首页 > 最新文献

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
An another perspective of face masks in emotion recognition: Comment on Shepherd and Rippon (2022). 从另一个角度看情绪识别中的人脸面具:对 Shepherd 和 Rippon(2022 年)的评论。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-30 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231195325
Elias Rodrigues de Almeida-Junior, Joaquim Pedro Brito-de-Sousa, Elenice Francisco da Silva, Rosineide Marques Ribas

In a recent article, Shepherd and Rippon (2022) investigated the impact of widespread face mask use on emotion recognition. They found that mask-wearing led to reduced accuracy in identifying emotions such as fear, sadness, and disgust when participants were exposed to brief facial stimuli. Their study highlights the significance of masks in concealing facial areas crucial for non-verbal communication, potentially affecting emotional well-being. Here in this comment, we have argued that despite concerns about impaired emotional recognition and social interactions, balancing COVID-19 protection and effective communication is essential. We stress the importance of adhering to mask guidelines while enhancing alternative cues and communication strategies. In public health emergencies like COVID-19, such research should acknowledge the whole complexity and prioritize safety aspects in a manner that prevents controversial issues.

在最近的一篇文章中,Shepherd 和 Rippon(2022 年)研究了广泛使用面具对情绪识别的影响。他们发现,当参与者受到短暂的面部刺激时,戴面具会降低识别恐惧、悲伤和厌恶等情绪的准确性。他们的研究强调了面具在隐藏面部非语言交流关键部位方面的重要性,这可能会影响情绪的健康。在这篇评论中,我们认为,尽管人们担心情绪识别和社会交往会受损,但在 COVID-19 保护和有效沟通之间取得平衡至关重要。我们强调,在加强替代提示和交流策略的同时,必须遵守口罩指南。在 COVID-19 等公共卫生突发事件中,此类研究应认识到整体的复杂性,并以防止出现争议问题的方式优先考虑安全方面。
{"title":"An another perspective of face masks in emotion recognition: Comment on Shepherd and Rippon (2022).","authors":"Elias Rodrigues de Almeida-Junior, Joaquim Pedro Brito-de-Sousa, Elenice Francisco da Silva, Rosineide Marques Ribas","doi":"10.1177/17470218231195325","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231195325","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a recent article, Shepherd and Rippon (2022) investigated the impact of widespread face mask use on emotion recognition. They found that mask-wearing led to reduced accuracy in identifying emotions such as fear, sadness, and disgust when participants were exposed to brief facial stimuli. Their study highlights the significance of masks in concealing facial areas crucial for non-verbal communication, potentially affecting emotional well-being. Here in this comment, we have argued that despite concerns about impaired emotional recognition and social interactions, balancing COVID-19 protection and effective communication is essential. We stress the importance of adhering to mask guidelines while enhancing alternative cues and communication strategies. In public health emergencies like COVID-19, such research should acknowledge the whole complexity and prioritize safety aspects in a manner that prevents controversial issues.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1137-1139"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10168176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Agency effects on the binding of event elements in episodic memory. 代理效应对情景记忆中事件元素结合的影响。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-31 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231203951
Marcel R Schreiner, Arndt Bröder, Thorsten Meiser

Representing events in episodic memory in a coherent manner requires that their constituent elements are bound together. So far, only few moderators of these binding processes have been identified. Here we investigate whether the presence of an agentic element in an event facilitates binding. The results from six experiments provided no evidence for a facilitating effect of agency on the binding of event elements. In addition, binding effects were only found when event elements were presented simultaneously, but not when they were presented sequentially pairwise, contrary to previous findings. The results suggest that the presence of an agentic element in an event does not, or only to a very limited extent, contribute to the formation of coherent memory representations and that additional processes may be required when binding event elements across temporarily divided encoding episodes. These findings add to a growing body of research regarding moderators and processes relevant for the binding of event elements in episodic memory. Explanations of these findings and directions for future research are discussed.

