首页 > 最新文献

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Predicting the memorability of scene pictures: Improved accuracy through one's own experience. 快讯预测场景图片的可记忆性:通过自身经验提高准确性。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-28 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241239829
Sofia Navarro-Báez, Monika Undorf, Arndt Bröder

There are conflicting findings regarding the accuracy of metamemory for scene pictures. Judgements of stimulus memorability in general (memorability judgements [MJs]) have been reported to be unpredictive of actual image memorability. However, other studies have found that judgements of learning (JOLs)-predictions of one's own later memory performance for recently studied items-are moderately predictive of people's own actual recognition memory for pictures. The current study directly compared the relative accuracy and cue basis of JOLs and MJs for scene pictures. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants completed an MJ task and a JOL task in counterbalanced order. In the MJ task, they judged the general memorability of each picture. In the JOL task, they studied pictures and made JOLs during a learning phase, followed by a recognition memory test. Results showed that MJs were predictive of general scene memorability and relied on the same cues as JOLs, but MJ accuracy considerably improved after the JOL task. Experiment 3 demonstrated that prior learning experiences drove this increase in MJ accuracy. This work demonstrates that people can predict not only their own future memory performance for scene pictures with moderate accuracy but also the general memorability of scene pictures. In addition, experiences with one's own learning and memory support the ability to assess scene memorability in general. This research contributes to our understanding of the basis and accuracy of different metamemory judgements.

关于场景图片元记忆的准确性,研究结果相互矛盾。据报道,对刺激物可记忆性的一般判断(可记忆性判断,MJs)无法预测实际图像的可记忆性。然而,其他研究发现,学习判断(JOLs)--即预测自己以后对最近学习过的项目的记忆表现--对人们自己对图片的实际识别记忆有一定的预测作用。本研究直接比较了 JOL 和 MJ 对场景图片的相对准确性和线索基础。在实验 1 和 2 中,受试者以平衡的顺序完成了 MJ 任务和 JOL 任务。在 MJ 任务中,他们判断每张图片的一般可记性。在 JOL 任务中,他们在学习阶段学习图片并进行 JOL,然后进行识别记忆测试。结果表明,MJ 可以预测一般场景的可记忆性,并且与 JOL 一样依赖于相同的线索,但在完成 JOL 任务后,MJ 的准确性大大提高。实验 3 表明,先前的学习经验推动了 MJ 准确率的提高。这项研究表明,人们不仅能以中等准确度预测自己未来对场景图片的记忆表现,还能预测场景图片的总体可记性。此外,与自身学习和记忆有关的经验也有助于评估一般场景的可记性。这项研究有助于我们理解不同元记忆判断的基础和准确性。
{"title":"Predicting the memorability of scene pictures: Improved accuracy through one's own experience.","authors":"Sofia Navarro-Báez, Monika Undorf, Arndt Bröder","doi":"10.1177/17470218241239829","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241239829","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There are conflicting findings regarding the accuracy of metamemory for scene pictures. Judgements of stimulus memorability in general (<i>memorability judgements</i> [MJs]) have been reported to be unpredictive of actual image memorability. However, other studies have found that <i>judgements of learning</i> (JOLs)-predictions of one's own later memory performance for recently studied items-are moderately predictive of people's own actual recognition memory for pictures. The current study directly compared the relative accuracy and cue basis of JOLs and MJs for scene pictures. In Experiments 1 and 2, participants completed an MJ task and a JOL task in counterbalanced order. In the MJ task, they judged the general memorability of each picture. In the JOL task, they studied pictures and made JOLs during a learning phase, followed by a recognition memory test. Results showed that MJs were predictive of general scene memorability and relied on the same cues as JOLs, but MJ accuracy considerably improved after the JOL task. Experiment 3 demonstrated that prior learning experiences drove this increase in MJ accuracy. This work demonstrates that people can predict not only their own future memory performance for scene pictures with moderate accuracy but also the general memorability of scene pictures. In addition, experiences with one's own learning and memory support the ability to assess scene memorability in general. This research contributes to our understanding of the basis and accuracy of different metamemory judgements.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"546-565"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11874493/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140028841","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
EXPRESS: The Relationship between Visuospatial and Analogical Reasoning.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251327076
Daniel Corral, Heather Burte

