IF 6.3 1区 医学 Q1 GENETICS & HEREDITY Pub Date : 2022-12-19 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00524-3
Olivia Surgent, Ali Riaz, Karla K Ausderau, Nagesh Adluru, Gregory R Kirk, Jose Guerrero-Gonzalez, Emily C Skaletski, Steven R Kecskemeti, Douglas C Dean Iii, Susan Ellis Weismer, Andrew L Alexander, Brittany G Travers

Background: Elevated or reduced responses to sensory stimuli, known as sensory features, are common in autistic individuals and often impact quality of life. Little is known about the neurobiological basis of sensory features in autistic children. However, the brainstem may offer critical insights as it has been associated with both basic sensory processing and core features of autism.

Methods: Diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) and parent-report of sensory features were acquired from 133 children (61 autistic children with and 72 non-autistic children, 6-11 years-old). Leveraging novel DWI processing techniques, we investigated the relationship between sensory features and white matter microstructure properties (free-water-elimination-corrected fractional anisotropy [FA] and mean diffusivity [MD]) in precisely delineated brainstem white matter tracts. Follow-up analyses assessed relationships between microstructure and sensory response patterns/modalities and analyzed whole brain white matter using voxel-based analysis.

Results: Results revealed distinct relationships between brainstem microstructure and sensory features in autistic children compared to non-autistic children. In autistic children, more prominent sensory features were generally associated with lower MD. Further, in autistic children, sensory hyporesponsiveness and tactile responsivity were strongly associated with white matter microstructure in nearly all brainstem tracts. Follow-up voxel-based analyses confirmed that these relationships were more prominent in the brainstem/cerebellum, with additional sensory-brain findings in the autistic group in the white matter of the primary motor and somatosensory cortices, the occipital lobe, the inferior parietal lobe, and the thalamic projections.

Limitations: All participants communicated via spoken language and acclimated to the sensory environment of an MRI session, which should be considered when assessing the generalizability of this work to the whole of the autism spectrum.

Conclusions: These findings suggest unique brainstem white matter contributions to sensory features in autistic children compared to non-autistic children. The brainstem correlates of sensory features underscore the potential reflex-like nature of behavioral responses to sensory stimuli in autism and have implications for how we conceptualize and address sensory features in autistic populations.

背景:对感官刺激的反应增强或减弱,即所谓的感官特征,在自闭症患者中很常见,并经常影响生活质量。我们对自闭症儿童的感觉特征的神经生物学基础知之甚少。然而,脑干可能提供关键的见解,因为它与基本的感觉处理和自闭症的核心特征有关。方法:对133例儿童(6 ~ 11岁,有自闭症儿童61例,非自闭症儿童72例)进行弥散加权成像(DWI)和感官特征的家长报告。利用新的DWI处理技术,我们研究了精确描绘的脑干白质束中感官特征与白质微观结构特性(经自由水消除校正的分数各向异性[FA]和平均扩散率[MD])之间的关系。后续分析评估了微观结构与感觉反应模式/模式之间的关系,并使用基于体素的分析分析了全脑白质。结果:自闭症儿童与非自闭症儿童相比,脑干微观结构与感觉特征有明显的关系。在自闭症儿童中,更突出的感觉特征通常与较低的MD相关。此外,在自闭症儿童中,几乎所有脑干束的感觉低反应性和触觉反应性与白质微结构密切相关。后续基于体素的分析证实,这些关系在脑干/小脑中更为突出,在自闭症组的初级运动皮层和体感觉皮层、枕叶、下顶叶和丘脑投射的白质中也有额外的感觉脑发现。局限性:所有参与者都通过口头语言进行交流,并适应MRI会话的感官环境,在评估这项工作对整个自闭症谱系的普遍性时应考虑到这一点。结论:这些发现表明,与非自闭症儿童相比,自闭症儿童的脑干白质对感觉特征有独特的贡献。感觉特征的脑干相关性强调了自闭症患者对感觉刺激的行为反应的潜在反射性质,并对我们如何概念化和处理自闭症人群的感觉特征具有启示意义。
{"title":"Brainstem white matter microstructure is associated with hyporesponsiveness and overall sensory features in autistic children.","authors":"Olivia Surgent, Ali Riaz, Karla K Ausderau, Nagesh Adluru, Gregory R Kirk, Jose Guerrero-Gonzalez, Emily C Skaletski, Steven R Kecskemeti, Douglas C Dean Iii, Susan Ellis Weismer, Andrew L Alexander, Brittany G Travers","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00524-3","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s13229-022-00524-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Elevated or reduced responses to sensory stimuli, known as sensory features, are common in autistic individuals and often impact quality of life. Little is known about the neurobiological basis of sensory features in autistic children. However, the brainstem may offer critical insights as it has been associated with both basic sensory processing and core features of autism.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) and parent-report of sensory features were acquired from 133 children (61 autistic children with and 72 non-autistic children, 6-11 years-old). Leveraging novel DWI processing techniques, we investigated the relationship between sensory features and white matter microstructure properties (free-water-elimination-corrected fractional anisotropy [FA] and mean diffusivity [MD]) in precisely delineated brainstem white matter tracts. Follow-up analyses assessed relationships between microstructure and sensory response patterns/modalities and analyzed whole brain white matter using voxel-based analysis.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Results revealed distinct relationships between brainstem microstructure and sensory features in autistic children compared to non-autistic children. In autistic children, more prominent sensory features were generally associated with lower MD. Further, in autistic children, sensory hyporesponsiveness and tactile responsivity were strongly associated with white matter microstructure in nearly all brainstem tracts. Follow-up voxel-based analyses confirmed that these relationships were more prominent in the brainstem/cerebellum, with additional sensory-brain findings in the autistic group in the white matter of the primary motor and somatosensory cortices, the occipital lobe, the inferior parietal lobe, and the thalamic projections.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>All participants communicated via spoken language and acclimated to the sensory environment of an MRI session, which should be considered when assessing the generalizability of this work to the whole of the autism spectrum.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings suggest unique brainstem white matter contributions to sensory features in autistic children compared to non-autistic children. The brainstem correlates of sensory features underscore the potential reflex-like nature of behavioral responses to sensory stimuli in autism and have implications for how we conceptualize and address sensory features in autistic populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.3,"publicationDate":"2022-12-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9762648/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9698789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Altered frontal connectivity as a mechanism for executive function deficits in fragile X syndrome. 额叶连通性改变作为脆性X综合征执行功能缺陷的机制。
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-12-09 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00527-0
Lauren M Schmitt, Joy Li, Rui Liu, Paul S Horn, John A Sweeney, Craig A Erickson, Ernest V Pedapati

Background: Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is the leading inherited monogenic cause of intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorder. Executive function (EF), necessary for adaptive goal-oriented behavior and dependent on frontal lobe function, is impaired in individuals with FXS. Yet, little is known how alterations in frontal lobe neural activity is related to EF deficits in FXS.

Methods: Sixty-one participants with FXS (54% males) and 71 age- and sex-matched typically-developing controls (TDC; 58% males) completed a five-minute resting state electroencephalography (EEG) protocol and a computerized battery of tests of EF, the Test of Attentional Performance for Children (KiTAP). Following source localization (minimum-norm estimate), we computed debiased weighted phase lag index (dWPLI), a phase connectivity value, for pairings between 18 nodes in frontal regions for gamma (30-55 Hz) and alpha (10.5-12.5 Hz) bands. Linear models were generated with fixed factors of group, sex, frequency, and connection. Relationships between frontal connectivity and EF variables also were examined.

Results: Individuals with FXS demonstrated increased gamma band and reduced alpha band connectivity across all frontal regions and across hemispheres compared to TDC. After controlling for nonverbal IQ, increased error rates on EF tasks were associated with increased gamma band and reduced alpha band connectivity.

Limitations: Frontal connectivity findings are limited to intrinsic brain activity during rest and may not generalize to frontal connectivity during EF tasks or everyday function.