以连贯的方式表示情景记忆中的事件需要将它们的组成元素结合在一起。到目前为止,只有少数调节这些结合过程的因素被确定。在这里,我们研究事件中代理元素的存在是否有助于绑定。六个实验的结果没有提供任何证据表明代理对事件元素结合的促进作用。此外,与先前的发现相反,只有当事件元素同时呈现时,才会发现结合效应,而当它们按顺序成对呈现时,则不会发现结合效应。结果表明,事件中代理元素的存在不会或仅在非常有限的程度上有助于连贯记忆表示的形成,并且当跨临时划分的编码集绑定事件元素时,可能需要额外的过程。这些发现增加了越来越多关于情景记忆中事件元素结合的调节因子和过程的研究。对这些发现的解释和未来研究的方向进行了讨论。
{"title":"Agency effects on the binding of event elements in episodic memory.","authors":"Marcel R Schreiner, Arndt Bröder, Thorsten Meiser","doi":"10.1177/17470218231203951","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231203951","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Representing events in episodic memory in a coherent manner requires that their constituent elements are bound together. So far, only few moderators of these binding processes have been identified. Here we investigate whether the presence of an agentic element in an event facilitates binding. The results from six experiments provided no evidence for a facilitating effect of agency on the binding of event elements. In addition, binding effects were only found when event elements were presented simultaneously, but not when they were presented sequentially pairwise, contrary to previous findings. The results suggest that the presence of an agentic element in an event does not, or only to a very limited extent, contribute to the formation of coherent memory representations and that additional processes may be required when binding event elements across temporarily divided encoding episodes. These findings add to a growing body of research regarding moderators and processes relevant for the binding of event elements in episodic memory. Explanations of these findings and directions for future research are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1201-1220"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11134989/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41164365","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Queues, crowds, and angry mobs: Face identification under distraction in a virtual airport. 排队、人群和愤怒的暴民:在虚拟机场分心下的人脸识别。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-08 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231203939
Matthew C Fysh, Edward Baker, Jodie Rockett, John Allen, Cade McCall, A Mike Burton, Markus Bindemann

In visual environments, selective attention must be employed to focus on task-relevant stimuli. A key question here concerns the extent to which other stimuli within the visual field influence target processing. In this study, we ask whether face identity matching is subject to similar effects from irrelevant stimuli in the visual field, specifically task-irrelevant people. Although most previous studies rely on highly controlled face and body stimuli presented in isolation, here we use a more realistic environment. Participants take the role of passport officers and must match a person's face to their photo-ID, while other people appear in the background, waiting to be processed. Presenting an interactive virtual environment on screen (Experiments 1 and 2) or in immersive VR (Experiment 3), we generally found no evidence for distraction from background people on face-matching accuracy. However, when immersed in VR, an angry crowd in the background delayed matching speed while not affecting accuracy. We discuss the theoretical implications of these results and their potential importance in practical settings.

在视觉环境中,必须使用选择性注意力来关注与任务相关的刺激。这里的一个关键问题涉及视野内的其他刺激对目标处理的影响程度。在这项研究中,我们询问人脸身份匹配是否会受到视野中不相关刺激的类似影响,特别是任务无关的人。尽管之前的大多数研究都依赖于高度控制的面部和身体刺激,但在这里我们使用了一个更现实的环境。参与者扮演护照官员的角色,必须将一个人的脸与带照片的身份证相匹配,而其他人则出现在后台,等待处理。在屏幕上呈现交互式虚拟环境(实验1和2)或在沉浸式VR中(实验3),我们通常没有发现任何证据表明背景人物在人脸匹配准确性上会分心。然而,当沉浸在VR中时,背景中愤怒的人群在不影响准确性的情况下延迟了匹配速度。我们讨论了这些结果的理论含义及其在实际环境中的潜在重要性。
{"title":"Queues, crowds, and angry mobs: Face identification under distraction in a virtual airport.","authors":"Matthew C Fysh, Edward Baker, Jodie Rockett, John Allen, Cade McCall, A Mike Burton, Markus Bindemann","doi":"10.1177/17470218231203939","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231203939","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In visual environments, selective attention must be employed to focus on task-relevant stimuli. A key question here concerns the extent to which other stimuli within the visual field influence target processing. In this study, we ask whether face identity matching is subject to similar effects from irrelevant stimuli in the visual field, specifically task-irrelevant people. Although most previous studies rely on highly controlled face and body stimuli presented in isolation, here we use a more realistic environment. Participants take the role of passport officers and must match a person's face to their photo-ID, while other people appear in the background, waiting to be processed. Presenting an interactive virtual environment on screen (Experiments 1 and 2) or in immersive VR (Experiment 3), we generally found no evidence for distraction from background people on face-matching accuracy. However, when immersed in VR, an angry crowd in the background delayed matching speed while not affecting accuracy. We discuss the theoretical implications of these results and their potential importance in practical settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1169-1178"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11103907/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10269096","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
SUBTLEX-CY: A new word frequency database for Welsh. SUBTLEX-CY:新的威尔士语词频数据库。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-30 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231190315
Walter Jb van Heuven, Joshua S Payne, Manon W Jones