We report six experiments that examine the relationship between visuospatial and analogical reasoning. In Experiments 1-3b, participants completed a series of spatial assessments and analogical reasoning tasks. In Experiment 2, participants were assigned to one of three training conditions that involved analogical reasoning. One group visualized the elements in each scenario (visualization training), a second group identified the spatial relationships in each scenario (spatial training), and a third group identified the corresponding elements between two scenarios (analogy training). Participants completed pre- and post-tests, wherein they solved various analogy problems; Experiments 3a-4b were similar but did not include an analogy training condition. In Experiments 1-3b, a positive relationship between visuospatial and analogical reasoning was observed (regardless of the perceptibility of the analogy's spatial relations), as participants who performed better on the spatial measures also demonstrated better analogical reasoning. Furthermore, in Experiments 2 and 4b, spatial training led to greater posttest performance than the visualization training, but this outcome was not observed in Experiments 3a-4a. A combined analysis (Experiments 2-4b), however, revealed a small, but reliable advantage of spatial over visualization training. These findings suggest that strategies that encourage spatial reasoning might better aid analogical learning and reasoning than strategies that encourage visualization.

{"title":"EXPRESS: The Relationship between Visuospatial and Analogical Reasoning.","authors":"Daniel Corral, Heather Burte","doi":"10.1177/17470218251327076","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218251327076","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We report six experiments that examine the relationship between visuospatial and analogical reasoning. In Experiments 1-3b, participants completed a series of spatial assessments and analogical reasoning tasks. In Experiment 2, participants were assigned to one of three training conditions that involved analogical reasoning. One group visualized the elements in each scenario (visualization training), a second group identified the spatial relationships in each scenario (spatial training), and a third group identified the corresponding elements between two scenarios (analogy training). Participants completed pre- and post-tests, wherein they solved various analogy problems; Experiments 3a-4b were similar but did not include an analogy training condition. In Experiments 1-3b, a positive relationship between visuospatial and analogical reasoning was observed (regardless of the perceptibility of the analogy's spatial relations), as participants who performed better on the spatial measures also demonstrated better analogical reasoning. Furthermore, in Experiments 2 and 4b, spatial training led to greater posttest performance than the visualization training, but this outcome was not observed in Experiments 3a-4a. A combined analysis (Experiments 2-4b), however, revealed a small, but reliable advantage of spatial over visualization training. These findings suggest that strategies that encourage spatial reasoning might better aid analogical learning and reasoning than strategies that encourage visualization.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251327076"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143531867","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lack of effects of acute exercise intensity on mnemonic discrimination. 快讯:急性运动强度对记忆辨别缺乏影响。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-22 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241238881
Paul D Loprinzi, Jeremy B Caplan

The hippocampus is thought to support episodic memory by pattern separation, thereby supporting the ability to discriminate high similarity items. Past research evaluating whether acute exercise can improve mnemonic discrimination of high similarity items is mixed. The present experiment attempts to extend these prior mixed findings by evaluating the effects of multiple exercise intensities on hippocampal-dependent, mnemonic discrimination and memory performance. Fifty-seven young adults completed a three-condition (control, moderate-intensity, and vigorous-intensity), within-subjects crossover pretest-posttest comparison. We observed no effects of acute exercise on recognition memory or mnemonic discrimination. We discuss the implications of these null findings with the broader literature by discussing the complexity of this potential exercise-mnemonic discrimination relationship, including the unique role of exercise intensity, differences in the level of processing (e.g., conceptual vs. perceptual), and unique brain regions involved in mnemonic discrimination.