Conclusions: We report gamma hyper-connectivity and alpha hypo-connectivity within source-localized frontal brain regions in FXS compared to TDC during resting-state EEG. For the first time in FXS, we report significant associations between EF and altered frontal connectivity, with increased error rate relating to increased gamma band connectivity and reduced alpha band connectivity. These findings suggest increased phase connectivity within gamma band may impair EF performance, whereas greater alpha band connectivity may provide compensatory support for EF. Together, these findings provide important insight into neurophysiological mechanisms of EF deficits in FXS and provide novel targets for treatment development.

背景:脆性X染色体综合征(FXS)是智力残疾和自闭症谱系障碍的主要遗传单基因病因。执行功能(EF)是适应性目标导向行为所必需的,依赖于额叶功能,在FXS个体中受损。然而,很少有人知道额叶神经活动的改变如何与FXS的EF缺陷相关。方法:61名FXS患者(54%为男性)和71名年龄和性别匹配的典型发育对照组(TDC;(58%男性)完成了5分钟的静息状态脑电图(EEG)方案和一系列儿童注意力表现测试(KiTAP)的计算机化测试。在源定位(最小范数估计)之后,我们计算了额叶区域18个节点之间的相位连接值(dWPLI),用于gamma (30-55 Hz)和alpha (10.5-12.5 Hz)频段的配对。以群体、性别、频率、连接等固定因子生成线性模型。额叶连通性与EF变量之间的关系也得到了检验。结果:与TDC相比,FXS个体在所有额叶区域和半球上表现出增加的伽马带和减少的α带连接。在控制了非语言智商后,EF任务错误率的增加与伽马波段的增加和α波段连接的减少有关。局限性:额叶连通性的发现仅限于休息时的内在大脑活动,可能不能推广到EF任务或日常功能时的额叶连通性。结论:我们报告了与静息状态脑电图中TDC相比,FXS中源定位额叶区域的伽马超连接和α低连接。在FXS中,我们首次报告了EF与额叶连接改变之间的显著关联,其错误率增加与伽马波段连接增加和α波段连接减少有关。这些发现表明,伽马波段的相位连通性增加可能会损害EF的表现,而更大的α波段连通性可能会为EF提供补偿性支持。总之,这些发现为FXS中EF缺陷的神经生理机制提供了重要的见解,并为治疗开发提供了新的靶点。
{"title":"Altered frontal connectivity as a mechanism for executive function deficits in fragile X syndrome.","authors":"Lauren M Schmitt,&nbsp;Joy Li,&nbsp;Rui Liu,&nbsp;Paul S Horn,&nbsp;John A Sweeney,&nbsp;Craig A Erickson,&nbsp;Ernest V Pedapati","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00527-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00527-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is the leading inherited monogenic cause of intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorder. Executive function (EF), necessary for adaptive goal-oriented behavior and dependent on frontal lobe function, is impaired in individuals with FXS. Yet, little is known how alterations in frontal lobe neural activity is related to EF deficits in FXS.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Sixty-one participants with FXS (54% males) and 71 age- and sex-matched typically-developing controls (TDC; 58% males) completed a five-minute resting state electroencephalography (EEG) protocol and a computerized battery of tests of EF, the Test of Attentional Performance for Children (KiTAP). Following source localization (minimum-norm estimate), we computed debiased weighted phase lag index (dWPLI), a phase connectivity value, for pairings between 18 nodes in frontal regions for gamma (30-55 Hz) and alpha (10.5-12.5 Hz) bands. Linear models were generated with fixed factors of group, sex, frequency, and connection. Relationships between frontal connectivity and EF variables also were examined.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Individuals with FXS demonstrated increased gamma band and reduced alpha band connectivity across all frontal regions and across hemispheres compared to TDC. After controlling for nonverbal IQ, increased error rates on EF tasks were associated with increased gamma band and reduced alpha band connectivity.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>Frontal connectivity findings are limited to intrinsic brain activity during rest and may not generalize to frontal connectivity during EF tasks or everyday function.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>We report gamma hyper-connectivity and alpha hypo-connectivity within source-localized frontal brain regions in FXS compared to TDC during resting-state EEG. For the first time in FXS, we report significant associations between EF and altered frontal connectivity, with increased error rate relating to increased gamma band connectivity and reduced alpha band connectivity. These findings suggest increased phase connectivity within gamma band may impair EF performance, whereas greater alpha band connectivity may provide compensatory support for EF. Together, these findings provide important insight into neurophysiological mechanisms of EF deficits in FXS and provide novel targets for treatment development.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-12-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9733336/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10590879","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Infant excitation/inhibition balance interacts with executive attention to predict autistic traits in childhood. 婴儿期兴奋/抑制平衡与执行注意力相互作用,可预测儿童期自闭症特征。
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-12-08 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00526-1
Virginia Carter Leno, Jannath Begum-Ali, Amy Goodwin, Luke Mason, Greg Pasco, Andrew Pickles, Shruti Garg, Jonathan Green, Tony Charman, Mark H Johnson, Emily J H Jones

Background: Autism is proposed to be characterised by an atypical balance of cortical excitation and inhibition (E/I). However, most studies have examined E/I alterations in older autistic individuals, meaning that findings could in part reflect homeostatic compensation. To assess the directionality of effects, it is necessary to examine alterations in E/I balance early in the lifespan before symptom emergence. Recent explanatory frameworks have argued that it is also necessary to consider how early risk features interact with later developing modifier factors to predict autism outcomes.

Method: We indexed E/I balance in early infancy by extracting the aperiodic exponent of the slope of the electroencephalogram (EEG) power spectrum ('1/f'). To validate our index of E/I balance, we tested for differences in the aperiodic exponent in 10-month-old infants with (n = 22) and without (n = 27) neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1), a condition thought to be characterised by alterations to cortical inhibition. We then tested for E/I alterations in a larger heterogeneous longitudinal cohort of infants with and without a family history of neurodevelopmental conditions (n = 150) who had been followed to early childhood. We tested the relevance of alterations in E/I balance and our proposed modifier, executive attention, by assessing whether associations between 10-month aperiodic slope and 36-month neurodevelopmental traits were moderated by 24-month executive attention. Analyses adjusted for age at EEG assessment, sex and number of EEG trials.

Results: Infants with NF1 were characterised by a higher aperiodic exponent, indicative of greater inhibition, supporting our infant measure of E/I. Longitudinal analyses showed a significant interaction between aperiodic slope and executive attention, such that higher aperiodic exponents predicted greater autistic traits in childhood, but only in infants who also had weaker executive functioning abilities.

Limitations: The current study relied on parent report of infant executive functioning-type abilities; future work is required to replicate effects with objective measures of cognition.

Conclusions: Results suggest alterations in E/I balance are on the developmental pathway to autism outcomes, and that higher executive functioning abilities may buffer the impact of early cortical atypicalities, consistent with proposals that stronger executive functioning abilities may modify the impact of a wide range of risk factors.