We present SUBTLEX-CY, a new word frequency database created from a 32-million-word corpus of Welsh television subtitles. An experiment comprising a lexical decision task examined SUBTLEX-CY frequency estimates against words with inconsistent frequencies in a much smaller Welsh corpus that is often used by researchers, the Cronfa Electroneg o'r Gymraeg (CEG), and three other Welsh word frequency databases. Words were selected that were classified as low frequency (LF) in SUBTLEX-CY and high frequency (HF) in CEG and compared with words that were classified as medium frequency (MF) in both SUBTLEX-CY and CEG. Reaction time analyses showed that HF words in CEG were responded to more slowly compared to MF words, suggesting that SUBTLEX-CY corpus provides a more reliable estimate of Welsh word frequencies. The new Welsh word frequency database that also includes part-of-speech, contextual diversity, and other lexical information is freely available for research purposes on the Open Science Framework repository at https://osf.io/9gkqm/.

我们介绍的 SUBTLEX-CY 是一个新的词频数据库,由 3200 万字的威尔士语电视字幕语料库创建而成。一项由词性判断任务组成的实验对照研究人员经常使用的规模小得多的威尔士语语料库 Cronfa Electroneg o'r Gymraeg (CEG) 和其他三个威尔士语词频数据库中词频不一致的词来检验 SUBTLEX-CY 的词频估计值。我们选择了在 SUBTLEX-CY 中被归类为低频 (LF) 和在 CEG 中被归类为高频 (HF) 的单词,并将其与在 SUBTLEX-CY 和 CEG 中被归类为中频 (MF) 的单词进行比较。反应时间分析表明,与中频词相比,CEG 中的高频词反应速度更慢,这表明 SUBTLEX-CY 语料库对威尔士语词频的估计更为可靠。新的威尔士语词频数据库还包括语音部分、上下文多样性和其他词汇信息,可在 https://osf.io/9gkqm/ 的开放科学框架资源库中免费获取,用于研究目的。
{"title":"SUBTLEX-CY: A new word frequency database for Welsh.","authors":"Walter Jb van Heuven, Joshua S Payne, Manon W Jones","doi":"10.1177/17470218231190315","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231190315","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We present SUBTLEX-CY, a new word frequency database created from a 32-million-word corpus of Welsh television subtitles. An experiment comprising a lexical decision task examined SUBTLEX-CY frequency estimates against words with inconsistent frequencies in a much smaller Welsh corpus that is often used by researchers, the <i>Cronfa Electroneg o'r Gymraeg</i> (CEG), and three other Welsh word frequency databases. Words were selected that were classified as low frequency (LF) in SUBTLEX-CY and high frequency (HF) in CEG and compared with words that were classified as medium frequency (MF) in both SUBTLEX-CY and CEG. Reaction time analyses showed that HF words in CEG were responded to more slowly compared to MF words, suggesting that SUBTLEX-CY corpus provides a more reliable estimate of Welsh word frequencies. The new Welsh word frequency database that also includes part-of-speech, contextual diversity, and other lexical information is freely available for research purposes on the Open Science Framework repository at https://osf.io/9gkqm/.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1052-1067"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11032624/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10113027","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Database of Expressionist, Impressionist, and Post-Impressionist paintings: Affective norms for 60 art pieces. 表现主义、印象派和后印象派绘画数据库:60件艺术作品的情感规范。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-04 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231200955
Kamil K Imbir, Adrianna Wielgopolan, Julia Stępniewska, Katarzyna Benda