海马体被认为通过模式分离来支持记忆,从而支持辨别高相似度项目的能力。过去关于急性运动是否能改善高相似度项目的记忆辨别能力的研究结果不一。本实验试图通过评估多种运动强度对依赖于海马的记忆辨别能力和记忆表现的影响,来扩展这些先前的混合研究结果。57 名年轻人完成了一项三条件(对照组、中等强度组和剧烈强度组)、受试者内交叉的前测-后测比较。我们没有观察到急性运动对识别记忆或记忆辨别力的影响。我们通过讨论这种潜在的运动-记忆辨别关系的复杂性,包括运动强度的独特作用、处理水平的差异(如概念与知觉)以及参与记忆辨别的独特脑区,讨论了这些无效研究结果对更广泛文献的影响。
{"title":"Lack of effects of acute exercise intensity on mnemonic discrimination.","authors":"Paul D Loprinzi, Jeremy B Caplan","doi":"10.1177/17470218241238881","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241238881","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The hippocampus is thought to support episodic memory by pattern separation, thereby supporting the ability to discriminate high similarity items. Past research evaluating whether acute exercise can improve mnemonic discrimination of high similarity items is mixed. The present experiment attempts to extend these prior mixed findings by evaluating the effects of multiple exercise intensities on hippocampal-dependent, mnemonic discrimination and memory performance. Fifty-seven young adults completed a three-condition (control, moderate-intensity, and vigorous-intensity), within-subjects crossover pretest-posttest comparison. We observed no effects of acute exercise on recognition memory or mnemonic discrimination. We discuss the implications of these null findings with the broader literature by discussing the complexity of this potential exercise-mnemonic discrimination relationship, including the unique role of exercise intensity, differences in the level of processing (e.g., conceptual vs. perceptual), and unique brain regions involved in mnemonic discrimination.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"534-545"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139997272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A fragile effect: The influence of episodic memory on delay discounting. 快讯脆弱的效应:外显记忆对延迟折扣的影响。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-31 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241239289
Nicky Duff, Rebecca Olsen, Zoe Walsh, Karen Salmon, Maree Hunt, Anne Macaskill

Delay discounting occurs when a reward loses value as a function of delay. Episodic future thinking (EFT) reliably decreases delay discounting. EFT may share cognitive features with recalling episodic memories such as constructive episodic simulation. We therefore explored whether recalling episodic memories also reduces delay discounting. In Experiment 1, participants wrote about episodic memories and recalled those memories before completing a delay discounting task. Episodic memories reduced delay discounting according to one commonly used delay discounting measure (area under the curve) but not another (using the hyperbolic model). Experiment 2 compared the effects of general and episodic memories. Neither general nor episodic memories significantly decreased delay discounting compared with a control "counting" condition, but episodic memories reduced delay discounting compared with general memories under some conditions. In Experiment 3, episodic memories did not decrease delay discounting compared with three other control conditions while EFT did. Experiment 3 therefore found that thinking must be both episodic and future orientated to reduce delay discounting. Together, these results suggest that episodic thinking is not sufficient to reliably decrease delay discounting, rather, features unique to episodic future thinking are required. Episodic memory might reduce delay discounting in some contexts, but this effect is small and fragile.

当奖赏的价值随着延迟而降低时,就会出现延迟折现。外显未来思维(EFT)能可靠地降低延迟折现。外显未来思维可能与回忆外显记忆(如建构性外显模拟)具有共同的认知特征。因此,我们探讨了回忆外显记忆是否也会降低延迟折现。在实验 1 中,受试者写下外显记忆,并在完成延迟折现任务前回忆这些记忆。根据一种常用的延迟折现测量方法(曲线下面积),外显记忆降低了延迟折现,而另一种方法(使用双曲线模型)则没有降低延迟折现。实验 2 比较了一般记忆和外显记忆的效果。与对照组的 "计数 "条件相比,一般记忆和外显记忆都没有显著降低延迟折现,但在某些条件下,外显记忆比一般记忆降低了延迟折现。在实验 3 中,与其他三个对照条件相比,外显记忆没有降低延迟折现,而 EFT 则降低了延迟折现。因此,实验 3 发现,要减少延迟折现,思维必须同时具有外显性和未来导向性。总之,这些结果表明,外显思维不足以可靠地减少延迟折现,相反,外显未来思维需要有独特的特征。在某些情况下,外显记忆可能会减少延迟折现,但这种影响很小,而且很脆弱。
{"title":"A fragile effect: The influence of episodic memory on delay discounting.","authors":"Nicky Duff, Rebecca Olsen, Zoe Walsh, Karen Salmon, Maree Hunt, Anne Macaskill","doi":"10.1177/17470218241239289","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241239289","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Delay discounting occurs when a reward loses value as a function of delay. Episodic future thinking (EFT) reliably decreases delay discounting. EFT may share cognitive features with recalling episodic memories such as constructive episodic simulation. We therefore explored whether recalling episodic memories also reduces delay discounting. In Experiment 1, participants wrote about episodic memories and recalled those memories before completing a delay discounting task. Episodic memories reduced delay discounting according to one commonly used delay discounting measure (area under the curve) but not another (using the hyperbolic model). Experiment 2 compared the effects of general and episodic memories. Neither general nor episodic memories significantly decreased delay discounting compared with a control \"counting\" condition, but episodic memories reduced delay discounting compared with general memories under some conditions. In Experiment 3, episodic memories did not decrease delay discounting compared with three other control conditions while EFT did. Experiment 3 therefore found that thinking must be both episodic <i>and</i> future orientated to reduce delay discounting. Together, these results suggest that episodic thinking is not sufficient to reliably decrease delay discounting, rather, features unique to episodic <i>future</i> thinking are required. Episodic memory might reduce delay discounting in some contexts, but this effect is small and fragile.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"514-533"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140013249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The development of lexical processing: Real-time phonological competition and semantic activation in school age children. 表达:词汇加工的发展:学龄儿童的实时语音竞争和语义激活。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-17 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241244799
Charlotte Jeppsen, Keith Baxelbaum, Bruce Tomblin, Kelsey Klein, Bob McMurray