背景:自闭症的特征被认为是大脑皮层兴奋和抑制(E/I)的非典型平衡。然而,大多数研究都是对年龄较大的自闭症患者的E/I改变进行研究,这意味着研究结果可能部分反映了同态复仇。为了评估影响的方向性,有必要在自闭症症状出现之前的生命早期研究 E/I 平衡的改变。最近的解释框架认为,还必须考虑早期风险特征如何与后期发展的调节因素相互作用,以预测自闭症的结果:方法:我们通过提取脑电图(EEG)功率谱斜率的非周期性指数("1/f")来确定婴儿早期的E/I平衡指数。为了验证我们的 E/I 平衡指数,我们测试了 10 个月大的神经纤维瘤病 1 型(NF1)婴儿(22 人)和非神经纤维瘤病 1 型婴儿(27 人)的非周期性指数差异。然后,我们在一个更大的异质性纵向队列中检测了E/I的改变,该队列包括有神经发育疾病家族史和无家族史的婴儿(n = 150),这些婴儿一直被跟踪到幼儿期。我们通过评估10个月的非周期性斜率与36个月的神经发育特征之间的关联是否会被24个月的执行注意力所调节,从而检验了E/I平衡的改变与我们提出的调节因子--执行注意力的相关性。分析对脑电图评估时的年龄、性别和脑电图试验次数进行了调整:结果:患有 NF1 的婴儿具有较高的非周期性指数,表明其抑制能力较强,这支持了我们的婴儿 E/I 测量方法。纵向分析表明,非周期性斜率与执行注意力之间存在显著的交互作用,因此较高的非周期性指数预示着较强的儿童自闭症特征,但这只适用于执行功能能力较弱的婴儿:局限性:目前的研究依赖于家长对婴儿执行功能型能力的报告;未来的工作需要用客观的认知测量方法来复制效果:研究结果表明,E/I平衡的改变是自闭症结局的发展途径,较高的执行功能能力可能会缓冲早期皮质非典型性的影响,这与较强的执行功能能力可能会改变各种风险因素的影响的建议是一致的。
{"title":"Infant excitation/inhibition balance interacts with executive attention to predict autistic traits in childhood.","authors":"Virginia Carter Leno, Jannath Begum-Ali, Amy Goodwin, Luke Mason, Greg Pasco, Andrew Pickles, Shruti Garg, Jonathan Green, Tony Charman, Mark H Johnson, Emily J H Jones","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00526-1","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s13229-022-00526-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Autism is proposed to be characterised by an atypical balance of cortical excitation and inhibition (E/I). However, most studies have examined E/I alterations in older autistic individuals, meaning that findings could in part reflect homeostatic compensation. To assess the directionality of effects, it is necessary to examine alterations in E/I balance early in the lifespan before symptom emergence. Recent explanatory frameworks have argued that it is also necessary to consider how early risk features interact with later developing modifier factors to predict autism outcomes.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We indexed E/I balance in early infancy by extracting the aperiodic exponent of the slope of the electroencephalogram (EEG) power spectrum ('1/f'). To validate our index of E/I balance, we tested for differences in the aperiodic exponent in 10-month-old infants with (n = 22) and without (n = 27) neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1), a condition thought to be characterised by alterations to cortical inhibition. We then tested for E/I alterations in a larger heterogeneous longitudinal cohort of infants with and without a family history of neurodevelopmental conditions (n = 150) who had been followed to early childhood. We tested the relevance of alterations in E/I balance and our proposed modifier, executive attention, by assessing whether associations between 10-month aperiodic slope and 36-month neurodevelopmental traits were moderated by 24-month executive attention. Analyses adjusted for age at EEG assessment, sex and number of EEG trials.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Infants with NF1 were characterised by a higher aperiodic exponent, indicative of greater inhibition, supporting our infant measure of E/I. Longitudinal analyses showed a significant interaction between aperiodic slope and executive attention, such that higher aperiodic exponents predicted greater autistic traits in childhood, but only in infants who also had weaker executive functioning abilities.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>The current study relied on parent report of infant executive functioning-type abilities; future work is required to replicate effects with objective measures of cognition.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Results suggest alterations in E/I balance are on the developmental pathway to autism outcomes, and that higher executive functioning abilities may buffer the impact of early cortical atypicalities, consistent with proposals that stronger executive functioning abilities may modify the impact of a wide range of risk factors.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9733024/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9506181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Non-suicidal self-injury and its relation to suicide through acquired capability: investigating this causal mechanism in a mainly late-diagnosed autistic sample. 非自杀性自我伤害及其通过获得性能力与自杀的关系:在主要晚期诊断的自闭症样本中探讨其因果机制。
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-11-12 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00522-5
Rachel L Moseley, Nicola J Gregory, Paula Smith, Carrie Allison, Sarah Cassidy, Simon Baron-Cohen

Background: Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) has been linked with a higher risk of suicide attempts in autistic and non-autistic people. In the general population, NSSI may confer acquired capability for suicide by eroding one's fear and avoidance of pain and death. The present study aimed to explore acquired capability as the mediator of increased suicide risk conferred by NSSI in autistic and non-autistic adults.

Methods: Autistic and non-autistic adults (n = 314, n = 312) completed an online survey exploring lifetime suicide attempts, experience with NSSI, and acquired capability for suicide. We explored relationships between lifetime incidence of NSSI and lifetime suicide attempts via three facets of acquired capability (pain tolerance, reduced fear of death, and mental rehearsal of suicide). In self-harming participants (224 autistic and 156 non-autistic), we explored whether particular types and features of NSSI might be especially associated with capability and through that with suicide: namely engagement in scratching, cutting, and self-hitting, and engaging in more numerous forms of NSSI.

Results: While a higher frequency of NSSI was associated with all three facets of acquired capability, only reduced fear of death and mental rehearsal of suicide mediated an indirect relationship with lifetime suicide attempts. NSSI also directly predicted more numerous suicide attempts. Autistic people tended towards reduced fear of death and mental rehearsal regardless of NSSI status. Among self-harming autistic and non-autistic participants, cutting and an increased number of NSSI behaviours were associated with lifetime suicide attempts directly and indirectly via acquired capability. In both groups, self-hitting was associated with lifetime suicide attempts only via acquired capability.

Limitations: Our cross-sectional methodology negates inferences of directionality. While we controlled for age, our samples were poorly matched, with the autistic group two times older on average. The autistic sample, predominantly late-diagnosed, female and highly qualified, were unrepresentative of the whole autistic community.

Conclusions: Our data suggest that acquired capability, as measured herein, is an incomplete explanation for the association between NSSI and suicide risk. A broader construct with stable and transient facets may offer greater explanatory power, but it is probable that other variables explain or provide additional means through which this association arises.