Modern painters' art is not only different from canvas created earlier, but also shows high internal variability. Being aware of the conditions arising from art history, we used paintings from three art movements-Impressionism, Post-Impressionism, and Expressionism, to see if we are able to respond to claims made by art theorists by using methods specific to social sciences, and validate the paintings as stimuli which might evoke different emotional reaction based on the movement they were created in. We wanted to conduct an exploratory analysis comparing the mean assessment of valence, arousal, and dominance among the three art movements. A total of 60 different paintings were selected (20 for each art movement) showing figurative works and visible human figures. They were assessed on five different affective scales: valence, arousal, dominance, origin, and subjective significance with use of Self-Assessment Manikins. The results showed expected differences in affective reactions to Expressionist paintings compared with those of Impressionist and Post-Impressionist ones. Therefore, we provided the very first dataset of emotional stimuli with validated affective norms, categorised by the art movement that they were created in and ready to be used in future experimental studies.

现代画家的艺术不仅不同于早期创作的画布,而且表现出高度的内在变异性。意识到艺术史产生的条件,我们使用了印象派、后印象派和表现主义三个艺术运动的画作,看看我们是否能够通过使用社会科学特有的方法来回应艺术理论家的主张,并验证这些画作是刺激物,可能会根据它们所处的运动引发不同的情绪反应。我们想进行一项探索性分析,比较三种艺术运动对效价、唤醒和支配力的平均评估。共选择了60幅不同的绘画作品(每一艺术运动20幅),展示了具象作品和可见的人物形象。使用自评量表对他们进行了五种不同的情感量表评估:效价、唤醒、支配、起源和主观意义。研究结果显示,与印象派和后印象派相比,表现主义绘画在情感反应方面存在预期差异。因此,我们提供了第一个具有经验证的情感规范的情感刺激数据集,按其创建的艺术运动进行分类,并准备用于未来的实验研究。
{"title":"Database of Expressionist, Impressionist, and Post-Impressionist paintings: Affective norms for 60 art pieces.","authors":"Kamil K Imbir, Adrianna Wielgopolan, Julia Stępniewska, Katarzyna Benda","doi":"10.1177/17470218231200955","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231200955","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Modern painters' art is not only different from canvas created earlier, but also shows high internal variability. Being aware of the conditions arising from art history, we used paintings from three art movements-Impressionism, Post-Impressionism, and Expressionism, to see if we are able to respond to claims made by art theorists by using methods specific to social sciences, and validate the paintings as stimuli which might evoke different emotional reaction based on the movement they were created in. We wanted to conduct an exploratory analysis comparing the mean assessment of valence, arousal, and dominance among the three art movements. A total of 60 different paintings were selected (20 for each art movement) showing figurative works and visible human figures. They were assessed on five different affective scales: valence, arousal, dominance, origin, and subjective significance with use of Self-Assessment Manikins. The results showed expected differences in affective reactions to Expressionist paintings compared with those of Impressionist and Post-Impressionist ones. Therefore, we provided the very first dataset of emotional stimuli with validated affective norms, categorised by the art movement that they were created in and ready to be used in future experimental studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1093-1105"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10194175","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How well do we do social distancing? 我们的社会疏离做得如何?
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-24 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231195247
Naohide Yamamoto, Mia Nightingale

During the pandemic of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), many jurisdictions around the world introduced a "social distance" rule under which people are instructed to keep a certain distance from others. Generally, this rule is implemented simply by telling people how many metres or feet of separation should be kept, without giving them precise instructions as to how the specified distance can be measured. Consequently, the rule is effective only to the extent that people are able to gauge this distance through their space perception. To examine the effectiveness of the rule from this point of view, this study empirically investigated how much distance people would leave from another person when they relied on their perception of this distance. Participants (N = 153) were asked to stand exactly 1.5 m away from a researcher, and resultant interpersonal distances showed that while their mean was close to the correct 1.5 m distance, they exhibited large individual differences. These results suggest that a number of people would not stay sufficiently away from others even when they intend to do proper social distancing. Given this outcome, it is suggested that official health advice include measures that compensate for this tendency.