Prior research suggests that the development of speech perception and word recognition stabilises in early childhood. However, recent work suggests that development of these processes continues throughout adolescence. This study aimed to investigate whether these developmental changes are based solely within the lexical system or are due to domain general changes, and to extend this investigation to lexical-semantic processing. We used two Visual World Paradigm tasks: one to examine phonological and semantic processing, one to capture non-linguistic domain-general skills. We tested 43 seven- to nine-year-olds, 42 ten- to thirteen-year-olds, and 30 sixteen- to seventeen-year-olds. Older children were quicker to fixate the target word and exhibited earlier onset and offset of fixations to both semantic and phonological competitors. Visual/cognitive skills explained significant, but not all, variance in the development of these effects. Developmental changes in semantic activation were largely attributable to changes in upstream phonological processing. These results suggest that the concurrent development of linguistic processes and broader visual/cognitive skills lead to developmental changes in real-time phonological competition, while semantic activation is more stable across these ages.

先前的研究表明,实时语音竞争过程在幼儿期已经稳定下来(Fernald 等人,2006 年)。然而,最近的研究表明,这些过程的发展会持续到整个青春期(Huang & Snedeker, 2011; Rigler et al.)本研究旨在调查这些发展变化是仅仅基于词汇系统还是由于领域的总体变化。本研究还旨在调查实时词汇-语义激活的发展情况。我们使用两个视觉世界范式实验,分别对 43 名 7-9 岁儿童、42 名 10-13 岁儿童和 30 名 16-17 岁儿童进行了语义激活语音竞争和非语言领域一般处理技能的研究。年龄较大的儿童对目标单词的固着更快,对语义和语音竞争者的固着出现和偏移也更早。视觉/认知技能可以解释这些效应发展过程中的显著差异,但不是全部差异。语义激活的发展变化主要归因于语音处理的变化。这些结果表明,语言过程和更广泛的视觉/认知技能的同时发展导致了实时语音竞争的发展变化,而语义激活在这些年龄段是稳定的。
{"title":"The development of lexical processing: Real-time phonological competition and semantic activation in school age children.","authors":"Charlotte Jeppsen, Keith Baxelbaum, Bruce Tomblin, Kelsey Klein, Bob McMurray","doi":"10.1177/17470218241244799","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241244799","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research suggests that the development of speech perception and word recognition stabilises in early childhood. However, recent work suggests that development of these processes continues throughout adolescence. This study aimed to investigate whether these developmental changes are based solely within the lexical system or are due to domain general changes, and to extend this investigation to lexical-semantic processing. We used two Visual World Paradigm tasks: one to examine phonological and semantic processing, one to capture non-linguistic domain-general skills. We tested 43 seven- to nine-year-olds, 42 ten- to thirteen-year-olds, and 30 sixteen- to seventeen-year-olds. Older children were quicker to fixate the target word and exhibited earlier onset and offset of fixations to both semantic and phonological competitors. Visual/cognitive skills explained significant, but not all, variance in the development of these effects. Developmental changes in semantic activation were largely attributable to changes in upstream phonological processing. These results suggest that the concurrent development of linguistic processes and broader visual/cognitive skills lead to developmental changes in real-time phonological competition, while semantic activation is more stable across these ages.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"437-458"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140176144","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reduced learning rates but successful learning of a coordinated rhythmic movement by older adults. 快讯老年人学习协调节奏运动的学习率降低,但学习成功。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-12 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241240983
Daniel Leach, Zoe Kolokotroni, Andrew D Wilson