背景:在自闭症和非自闭症人群中,非自杀性自伤(NSSI)与更高的自杀企图风险有关。在一般人群中,自伤可能通过侵蚀一个人对痛苦和死亡的恐惧和避免而赋予获得性自杀能力。本研究旨在探讨孤独症和非孤独症成人自伤后获得性能力在自杀风险增加中的中介作用。方法:自闭症和非自闭症成年人(n = 314, n = 312)完成了一项在线调查,调查内容包括终生自杀企图、自伤经历和获得性自杀能力。我们通过三个方面的获得性能力(疼痛耐受性、对死亡的恐惧减少和自杀的心理预演)探讨了终生自伤发生率与终生自杀企图之间的关系。在自伤参与者中(224名自闭者和156名非自闭者),我们探讨了自伤的特定类型和特征是否可能与能力特别相关,并通过这种能力与自杀相关:即参与抓伤、切割和自伤,以及参与更多形式的自伤。结果:虽然较高的自伤频率与获得性能力的所有三个方面都相关,但只有减少对死亡的恐惧和自杀的心理预演与终生自杀企图有间接关系。自伤也直接预示着更多的自杀企图。自闭症患者倾向于减少对死亡的恐惧和心理排练,而不管自伤状态如何。在自我伤害的自闭症和非自闭症参与者中,自伤行为的减少和自伤行为数量的增加与终生自杀企图直接或间接地通过后天能力相关。在这两组中,自残与终生自杀企图的关联仅通过后天能力。局限性:我们的横断面方法否定了方向性的推论。虽然我们控制了年龄,但我们的样本匹配得很差,自闭症组的平均年龄是他们的两倍。自闭症样本,主要是晚期诊断的女性和高质量的,不能代表整个自闭症群体。结论:我们的数据表明,本文所测量的获得性能力并不能完全解释自伤与自杀风险之间的关系。具有稳定和短暂方面的更广泛的结构可能提供更大的解释力,但其他变量可能解释或提供通过这种关联产生的额外手段。
{"title":"Non-suicidal self-injury and its relation to suicide through acquired capability: investigating this causal mechanism in a mainly late-diagnosed autistic sample.","authors":"Rachel L Moseley,&nbsp;Nicola J Gregory,&nbsp;Paula Smith,&nbsp;Carrie Allison,&nbsp;Sarah Cassidy,&nbsp;Simon Baron-Cohen","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00522-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00522-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) has been linked with a higher risk of suicide attempts in autistic and non-autistic people. In the general population, NSSI may confer acquired capability for suicide by eroding one's fear and avoidance of pain and death. The present study aimed to explore acquired capability as the mediator of increased suicide risk conferred by NSSI in autistic and non-autistic adults.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Autistic and non-autistic adults (n = 314, n = 312) completed an online survey exploring lifetime suicide attempts, experience with NSSI, and acquired capability for suicide. We explored relationships between lifetime incidence of NSSI and lifetime suicide attempts via three facets of acquired capability (pain tolerance, reduced fear of death, and mental rehearsal of suicide). In self-harming participants (224 autistic and 156 non-autistic), we explored whether particular types and features of NSSI might be especially associated with capability and through that with suicide: namely engagement in scratching, cutting, and self-hitting, and engaging in more numerous forms of NSSI.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>While a higher frequency of NSSI was associated with all three facets of acquired capability, only reduced fear of death and mental rehearsal of suicide mediated an indirect relationship with lifetime suicide attempts. NSSI also directly predicted more numerous suicide attempts. Autistic people tended towards reduced fear of death and mental rehearsal regardless of NSSI status. Among self-harming autistic and non-autistic participants, cutting and an increased number of NSSI behaviours were associated with lifetime suicide attempts directly and indirectly via acquired capability. In both groups, self-hitting was associated with lifetime suicide attempts only via acquired capability.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>Our cross-sectional methodology negates inferences of directionality. While we controlled for age, our samples were poorly matched, with the autistic group two times older on average. The autistic sample, predominantly late-diagnosed, female and highly qualified, were unrepresentative of the whole autistic community.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our data suggest that acquired capability, as measured herein, is an incomplete explanation for the association between NSSI and suicide risk. A broader construct with stable and transient facets may offer greater explanatory power, but it is probable that other variables explain or provide additional means through which this association arises.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9655904/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10442743","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Maternal vitamin D during pregnancy and offspring autism and autism-associated traits: a prospective cohort study. 孕妇孕期维生素D与后代自闭症及自闭症相关特征:一项前瞻性队列研究
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-11-12 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00523-4
Paul Madley-Dowd, Christina Dardani, Robyn E Wootton, Kyle Dack, Tom Palmer, Rupert Thurston, Alexandra Havdahl, Jean Golding, Deborah Lawlor, Dheeraj Rai

Background: There has been a growing interest in the association between maternal levels of vitamin D during pregnancy and offspring autism. However, whether any associations reflect causal effects is still inconclusive.

Methods: We used data from a UK-based pregnancy cohort study (Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children) comprising 7689 births between 1991 and 1992 with maternal blood vitamin D levels recorded during pregnancy and at least one recorded outcome measure, including autism diagnosis and autism-associated traits. The association between each outcome with seasonal and gestational age-adjusted maternal serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D during pregnancy was estimated using confounder-adjusted regression models. Multiple imputation was used to account for missing data, and restricted cubic splines were used to investigate nonlinear associations. Mendelian randomization was used to strengthen causal inference.

Results: No strong evidence of an association between maternal serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D during pregnancy and any offspring autism-associated outcome was found using multivariable regression analysis (autism diagnosis: adjusted OR = 0.98, 95% CI = 0.90-1.06), including with multiple imputation (autism diagnosis: adjusted OR = 0.99, 95% CI = 0.93-1.06), and no evidence of a causal effect was suggested by Mendelian randomization (autism diagnosis: causal OR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.46-2.55). Some evidence of increased odds of autism-associated traits at lower levels of maternal serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D was found using spline analysis.

Limitations: Our study was potentially limited by low power, particularly for diagnosed autism cases as an outcome. The cohort may not have captured the extreme lows of the distribution of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D, and our analyses may have been biased by residual confounding and missing data.

Conclusions: The present study found no strong evidence of a causal link between maternal vitamin D levels in pregnancy and offspring diagnosis or traits of autism.

背景:人们对孕妇孕期维生素D水平与后代自闭症之间的关系越来越感兴趣。然而,是否有任何关联反映了因果关系仍然没有定论。方法:我们使用了一项英国妊娠队列研究(雅芳父母与儿童纵向研究)的数据,该研究包括7689名1991年至1992年间出生的孕妇,她们在怀孕期间记录了母亲血液中维生素D的水平,并至少记录了一项结果测量,包括自闭症诊断和自闭症相关特征。每个结果与季节和妊娠期经胎龄调整的母体血清25-羟基维生素D之间的关联使用混杂校正回归模型进行估计。多重输入用于解释缺失数据,限制三次样条用于研究非线性关联。采用孟德尔随机化来加强因果推理。结果:使用多变量回归分析(自闭症诊断:校正OR = 0.98, 95% CI = 0.90-1.06),包括多重归算(自闭症诊断:校正OR = 0.99, 95% CI = 0.93-1.06),以及孟德尔随机化(自闭症诊断:因果OR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.46-2.55),均未发现妊娠期间母体血清25-羟基维生素D与任何后代自闭症相关结局之间存在关联的有力证据。使用样条分析发现,在母体血清25-羟基维生素D水平较低时,自闭症相关特征的几率增加。局限性:我们的研究可能受到低功率的限制,特别是对于诊断为自闭症的病例。该队列可能没有捕捉到血清25-羟基维生素D分布的极端低点,我们的分析可能因残留混淆和缺失数据而有偏差。结论:目前的研究没有发现强有力的证据表明怀孕期间母亲维生素D水平与后代自闭症的诊断或特征之间存在因果关系。
{"title":"Maternal vitamin D during pregnancy and offspring autism and autism-associated traits: a prospective cohort study.","authors":"Paul Madley-Dowd,&nbsp;Christina Dardani,&nbsp;Robyn E Wootton,&nbsp;Kyle Dack,&nbsp;Tom Palmer,&nbsp;Rupert Thurston,&nbsp;Alexandra Havdahl,&nbsp;Jean Golding,&nbsp;Deborah Lawlor,&nbsp;Dheeraj Rai","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00523-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00523-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>There has been a growing interest in the association between maternal levels of vitamin D during pregnancy and offspring autism. However, whether any associations reflect causal effects is still inconclusive.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>We used data from a UK-based pregnancy cohort study (Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children) comprising 7689 births between 1991 and 1992 with maternal blood vitamin D levels recorded during pregnancy and at least one recorded outcome measure, including autism diagnosis and autism-associated traits. The association between each outcome with seasonal and gestational age-adjusted maternal serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D during pregnancy was estimated using confounder-adjusted regression models. Multiple imputation was used to account for missing data, and restricted cubic splines were used to investigate nonlinear associations. Mendelian randomization was used to strengthen causal inference.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>No strong evidence of an association between maternal serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D during pregnancy and any offspring autism-associated outcome was found using multivariable regression analysis (autism diagnosis: adjusted OR = 0.98, 95% CI = 0.90-1.06), including with multiple imputation (autism diagnosis: adjusted OR = 0.99, 95% CI = 0.93-1.06), and no evidence of a causal effect was suggested by Mendelian randomization (autism diagnosis: causal OR = 1.08, 95% CI = 0.46-2.55). Some evidence of increased odds of autism-associated traits at lower levels of maternal serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D was found using spline analysis.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>Our study was potentially limited by low power, particularly for diagnosed autism cases as an outcome. The cohort may not have captured the extreme lows of the distribution of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D, and our analyses may have been biased by residual confounding and missing data.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The present study found no strong evidence of a causal link between maternal vitamin D levels in pregnancy and offspring diagnosis or traits of autism.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-11-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9652971/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9153023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Facial expression recognition is linked to clinical and neurofunctional differences in autism. 面部表情识别与自闭症患者的临床和神经功能差异有关。
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-11-10 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00520-7
Hannah Meyer-Lindenberg, Carolin Moessnang, Bethany Oakley, Jumana Ahmad, Luke Mason, Emily J H Jones, Hannah L Hayward, Jennifer Cooke, Daisy Crawley, Rosemary Holt, Julian Tillmann, Tony Charman, Simon Baron-Cohen, Tobias Banaschewski, Christian Beckmann, Heike Tost, Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg, Jan K Buitelaar, Declan G Murphy, Michael J Brammer, Eva Loth

Background: Difficulties in social communication are a defining clinical feature of autism. However, the underlying neurobiological heterogeneity has impeded targeted therapies and requires new approaches to identifying clinically relevant bio-behavioural subgroups. In the largest autism cohort to date, we comprehensively examined difficulties in facial expression recognition, a key process in social communication, as a bio-behavioural stratification biomarker, and validated them against clinical features and neurofunctional responses.