在 2019 年冠状病毒病(COVID-19)大流行期间,全球许多司法管辖区都引入了 "社会距离 "规则,指示人们与他人保持一定距离。一般来说,这一规则的实施方式只是告诉人们应与他人保持多少米或多少英尺的距离,而没有给出如何测量指定距离的精确指示。因此,只有当人们能够通过空间感知来衡量这一距离时,这一规则才会有效。为了从这一角度考察该规则的有效性,本研究通过实证调查了解了当人们依靠自己的感知来判断距离时,会与他人保持多少距离。参与者(153 人)被要求站在距离研究人员 1.5 米远的地方,由此得出的人际距离显示,虽然他们的平均值接近正确的 1.5 米距离,但却表现出很大的个体差异。这些结果表明,许多人即使有意保持适当的社交距离,也不会与他人保持足够的距离。鉴于这一结果,建议在官方健康建议中纳入弥补这一倾向的措施。
{"title":"How well do we do social distancing?","authors":"Naohide Yamamoto, Mia Nightingale","doi":"10.1177/17470218231195247","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231195247","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>During the pandemic of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), many jurisdictions around the world introduced a \"social distance\" rule under which people are instructed to keep a certain distance from others. Generally, this rule is implemented simply by telling people how many metres or feet of separation should be kept, without giving them precise instructions as to how the specified distance can be measured. Consequently, the rule is effective only to the extent that people are able to gauge this distance through their space perception. To examine the effectiveness of the rule from this point of view, this study empirically investigated how much distance people would leave from another person when they relied on their perception of this distance. Participants (<i>N</i> = 153) were asked to stand exactly 1.5 m away from a researcher, and resultant interpersonal distances showed that while their mean was close to the correct 1.5 m distance, they exhibited large individual differences. These results suggest that a number of people would not stay sufficiently away from others even when they intend to do proper social distancing. Given this outcome, it is suggested that official health advice include measures that compensate for this tendency.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1106-1112"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11032622/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10457084","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Non-arbitrary mappings between size and sound of English words: Form typicality effects during lexical access and memory. 英语单词大小与读音之间的非任意映射:词汇存取和记忆过程中的形式典型性效应。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-06 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231184940
Greig I de Zubicaray, Joanne Arciuli, Frank H Guenther, Katie L McMahon, Elaine Kearney

A century of research has provided evidence of limited size sound symbolism in English, that is, certain vowels are non-arbitrarily associated with words denoting small versus large referents (e.g., /i/ as in teensy and /ɑ/ as in tall). In the present study, we investigated more extensive statistical regularities between surface form properties of English words and ratings of their semantic size, that is, form typicality, and its impact on language and memory processing. Our findings provide the first evidence of significant word form typicality for semantic size. In five empirical studies using behavioural megastudy data sets of performance on written and auditory lexical decision, reading aloud, semantic decision, and recognition memory tasks, we show that form typicality for size is a stronger and more consistent predictor of lexical access during word comprehension and production than semantic size, in addition to playing a significant role in verbal memory. The empirical results demonstrate that statistical information about non-arbitrary form-size mappings is accessed automatically during language and verbal memory processing, unlike semantic size that is largely dependent on task contexts that explicitly require participants to access size knowledge. We discuss how a priori knowledge about non-arbitrary form-meaning associations in the lexicon might be incorporated in models of language processing that implement Bayesian statistical inference.