Previous work has investigated the information-based mechanism for learning and transfer of learning in coordinated rhythmic movements. In those papers, we trained young adults to produce either 90° or 60° and showed in both cases that learning entailed learning to use relative position as information for the relative phase. This variable then supported transfer of learning to untrained coordinations +/30° on either side. In this article, we replicate the 90° study with younger adults and extend it by training older adults (aged between 55 and 65 years). Other work has revealed a steep decline in learning rate around this age, and no follow-up study has been able to successfully train older adults to perform a novel coordination. We used a more intensive training paradigm and showed that while older adult learning rates remain about half that of younger adults, given time they are able to acquire the new coordination. They also learn to use relative position, and consequently show the same pattern of transfer. We discuss implications for attempts to model the process of learning in this task.

之前的研究已经对协调节奏运动中基于信息的学习和学习转移机制进行了研究(Leach, Kolokotroni & Wilson, 2021a,b)。在这些论文中,我们训练年轻成人做出 90° 或 60° 的动作,结果表明,在这两种情况下,学习都需要学会使用相对位置作为相对相位的信息。然后,这一变量支持将学习转移到未经训练的两侧+/30°的协调上。在本文中,我们用较年轻的成年人重复了 90° 的研究,并通过训练较年长的成年人(年龄在 55 岁至 65 岁之间)对其进行了扩展。其他研究(如 Coats、Wilson、Snapp-Childs、Fath 和 Bingham,2014 年)显示,老年人在这个年龄段的学习率会急剧下降,而且还没有后续研究能够成功训练老年人完成新的协调动作。我们采用了一种强度更大的训练范式,结果表明,虽然老年人的学习率仍然只有年轻人的一半左右,但只要假以时日,他们就能掌握新的协调能力。他们还学会了使用相对位置,并因此表现出相同的迁移模式。我们讨论了在这项任务中尝试建立学习过程模型的意义。
{"title":"Reduced learning rates but successful learning of a coordinated rhythmic movement by older adults.","authors":"Daniel Leach, Zoe Kolokotroni, Andrew D Wilson","doi":"10.1177/17470218241240983","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241240983","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous work has investigated the information-based mechanism for learning and transfer of learning in coordinated rhythmic movements. In those papers, we trained young adults to produce either 90° or 60° and showed in both cases that learning entailed learning to use relative position as information for the relative phase. This variable then supported transfer of learning to untrained coordinations +/30° on either side. In this article, we replicate the 90° study with younger adults and extend it by training older adults (aged between 55 and 65 years). Other work has revealed a steep decline in learning rate around this age, and no follow-up study has been able to successfully train older adults to perform a novel coordination. We used a more intensive training paradigm and showed that while older adult learning rates remain about half that of younger adults, given time they are able to acquire the new coordination. They also learn to use relative position, and consequently show the same pattern of transfer. We discuss implications for attempts to model the process of learning in this task.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"498-513"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11874608/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140065796","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-related contextual cueing features are more evident in reaction variability than in reaction time. 表达:与年龄相关的情境提示特征在反应变异性中比在反应时间中更为明显。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-05 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241241954
Yipeng Yao, Rong Luo, Chengyu Fan, Yeke Qian, Xuelian Zang

Visual-spatial contextual cueing learning underpins the daily lives of older adults, enabling them to navigate their surroundings, perform daily activities, and maintain cognitive function. While the contextual cueing effect has received increasing attention from researchers, the relationship between this cognitive ability and healthy ageing remains controversial. To investigate whether visual-spatial contextual cueing learning declines with age, we examined the contextual learning patterns of older (60-71 years old) and younger adults (18-26 years old) using a contextual-guided visual search paradigm and response variability measurements. We observed significant contextual learning effects in both age groups, impacting response speed and variability, with these effects persisting for at least 24 days. However, older adults required more repetitions and memorised fewer repeated stimuli during initial learning. Interestingly, their long-term memory maintenance appeared stronger, as their contextual facilitation persisted in both response speed and variability, while younger adults only persisted in response speed but not variability. Overall, our results suggest an age-related complex and diverse contextual cueing pattern, with older adults showing weaker learning but stronger long-term memory maintenance compared with younger adults.