Methods: Between 255 and 488 participants aged 6-30 years with autism, typical development and/or mild intellectual disability completed the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces task, the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task and/or the Films Expression Task. We first examined mean-group differences on each test. Then, we used a novel intersection approach that compares two centroid and connectivity-based clustering methods to derive subgroups based on the combined performance across the three tasks. Measures and subgroups were then related to clinical features and neurofunctional differences measured using fMRI during a fearful face-matching task.

Results: We found significant mean-group differences on each expression recognition test. However, cluster analyses showed that these were driven by a low-performing autistic subgroup (~ 30% of autistic individuals who performed below 2SDs of the neurotypical mean on at least one test), while a larger subgroup (~ 70%) performed within 1SD on at least 2 tests. The low-performing subgroup also had on average significantly more social communication difficulties and lower activation in the amygdala and fusiform gyrus than the high-performing subgroup.

Limitations: Findings of autism expression recognition subgroups and their characteristics require independent replication. This is currently not possible, as there is no other existing dataset that includes all relevant measures. However, we demonstrated high internal robustness (91.6%) of findings between two clustering methods with fundamentally different assumptions, which is a critical pre-condition for independent replication.

Conclusions: We identified a subgroup of autistic individuals with expression recognition difficulties and showed that this related to clinical and neurobiological characteristics. If replicated, expression recognition may serve as bio-behavioural stratification biomarker and aid in the development of targeted interventions for a subgroup of autistic individuals.

背景:社会沟通困难是自闭症的典型临床特征。然而,潜在的神经生物学异质性阻碍了靶向治疗,需要新的方法来确定临床相关的生物行为亚群。在迄今为止最大的自闭症队列中,我们全面检查了面部表情识别的困难,这是社会交流的关键过程,作为生物行为分层生物标志物,并根据临床特征和神经功能反应验证了它们。方法:255 ~ 488名年龄在6-30岁之间的自闭症、典型发育和/或轻度智力障碍的参与者完成了卡罗林斯卡定向情绪面孔任务、通过眼睛读心任务和/或电影表达任务。我们首先检查了每个测试的平均组差异。然后,我们使用了一种新颖的交叉点方法,该方法比较了两种基于质心和基于连通性的聚类方法,从而基于三个任务的综合性能派生出子组。然后,测量和亚组与临床特征和神经功能差异相关,这些差异是在恐惧面孔匹配任务中使用功能磁共振成像测量的。结果:各表情识别测试结果组间均值差异显著。然而,聚类分析表明,这些是由低表现的自闭症亚组(约30%的自闭症个体在至少一项测试中表现低于神经典型平均值的2sd)驱动的,而更大的亚组(约70%)在至少两项测试中表现在1SD内。表现不佳的小组也比表现出色的小组平均有更多的社会沟通困难,杏仁核和梭状回的激活也较低。局限性:自闭症表达识别亚群的发现及其特征需要独立的复制。这目前是不可能的,因为没有其他现有的数据集包括所有相关的措施。然而,我们证明了具有根本不同假设的两种聚类方法之间的结果具有很高的内部稳健性(91.6%),这是独立复制的关键先决条件。结论:我们确定了一个有表情识别困难的自闭症个体亚组,并表明这与临床和神经生物学特征有关。如果复制,表达识别可以作为生物行为分层生物标志物,并有助于开发针对自闭症个体亚群的有针对性的干预措施。
{"title":"Facial expression recognition is linked to clinical and neurofunctional differences in autism.","authors":"Hannah Meyer-Lindenberg,&nbsp;Carolin Moessnang,&nbsp;Bethany Oakley,&nbsp;Jumana Ahmad,&nbsp;Luke Mason,&nbsp;Emily J H Jones,&nbsp;Hannah L Hayward,&nbsp;Jennifer Cooke,&nbsp;Daisy Crawley,&nbsp;Rosemary Holt,&nbsp;Julian Tillmann,&nbsp;Tony Charman,&nbsp;Simon Baron-Cohen,&nbsp;Tobias Banaschewski,&nbsp;Christian Beckmann,&nbsp;Heike Tost,&nbsp;Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg,&nbsp;Jan K Buitelaar,&nbsp;Declan G Murphy,&nbsp;Michael J Brammer,&nbsp;Eva Loth","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00520-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00520-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Difficulties in social communication are a defining clinical feature of autism. However, the underlying neurobiological heterogeneity has impeded targeted therapies and requires new approaches to identifying clinically relevant bio-behavioural subgroups. In the largest autism cohort to date, we comprehensively examined difficulties in facial expression recognition, a key process in social communication, as a bio-behavioural stratification biomarker, and validated them against clinical features and neurofunctional responses.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>Between 255 and 488 participants aged 6-30 years with autism, typical development and/or mild intellectual disability completed the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces task, the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task and/or the Films Expression Task. We first examined mean-group differences on each test. Then, we used a novel intersection approach that compares two centroid and connectivity-based clustering methods to derive subgroups based on the combined performance across the three tasks. Measures and subgroups were then related to clinical features and neurofunctional differences measured using fMRI during a fearful face-matching task.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found significant mean-group differences on each expression recognition test. However, cluster analyses showed that these were driven by a low-performing autistic subgroup (~ 30% of autistic individuals who performed below 2SDs of the neurotypical mean on at least one test), while a larger subgroup (~ 70%) performed within 1SD on at least 2 tests. The low-performing subgroup also had on average significantly more social communication difficulties and lower activation in the amygdala and fusiform gyrus than the high-performing subgroup.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>Findings of autism expression recognition subgroups and their characteristics require independent replication. This is currently not possible, as there is no other existing dataset that includes all relevant measures. However, we demonstrated high internal robustness (91.6%) of findings between two clustering methods with fundamentally different assumptions, which is a critical pre-condition for independent replication.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>We identified a subgroup of autistic individuals with expression recognition difficulties and showed that this related to clinical and neurobiological characteristics. If replicated, expression recognition may serve as bio-behavioural stratification biomarker and aid in the development of targeted interventions for a subgroup of autistic individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-11-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9650909/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10450090","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Predictability modulates neural response to eye contact in ASD. 可预测性会调节 ASD 患者对目光接触的神经反应。
IF 6.3 1区 医学 Q1 GENETICS & HEREDITY Pub Date : 2022-10-29 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00519-0
Adam J Naples, Jennifer H Foss-Feig, Julie M Wolf, Vinod H Srihari, James C McPartland

Background: Deficits in establishing and maintaining eye-contact are early and persistent vulnerabilities of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and the neural bases of these deficits remain elusive. A promising hypothesis is that social features of autism may reflect difficulties in making predictions about the social world under conditions of uncertainty. However, no research in ASD has examined how predictability impacts the neural processing of eye-contact in naturalistic interpersonal interactions.

Method: We used eye tracking to facilitate an interactive social simulation wherein onscreen faces would establish eye-contact when the participant looked at them. In Experiment One, receipt of eye-contact was unpredictable; in Experiment Two, receipt of eye-contact was predictable. Neural response to eye-contact was measured via the N170 and P300 event-related potentials (ERPs). Experiment One included 23 ASD and 46 typically developing (TD) adult participants. Experiment Two included 25 ASD and 43 TD adult participants.

Results: When receipt of eye-contact was unpredictable, individuals with ASD showed increased N170 and increased, but non-specific, P300 responses. The magnitude of the N170 responses correlated with measures of sensory and anxiety symptomology, such that increased response to eye-contact was associated with increased symptomology. However, when receipt of eye-contact was predictable, individuals with ASD, relative to controls, exhibited slower N170s and no differences in the amplitude of N170 or P300.

Limitations: Our ASD sample was composed of adults with IQ > 70 and included only four autistic women. Thus, further research is needed to evaluate how these results generalize across the spectrum of age, sex, and cognitive ability. Additionally, as analyses were exploratory, some findings failed to survive false-discovery rate adjustment.

Conclusions: Neural response to eye-contact in ASD ranged from attenuated to hypersensitive depending on the predictability of the social context. These findings suggest that the vulnerabilities in eye-contact during social interactions in ASD may arise from differences in anticipation and expectation of eye-contact in addition to the perception of gaze alone.

背景:自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)患者在建立和保持目光接触方面存在早期和持续的缺陷,而这些缺陷的神经基础仍然难以捉摸。一个很有希望的假设是,自闭症的社交特征可能反映了在不确定条件下对社会世界进行预测的困难。然而,目前还没有针对自闭症的研究探讨在自然的人际交往中,可预测性如何影响目光接触的神经处理:方法:我们使用眼动追踪来促进互动社交模拟,当被试者注视屏幕上的面孔时,屏幕上的面孔会与之建立目光接触。在实验一中,眼球接触的接收是不可预测的;在实验二中,眼球接触的接收是可预测的。通过 N170 和 P300 事件相关电位(ERPs)测量对目光接触的神经反应。实验一包括 23 名 ASD 和 46 名发育典型(TD)成人参与者。实验二包括 25 名 ASD 和 43 名 TD 成人参与者:结果:当目光接触的接收不可预测时,ASD 患者的 N170 反应增强,P300 反应增强,但无特异性。N170 反应的大小与感觉和焦虑症状的测量结果相关,因此对目光接触的反应增加与症状的增加相关。然而,当目光接触的接收是可预测的时,相对于对照组,ASD患者的N170反应较慢,N170或P300的振幅没有差异:局限性:我们的 ASD 样本由智商大于 70 的成年人组成,其中只有四名女性自闭症患者。因此,还需要进一步的研究来评估这些结果在不同年龄、性别和认知能力的人群中的普遍性。此外,由于分析是探索性的,一些结果未能通过误发现率调整:结论:ASD患者对目光接触的神经反应从减弱到过度敏感不等,这取决于社会环境的可预测性。这些研究结果表明,ASD患者在社交互动过程中目光接触的脆弱性可能源于对目光接触的预期和期望的差异,而不仅仅是对注视的感知。
{"title":"Predictability modulates neural response to eye contact in ASD.","authors":"Adam J Naples, Jennifer H Foss-Feig, Julie M Wolf, Vinod H Srihari, James C McPartland","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00519-0","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s13229-022-00519-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Deficits in establishing and maintaining eye-contact are early and persistent vulnerabilities of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and the neural bases of these deficits remain elusive. A promising hypothesis is that social features of autism may reflect difficulties in making predictions about the social world under conditions of uncertainty. However, no research in ASD has examined how predictability impacts the neural processing of eye-contact in naturalistic interpersonal interactions.</p><p><strong>Method: </strong>We used eye tracking to facilitate an interactive social simulation wherein onscreen faces would establish eye-contact when the participant looked at them. In Experiment One, receipt of eye-contact was unpredictable; in Experiment Two, receipt of eye-contact was predictable. Neural response to eye-contact was measured via the N170 and P300 event-related potentials (ERPs). Experiment One included 23 ASD and 46 typically developing (TD) adult participants. Experiment Two included 25 ASD and 43 TD adult participants.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>When receipt of eye-contact was unpredictable, individuals with ASD showed increased N170 and increased, but non-specific, P300 responses. The magnitude of the N170 responses correlated with measures of sensory and anxiety symptomology, such that increased response to eye-contact was associated with increased symptomology. However, when receipt of eye-contact was predictable, individuals with ASD, relative to controls, exhibited slower N170s and no differences in the amplitude of N170 or P300.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>Our ASD sample was composed of adults with IQ > 70 and included only four autistic women. Thus, further research is needed to evaluate how these results generalize across the spectrum of age, sex, and cognitive ability. Additionally, as analyses were exploratory, some findings failed to survive false-discovery rate adjustment.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Neural response to eye-contact in ASD ranged from attenuated to hypersensitive depending on the predictability of the social context. These findings suggest that the vulnerabilities in eye-contact during social interactions in ASD may arise from differences in anticipation and expectation of eye-contact in addition to the perception of gaze alone.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.3,"publicationDate":"2022-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9618208/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9158315","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beyond the three-chamber test: toward a multimodal and objective assessment of social behavior in rodents. 超越三室试验:对啮齿类动物的社会行为进行多模式客观评估。
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-10-25 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00521-6
Renad Jabarin, Shai Netser, Shlomo Wagner

MAIN: In recent years, substantial advances in social neuroscience have been realized, including the generation of numerous rodent models of autism spectrum disorder. Still, it can be argued that those methods currently being used to analyze animal social behavior create a bottleneck that significantly slows down progress in this field. Indeed, the bulk of research still relies on a small number of simple behavioral paradigms, the results of which are assessed without considering behavioral dynamics. Moreover, only few variables are examined in each paradigm, thus overlooking a significant portion of the complexity that characterizes social interaction between two conspecifics, subsequently hindering our understanding of the neural mechanisms governing different aspects of social behavior. We further demonstrate these constraints by discussing the most commonly used paradigm for assessing rodent social behavior, the three-chamber test. We also point to the fact that although emotions greatly influence human social behavior, we lack reliable means for assessing the emotional state of animals during social tasks. As such, we also discuss current evidence supporting the existence of pro-social emotions and emotional cognition in animal models. We further suggest that adequate social behavior analysis requires a novel multimodal approach that employs automated and simultaneous measurements of multiple behavioral and physiological variables at high temporal resolution in socially interacting animals. We accordingly describe several computerized systems and computational tools for acquiring and analyzing such measurements. Finally, we address several behavioral and physiological variables that can be used to assess socio-emotional states in animal models and thus elucidate intricacies of social behavior so as to attain deeper insight into the brain mechanisms that mediate such behaviors. CONCLUSIONS: In summary, we suggest that combining automated multimodal measurements with machine-learning algorithms will help define socio-emotional states and determine their dynamics during various types of social tasks, thus enabling a more thorough understanding of the complexity of social behavior.