视觉空间情境提示学习是老年人日常生活的基础,它使老年人能够浏览周围环境、进行日常活动并保持认知功能。虽然语境提示效应越来越受到研究人员的关注,但这种认知能力与健康老龄化之间的关系仍存在争议。为了研究视觉空间情境提示学习是否会随着年龄的增长而下降,我们采用情境引导视觉搜索范式和反应变异性测量方法,研究了老年人(60-71 岁)和年轻人(18-26 岁)的情境学习模式。我们在两个年龄组中都观察到了明显的情境学习效应,影响了反应速度和变异性,这些效应至少持续了 24 天。然而,在初始学习过程中,老年人需要重复的次数更多,记住的重复刺激更少。有趣的是,他们的长期记忆维持能力似乎更强,因为他们的情境促进作用在反应速度和变异性方面都持续存在,而年轻人只在反应速度方面持续存在,在变异性方面却没有。总之,我们的研究结果表明,与年龄相关的情境提示模式复杂多样,与年轻人相比,老年人的学习能力较弱,但长期记忆保持能力较强。
{"title":"Age-related contextual cueing features are more evident in reaction variability than in reaction time.","authors":"Yipeng Yao, Rong Luo, Chengyu Fan, Yeke Qian, Xuelian Zang","doi":"10.1177/17470218241241954","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241241954","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual-spatial contextual cueing learning underpins the daily lives of older adults, enabling them to navigate their surroundings, perform daily activities, and maintain cognitive function. While the contextual cueing effect has received increasing attention from researchers, the relationship between this cognitive ability and healthy ageing remains controversial. To investigate whether visual-spatial contextual cueing learning declines with age, we examined the contextual learning patterns of older (60-71 years old) and younger adults (18-26 years old) using a contextual-guided visual search paradigm and response variability measurements. We observed significant contextual learning effects in both age groups, impacting response speed and variability, with these effects persisting for at least 24 days. However, older adults required more repetitions and memorised fewer repeated stimuli during initial learning. Interestingly, their long-term memory maintenance appeared stronger, as their contextual facilitation persisted in both response speed and variability, while younger adults only persisted in response speed but not variability. Overall, our results suggest an age-related complex and diverse contextual cueing pattern, with older adults showing weaker learning but stronger long-term memory maintenance compared with younger adults.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"604-618"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11874583/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140132429","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fixation offset decreases manual inhibition of return (IOR) in detection and discrimination tasks. 表达:在检测和辨别任务中,定点偏移会降低手动抑制返回(IOR)。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-07 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241240978
Łukasz Michalczyk

Attention can be covertly shifted to peripheral stimuli to improve their processing. However, attention is also then inhibited against returning to the previously attended location; thus, both detection and discrimination of a stimulus presented at that location decrease (the inhibition of return [IOR] effect). The after-effect of the covert orienting hypothesis postulates a close link between attention shifting, IOR, and oculomotor control. The fixation offset, which improves the generation of saccades, decreases IOR in detection tasks, suggesting a close link between IOR and oculomotor control. However, according to some alternative views (e.g., the input-based IOR hypothesis and the object files segregation/integration hypothesis), IOR may be related to some sensory rather than motor processes. Some studies support that view and show that IOR may occur differently in detection and discrimination tasks and that oculomotor processes do not affect IOR in tasks where manual responses are required and eye movements are suppressed. Two experiments presented in this article show that removing the fixation point decreases manual IOR in detection and discrimination tasks. The results are discussed in terms of various theoretical approaches.

注意力可以隐蔽地转移到周围的刺激物上,以改善对这些刺激物的处理。然而,注意力随后也会受到抑制,无法回到之前注意的位置;因此,对出现在该位置的刺激物的检测和辨别能力都会下降(抑制返回效应;IOR)。隐蔽定向后效应假说(ACOH)认为,注意力转移、IOR 和眼球运动控制之间存在密切联系。在检测任务中,固定偏移会改善囊回的产生,从而降低 IOR,这表明 IOR 与眼球运动控制之间存在密切联系。然而,根据另一些观点(如基于输入的 IOR 假说和物体档案分离/整合假说),IOR 可能与某些感觉过程而非运动过程有关。一些研究支持这一观点,并表明在检测和辨别任务中,IOR 的发生可能不同,而且在需要手动反应和眼球运动被抑制的任务中,眼球运动过程不会影响 IOR。本文介绍的两个实验表明,在检测和辨别任务中,移除固定点会降低手动IOR。本文从各种理论方法的角度对实验结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"Fixation offset decreases manual inhibition of return (IOR) in detection and discrimination tasks.","authors":"Łukasz Michalczyk","doi":"10.1177/17470218241240978","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241240978","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Attention can be covertly shifted to peripheral stimuli to improve their processing. However, attention is also then inhibited against returning to the previously attended location; thus, both detection and discrimination of a stimulus presented at that location decrease (the inhibition of return [IOR] effect). The after-effect of the covert orienting hypothesis postulates a close link between attention shifting, IOR, and oculomotor control. The fixation offset, which improves the generation of saccades, decreases IOR in detection tasks, suggesting a close link between IOR and oculomotor control. However, according to some alternative views (e.g., the input-based IOR hypothesis and the object files segregation/integration hypothesis), IOR may be related to some sensory rather than motor processes. Some studies support that view and show that IOR may occur differently in detection and discrimination tasks and that oculomotor processes do not affect IOR in tasks where manual responses are required and eye movements are suppressed. Two experiments presented in this article show that removing the fixation point decreases manual IOR in detection and discrimination tasks. The results are discussed in terms of various theoretical approaches.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"594-603"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140065795","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pre-crastination across physical and cognitive tasks. 体力和认知任务中的前期拖延症。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241246972
Adi David, Justyne Ingwu, Nicole Meselsohn, Clara Retzloff, Thomas G Hutcheon