主要:近年来,社会神经科学取得了实质性进展,包括产生了许多自闭症谱系障碍的啮齿动物模型。尽管如此,可以说,目前用于分析动物社会行为的方法造成了一个瓶颈,大大减缓了该领域的进展。事实上,大部分研究仍然依赖于少数简单的行为范式,其结果在评估时没有考虑行为动力学。此外,在每个范式中,只有少数变量被检查,因此忽略了两个同种之间社会互动的复杂性的很大一部分,从而阻碍了我们对控制社会行为不同方面的神经机制的理解。我们通过讨论最常用的评估啮齿动物社会行为的范式,即三室测试,进一步证明了这些限制。我们还指出,尽管情绪在很大程度上影响着人类的社会行为,但我们缺乏可靠的方法来评估动物在社会任务中的情绪状态。因此,我们还讨论了目前支持动物模型中存在亲社会情绪和情绪认知的证据。我们进一步提出,充分的社会行为分析需要一种新的多模式方法,该方法在高时间分辨率下对社会互动动物的多个行为和生理变量进行自动和同时测量。因此,我们描述了几种用于获取和分析此类测量的计算机化系统和计算工具。最后,我们讨论了几个行为和生理变量,这些变量可用于评估动物模型中的社会情绪状态,从而阐明社会行为的复杂性,从而更深入地了解介导这些行为的大脑机制。结论:总之,我们建议将自动多模式测量与机器学习算法相结合,将有助于定义社会情绪状态,并确定其在各种类型的社会任务中的动态,从而能够更彻底地了解社会行为的复杂性。
{"title":"Beyond the three-chamber test: toward a multimodal and objective assessment of social behavior in rodents.","authors":"Renad Jabarin,&nbsp;Shai Netser,&nbsp;Shlomo Wagner","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00521-6","DOIUrl":"10.1186/s13229-022-00521-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>MAIN: In recent years, substantial advances in social neuroscience have been realized, including the generation of numerous rodent models of autism spectrum disorder. Still, it can be argued that those methods currently being used to analyze animal social behavior create a bottleneck that significantly slows down progress in this field. Indeed, the bulk of research still relies on a small number of simple behavioral paradigms, the results of which are assessed without considering behavioral dynamics. Moreover, only few variables are examined in each paradigm, thus overlooking a significant portion of the complexity that characterizes social interaction between two conspecifics, subsequently hindering our understanding of the neural mechanisms governing different aspects of social behavior. We further demonstrate these constraints by discussing the most commonly used paradigm for assessing rodent social behavior, the three-chamber test. We also point to the fact that although emotions greatly influence human social behavior, we lack reliable means for assessing the emotional state of animals during social tasks. As such, we also discuss current evidence supporting the existence of pro-social emotions and emotional cognition in animal models. We further suggest that adequate social behavior analysis requires a novel multimodal approach that employs automated and simultaneous measurements of multiple behavioral and physiological variables at high temporal resolution in socially interacting animals. We accordingly describe several computerized systems and computational tools for acquiring and analyzing such measurements. Finally, we address several behavioral and physiological variables that can be used to assess socio-emotional states in animal models and thus elucidate intricacies of social behavior so as to attain deeper insight into the brain mechanisms that mediate such behaviors. CONCLUSIONS: In summary, we suggest that combining automated multimodal measurements with machine-learning algorithms will help define socio-emotional states and determine their dynamics during various types of social tasks, thus enabling a more thorough understanding of the complexity of social behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9598038/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10455878","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 18
Enhanced fear limits behavioral flexibility in Shank2-deficient mice. 增强的恐惧限制了shank2缺陷小鼠的行为灵活性。
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-10-03 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00518-1
Miru Yun, Eunjoon Kim, Min Whan Jung

Background: A core symptom of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is repetitive and restrictive patterns of behavior. Cognitive inflexibility has been proposed as a potential basis for these symptoms of ASD. More generally, behavioral inflexibility has been proposed to underlie repetitive and restrictive behavior in ASD. Here, we investigated whether and how behavioral flexibility is compromised in a widely used animal model of ASD.

Methods: We compared the behavioral performance of Shank2-knockout mice and wild-type littermates in reversal learning employing a probabilistic classical trace conditioning paradigm. A conditioned stimulus (odor) was paired with an unconditioned appetitive (water, 6 µl) or aversive (air puff) stimulus in a probabilistic manner. We also compared air puff-induced eye closure responses of Shank2-knockout and wild-type mice.

Results: Male, but not female, Shank2-knockout mice showed impaired reversal learning when the expected outcomes consisted of a water reward and a strong air puff. Moreover, male, but not female, Shank2-knockout mice showed stronger anticipatory eye closure responses to the air puff compared to wild-type littermates, raising the possibility that the impairment might reflect enhanced fear. In support of this contention, male Shank2-knockout mice showed intact reversal learning when the strong air puff was replaced with a mild air puff and when the expected outcomes consisted of only rewards.

Limitations: We examined behavioral flexibility in one behavioral task (reversal learning in a probabilistic classical trace conditioning paradigm) using one ASD mouse model (Shank2-knockout mice). Thus, future work is needed to clarify the extent to which our findings (that enhanced fear limits behavioral flexibility in ASD) can explain the behavioral inflexibility associated with ASD. Also, we examined only the relationship between fear and behavioral flexibility, leaving open the question of whether abnormalities in processes other than fear contribute to behavioral inflexibility in ASD. Finally, the neurobiological mechanisms linking Shank2-knockout and enhanced fear remain to be elucidated.

Conclusions: Our results indicate that enhanced fear suppresses reversal learning in the presence of an intact capability to learn cue-outcome contingency changes in Shank2-knockout mice. Our findings suggest that behavioral flexibility might be seriously limited by abnormal emotional responses in ASD.

背景:自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)的核心症状是重复性和限制性行为模式。认知不灵活性被认为是ASD这些症状的潜在基础。更普遍的是,行为不灵活性被认为是ASD中重复性和限制性行为的基础。在这里,我们研究了在广泛使用的ASD动物模型中行为灵活性是否以及如何受到损害。方法:采用概率经典痕迹条件反射范式,比较shank2基因敲除小鼠和野生型小鼠在逆向学习中的行为表现。条件刺激(气味)以概率方式与非条件食欲刺激(水,6µl)或厌恶刺激(吹气)配对。我们还比较了shank2敲除小鼠和野生型小鼠的充气诱导闭眼反应。结果:当预期的结果包括水奖励和强烈的吹气时,雄性而非雌性shank2敲除小鼠表现出逆转学习受损。此外,与野生型小鼠相比,shank2基因敲除的雄性小鼠(而非雌性)对吹气表现出更强的预期闭眼反应,这增加了这种损伤可能反映出恐惧增强的可能性。为了支持这一论点,当用温和的鼓气代替强烈的鼓气时,当预期的结果只包括奖励时,雄性shank2敲除小鼠表现出完整的逆转学习。局限性:我们使用一种ASD小鼠模型(shank2敲除小鼠)研究了一种行为任务(概率经典痕量条件反射范式中的反转学习)的行为灵活性。因此,未来的工作需要澄清我们的发现(增强的恐惧限制了ASD的行为灵活性)在多大程度上可以解释与ASD相关的行为不灵活性。此外,我们只研究了恐惧和行为灵活性之间的关系,留下了一个问题,即除了恐惧之外,其他过程的异常是否会导致ASD的行为不灵活性。最后,尚不清楚shank2基因敲除与恐惧增强之间的神经生物学机制。结论:我们的研究结果表明,在shank2敲除小鼠学习线索-结果偶然性变化的完整能力存在的情况下,增强的恐惧抑制了逆转学习。我们的研究结果表明,行为灵活性可能受到ASD患者异常情绪反应的严重限制。
{"title":"Enhanced fear limits behavioral flexibility in Shank2-deficient mice.","authors":"Miru Yun,&nbsp;Eunjoon Kim,&nbsp;Min Whan Jung","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00518-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00518-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>A core symptom of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is repetitive and restrictive patterns of behavior. Cognitive inflexibility has been proposed as a potential basis for these symptoms of ASD. More generally, behavioral inflexibility has been proposed to underlie repetitive and restrictive behavior in ASD. Here, we investigated whether and how behavioral flexibility is compromised in a widely used animal model of ASD.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>We compared the behavioral performance of Shank2-knockout mice and wild-type littermates in reversal learning employing a probabilistic classical trace conditioning paradigm. A conditioned stimulus (odor) was paired with an unconditioned appetitive (water, 6 µl) or aversive (air puff) stimulus in a probabilistic manner. We also compared air puff-induced eye closure responses of Shank2-knockout and wild-type mice.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Male, but not female, Shank2-knockout mice showed impaired reversal learning when the expected outcomes consisted of a water reward and a strong air puff. Moreover, male, but not female, Shank2-knockout mice showed stronger anticipatory eye closure responses to the air puff compared to wild-type littermates, raising the possibility that the impairment might reflect enhanced fear. In support of this contention, male Shank2-knockout mice showed intact reversal learning when the strong air puff was replaced with a mild air puff and when the expected outcomes consisted of only rewards.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>We examined behavioral flexibility in one behavioral task (reversal learning in a probabilistic classical trace conditioning paradigm) using one ASD mouse model (Shank2-knockout mice). Thus, future work is needed to clarify the extent to which our findings (that enhanced fear limits behavioral flexibility in ASD) can explain the behavioral inflexibility associated with ASD. Also, we examined only the relationship between fear and behavioral flexibility, leaving open the question of whether abnormalities in processes other than fear contribute to behavioral inflexibility in ASD. Finally, the neurobiological mechanisms linking Shank2-knockout and enhanced fear remain to be elucidated.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Our results indicate that enhanced fear suppresses reversal learning in the presence of an intact capability to learn cue-outcome contingency changes in Shank2-knockout mice. Our findings suggest that behavioral flexibility might be seriously limited by abnormal emotional responses in ASD.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-10-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9531513/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"33487162","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Atypical gaze patterns in autistic adults are heterogeneous across but reliable within individuals. 自闭症成人的非典型凝视模式是异质的,但在个体内是可靠的。
IF 6.2 1区 医学 Q1 Neuroscience Pub Date : 2022-09-24 DOI: 10.1186/s13229-022-00517-2
Umit Keles, Dorit Kliemann, Lisa Byrge, Heini Saarimäki, Lynn K Paul, Daniel P Kennedy, Ralph Adolphs