Pre-crastination refers to the tendency to begin a task as soon as possible, even at the cost of additional effort. This phenomenon is consistently observed in tasks in which participants are asked to select one of two buckets to carry to a target. Surprisingly, on a high proportion of trials participants choose the bucket that is closer to them (and further from the target) as opposed to the bucket that is further from them (and closer to the target). In other words, participants tend to complete the task of picking up a bucket as soon as possible, even when this requires additional physical effort. The purpose of the current experiment was to test whether an individual's tendency to pre-crastinate is stable across tasks. Participants performed a physical load task where they selected one of two buckets to carry to a target. The same participants performed a cognitive load task where they picked up number strings at one of the two bucket locations and mentally carried the number string to a target. We found that participants pre-crastinated in both tasks and this tendency was reduced as task difficulty increased. Importantly, we found a significant association between an individual's tendency to pre-crastinate in the physical load task and their tendency to pre-crastinate in the cognitive load task. Thus, an individual's tendency to pre-crastinate is consistent across tasks and suggests that this is a stable characteristic of how individuals choose to order tasks.

预先拖延指的是尽快开始一项任务的倾向,甚至不惜付出额外的努力。在要求参与者从两个水桶中选择一个拎到目标地点的任务中,我们可以持续观察到这种现象(Rosenbaum 等人,2014 年)。令人惊讶的是,在很大比例的试验中,参与者会选择离他们更近(但离目标更远)的桶,而不是离他们更远(但离目标更近)的桶。换句话说,参与者倾向于尽快完成拾取水桶的任务,即使这需要付出额外的体力。本实验的目的是测试个体在不同任务中的预碰撞倾向是否稳定。参与者进行了一项体力负荷任务,从两个水桶中选择一个拎到目标地点。同样的参与者还进行了一项认知负荷任务,即在两个水桶中的一个位置拾取数字串,然后在头脑中将数字串搬运到目标处。我们发现,受试者在这两项任务中都出现了预先拖延的现象,而且随着任务难度的增加,这种倾向有所减弱。重要的是,我们发现个体在体力负荷任务中的预先拖延倾向与他们在认知负荷任务中的预先拖延倾向之间存在显著关联。因此,个体在不同任务中的预先拖延倾向是一致的,这表明这是个体选择任务顺序的一个稳定特征。
{"title":"Pre-crastination across physical and cognitive tasks.","authors":"Adi David, Justyne Ingwu, Nicole Meselsohn, Clara Retzloff, Thomas G Hutcheon","doi":"10.1177/17470218241246972","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241246972","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Pre-crastination refers to the tendency to begin a task as soon as possible, even at the cost of additional effort. This phenomenon is consistently observed in tasks in which participants are asked to select one of two buckets to carry to a target. Surprisingly, on a high proportion of trials participants choose the bucket that is closer to them (and further from the target) as opposed to the bucket that is further from them (and closer to the target). In other words, participants tend to complete the task of picking up a bucket as soon as possible, even when this requires additional physical effort. The purpose of the current experiment was to test whether an individual's tendency to pre-crastinate is stable across tasks. Participants performed a physical load task where they selected one of two buckets to carry to a target. The same participants performed a cognitive load task where they picked up number strings at one of the two bucket locations and mentally carried the number string to a target. We found that participants pre-crastinated in both tasks and this tendency was reduced as task difficulty increased. Importantly, we found a significant association between an individual's tendency to pre-crastinate in the physical load task and their tendency to pre-crastinate in the cognitive load task. Thus, an individual's tendency to pre-crastinate is consistent across tasks and suggests that this is a stable characteristic of how individuals choose to order tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"490-497"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140336700","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stimulus specificity in combined action observation and motor imagery of typing. 表达:打字动作观察与运动想象相结合的刺激特异性。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241241502
Camilla Woodrow-Hill, Emma Gowen, Stefan Vogt, Eve Edmonds, Ellen Poliakoff

Combined action observation and motor imagery (AO + MI) can improve movement execution (ME) in healthy adults and certain patient populations. However, it is unclear how the specificity of the observation component during AO + MI influences ME. As generalised observation could result in more flexible AO + MI rehabilitation programmes, this study investigated whether observing typing of target words (specific condition) or non-matching words (general condition) during AO + MI would have different effects on keyboard typing in healthy young adults. In Experiment 1, 51 students imagined typing a target word while watching typing videos that were either specific to the target word or general. There were no differences in typing execution between AO + MI conditions, though participants typed more slowly after both AO + MI conditions compared with no observation or imagery. Experiment 2 repeated Experiment 1 in 20 students, but with a faster stimulus speed in the AO + MI conditions and increased cognitive difficulty in the control condition. The results showed that the slowed typing after AO + MI was likely due to a strong influence of task-switching between imagery and execution, as well as an automatic imitation effect. Both experiments demonstrate that general and specific AO + MI comparably affect ME. In addition, slower ME following both AO + MI and a challenging cognitive task provides support for the motor-cognitive model of MI.

结合动作观察和运动想象(AO+MI)可以改善健康成年人和某些患者的运动执行力(ME)。然而,目前还不清楚 AO+MI 过程中观察部分的特异性如何影响 ME。由于泛化观察可使 AO+MI 康复计划更加灵活,本研究调查了在 AO+MI 过程中观察目标单词(特定条件)或非匹配单词(泛化条件)的键入是否会对健康青壮年的键盘键入产生不同的影响。在实验 1 中,51 名学生一边观看打字视频,一边想象输入目标单词,这些视频要么是目标单词的特定视频,要么是一般视频。在 "AO+MI "条件下,学生的键入速度没有差异,但在 "AO+MI "条件下,学生的键入速度比没有观察或想象的要慢。实验 2 在 20 名学生中重复了实验 1,但在 AO+MI 条件下,刺激速度更快,而在对照条件下,认知难度增加。结果表明,AO+MI 后打字速度变慢可能是由于意象和执行之间的任务切换以及自动模仿效应的强烈影响。这两项实验都证明,一般和特定的 AO+MI 对 ME 的影响相当。此外,AO+MI 和一项具有挑战性的认知任务之后的 ME 速度都较慢,这为 MI 的运动认知模型提供了支持。
{"title":"Stimulus specificity in combined action observation and motor imagery of typing.","authors":"Camilla Woodrow-Hill, Emma Gowen, Stefan Vogt, Eve Edmonds, Ellen Poliakoff","doi":"10.1177/17470218241241502","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241241502","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Combined action observation and motor imagery (AO + MI) can improve movement execution (ME) in healthy adults and certain patient populations. However, it is unclear how the specificity of the observation component during AO + MI influences ME. As generalised observation could result in more flexible AO + MI rehabilitation programmes, this study investigated whether observing typing of target words (specific condition) or non-matching words (general condition) during AO + MI would have different effects on keyboard typing in healthy young adults. In Experiment 1, 51 students imagined typing a target word while watching typing videos that were either specific to the target word or general. There were no differences in typing execution between AO + MI conditions, though participants typed more slowly after both AO + MI conditions compared with no observation or imagery. Experiment 2 repeated Experiment 1 in 20 students, but with a faster stimulus speed in the AO + MI conditions and increased cognitive difficulty in the control condition. The results showed that the slowed typing after AO + MI was likely due to a strong influence of task-switching between imagery and execution, as well as an automatic imitation effect. Both experiments demonstrate that general and specific AO + MI comparably affect ME. In addition, slower ME following both AO + MI and a challenging cognitive task provides support for the motor-cognitive model of MI.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"575-593"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11874476/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140120487","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1