Background: Across behavioral studies, autistic individuals show greater variability than typically developing individuals. However, it remains unknown to what extent this variability arises from heterogeneity across individuals, or from unreliability within individuals. Here, we focus on eye tracking, which provides rich dependent measures that have been used extensively in studies of autism. Autistic individuals have an atypical gaze onto both static visual images and dynamic videos that could be leveraged for diagnostic purposes if the above open question could be addressed.

Methods: We tested three competing hypotheses: (1) that gaze patterns of autistic individuals are less reliable or noisier than those of controls, (2) that atypical gaze patterns are individually reliable but heterogeneous across autistic individuals, or (3) that atypical gaze patterns are individually reliable and also homogeneous among autistic individuals. We collected desktop-based eye tracking data from two different full-length television sitcom episodes, at two independent sites (Caltech and Indiana University), in a total of over 150 adult participants (N = 48 autistic individuals with IQ in the normal range, 105 controls) and quantified gaze onto features of the videos using automated computer vision-based feature extraction.

Results: We found support for the second of these hypotheses. Autistic people and controls showed equivalently reliable gaze onto specific features of videos, such as faces, so much so that individuals could be identified significantly above chance using a fingerprinting approach from video epochs as short as 2 min. However, classification of participants into diagnostic groups based on their eye tracking data failed to produce clear group classifications, due to heterogeneity in the autistic group.

Limitations: Three limitations are the relatively small sample size, assessment across only two videos (from the same television series), and the absence of other dependent measures (e.g., neuroimaging or genetics) that might have revealed individual-level variability that was not evident with eye tracking. Future studies should expand to larger samples across longer longitudinal epochs, an aim that is now becoming feasible with Internet- and phone-based eye tracking.

Conclusions: These findings pave the way for the investigation of autism subtypes, and for elucidating the specific visual features that best discriminate gaze patterns-directions that will also combine with and inform neuroimaging and genetic studies of this complex disorder.

背景:在行为研究中,自闭症个体比正常发育的个体表现出更大的变异性。然而,目前尚不清楚这种可变性在多大程度上是由于个体之间的异质性,还是个体内部的不可靠性。在这里,我们专注于眼动追踪,它提供了丰富的依赖测量,已广泛用于自闭症的研究。自闭症患者对静态视觉图像和动态视频都有非典型的凝视,如果上述开放性问题能够得到解决,这可以用于诊断目的。方法:我们测试了三个相互竞争的假设:(1)自闭症个体的凝视模式比对照组更不可靠或更嘈杂;(2)非典型凝视模式是个体可靠的,但在自闭症个体中是异质的;(3)非典型凝视模式是个体可靠的,但在自闭症个体中也是同质的。我们在两个独立的地点(加州理工学院和印第安纳大学)收集了150多名成年参与者(N = 48名智商在正常范围内的自闭症患者,105名对照组)的两集不同长度的电视情景喜剧的桌面眼动追踪数据,并使用基于自动计算机视觉的特征提取对视频特征进行量化。结果:我们发现了第二个假设的支持。自闭症患者和对照组对视频的特定特征(如面部)表现出同样可靠的凝视,以至于使用指纹识别方法可以从短至2分钟的视频片段中识别出个体。然而,由于自闭症组的异质性,根据参与者的眼动追踪数据将其划分为诊断组未能产生明确的组分类。局限性:三个局限性是相对较小的样本量,仅对两个视频(来自同一电视连续剧)进行评估,以及缺乏其他相关措施(例如,神经影像学或遗传学),这些措施可能揭示了眼动追踪不明显的个体水平变异性。未来的研究应该扩展到更大的样本,跨越更长的纵向时代,这个目标现在已经可以通过基于互联网和手机的眼动追踪来实现。结论:这些发现为研究自闭症亚型铺平了道路,并阐明了最能区分凝视模式的特定视觉特征——这些方向也将与这种复杂疾病的神经成像和遗传研究相结合,并为其提供信息。
{"title":"Atypical gaze patterns in autistic adults are heterogeneous across but reliable within individuals.","authors":"Umit Keles,&nbsp;Dorit Kliemann,&nbsp;Lisa Byrge,&nbsp;Heini Saarimäki,&nbsp;Lynn K Paul,&nbsp;Daniel P Kennedy,&nbsp;Ralph Adolphs","doi":"10.1186/s13229-022-00517-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00517-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Across behavioral studies, autistic individuals show greater variability than typically developing individuals. However, it remains unknown to what extent this variability arises from heterogeneity across individuals, or from unreliability within individuals. Here, we focus on eye tracking, which provides rich dependent measures that have been used extensively in studies of autism. Autistic individuals have an atypical gaze onto both static visual images and dynamic videos that could be leveraged for diagnostic purposes if the above open question could be addressed.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>We tested three competing hypotheses: (1) that gaze patterns of autistic individuals are less reliable or noisier than those of controls, (2) that atypical gaze patterns are individually reliable but heterogeneous across autistic individuals, or (3) that atypical gaze patterns are individually reliable and also homogeneous among autistic individuals. We collected desktop-based eye tracking data from two different full-length television sitcom episodes, at two independent sites (Caltech and Indiana University), in a total of over 150 adult participants (N = 48 autistic individuals with IQ in the normal range, 105 controls) and quantified gaze onto features of the videos using automated computer vision-based feature extraction.</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>We found support for the second of these hypotheses. Autistic people and controls showed equivalently reliable gaze onto specific features of videos, such as faces, so much so that individuals could be identified significantly above chance using a fingerprinting approach from video epochs as short as 2 min. However, classification of participants into diagnostic groups based on their eye tracking data failed to produce clear group classifications, due to heterogeneity in the autistic group.</p><p><strong>Limitations: </strong>Three limitations are the relatively small sample size, assessment across only two videos (from the same television series), and the absence of other dependent measures (e.g., neuroimaging or genetics) that might have revealed individual-level variability that was not evident with eye tracking. Future studies should expand to larger samples across longer longitudinal epochs, an aim that is now becoming feasible with Internet- and phone-based eye tracking.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>These findings pave the way for the investigation of autism subtypes, and for elucidating the specific visual features that best discriminate gaze patterns-directions that will also combine with and inform neuroimaging and genetic studies of this complex disorder.</p>","PeriodicalId":18733,"journal":{"name":"Molecular Autism","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":6.2,"publicationDate":"2022-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9508778/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9548226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Molecular Autism
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1