一个世纪以来的研究已经提供了英语中有限大小音符号的证据,也就是说,某些元音与表示小或大的指代物(例如 teensy 中的 /i/ 和 tall 中的 /ɑ/)非任意地联系在一起。在本研究中,我们对英语单词的表面形式属性与语义大小评分(即形式典型性)之间更广泛的统计规律性及其对语言和记忆加工的影响进行了调查。我们的研究结果首次证明了语义大小具有显著的词形典型性。在五项实证研究中,我们利用行为大研究数据集对书面和听觉词汇判断、朗读、语义判断和识别记忆任务的表现进行了研究,结果表明,与语义大小相比,形式典型性对词汇理解和生成过程中的词汇访问具有更强、更一致的预测作用,此外,它还在言语记忆中发挥着重要作用。实证结果表明,在语言和言语记忆处理过程中,可以自动获取有关非任意形式-大小映射的统计信息,而语义大小则不同,它在很大程度上取决于明确要求参与者获取大小知识的任务情境。我们讨论了如何将词典中有关非任意形式-意义关联的先验知识纳入实现贝叶斯统计推理的语言处理模型中。
{"title":"Non-arbitrary mappings between size and sound of English words: Form typicality effects during lexical access and memory.","authors":"Greig I de Zubicaray, Joanne Arciuli, Frank H Guenther, Katie L McMahon, Elaine Kearney","doi":"10.1177/17470218231184940","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231184940","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A century of research has provided evidence of limited size sound symbolism in English, that is, certain vowels are non-arbitrarily associated with words denoting small versus large referents (e.g., /i/ as in <i>teensy</i> and /ɑ/ as in <i>tall</i>). In the present study, we investigated more extensive statistical regularities between surface form properties of English words and ratings of their semantic size, that is, <i>form typicality</i>, and its impact on language and memory processing. Our findings provide the first evidence of significant word form typicality for semantic size. In five empirical studies using behavioural megastudy data sets of performance on written and auditory lexical decision, reading aloud, semantic decision, and recognition memory tasks, we show that form typicality for size is a stronger and more consistent predictor of lexical access during word comprehension and production than semantic size, in addition to playing a significant role in verbal memory. The empirical results demonstrate that statistical information about non-arbitrary form-size mappings is accessed automatically during language and verbal memory processing, unlike semantic size that is largely dependent on task contexts that explicitly require participants to access size knowledge. We discuss how a priori knowledge about non-arbitrary form-meaning associations in the lexicon might be incorporated in models of language processing that implement Bayesian statistical inference.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"943-963"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11032636/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9751046","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Representational level matters for tone-word recognition: Evidence from form priming. 语气词识别的表征水平:来自形式启动的证据。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-24 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231203615
Jinxing Yue, Roelien Bastiaanse, David Howard, Kai Alter

In a form priming experiment with a lexical decision task, we investigated whether the representational structure of lexical tone in lexical memory impacts spoken-word recognition in Mandarin. Target monosyllabic words were preceded by five types of primes: (1) the same real words (/lun4/-/lun4/), (2) real words with only tone contrasts (/lun2/-/lun4/), (3) unrelated real words (/pie3/-/lun4/), (4) pseudowords with only tone contrasts (*/lun3/-/lun4/), and (5) unrelated pseudowords (*/tai3/-/lun4/). We found a facilitation effect in target words with pseudoword primes that share the segmental syllable but contrast in tones (*/lun3/-/lun4/). Moreover, no evident form priming effect was observed in target words primed by real words with only tone contrasts (/lun2/-/lun4/). These results suggest that the recognition of a tone word is influenced by the representational level of tone accessed by the prime word. The distinctive priming patterns between real-word and pseudoword primes are best explained by the connectionist models of tone-word recognition, which assume a hierarchical representation of lexical tone.

在一项词汇决策任务的形式启动实验中,我们研究了词汇记忆中词汇语气的表征结构是否影响普通话口语识别。目标单音节词前面有五种类型的素数:(1)相同的实词(/lun4/-/lun4/),(2)只有音调对比的实词,(3)不相关的实词。我们发现,在具有伪词素数的目标词中,有一种促进效应,这些词共用分段音节,但在音调上有对比(*/lun3/-/lun4/)。此外,在只有音调对比的真实单词(/lun2/-/lun4/)引发的目标单词中,没有观察到明显的形式引发效应。这些结果表明,语气词的识别受到主词所访问的语气的表征水平的影响。语气词识别的连接主义模型最好地解释了真实词和伪词之间独特的启动模式,该模型假设了词汇语气的层次表示。
{"title":"Representational level matters for tone-word recognition: Evidence from form priming.","authors":"Jinxing Yue, Roelien Bastiaanse, David Howard, Kai Alter","doi":"10.1177/17470218231203615","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231203615","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In a form priming experiment with a lexical decision task, we investigated whether the representational structure of lexical tone in lexical memory impacts spoken-word recognition in Mandarin. Target monosyllabic words were preceded by five types of primes: (1) the same real words (/lun4/-/lun4/), (2) real words with only tone contrasts (/lun2/-/lun4/), (3) unrelated real words (/pie3/-/lun4/), (4) pseudowords with only tone contrasts (*/lun3/-/lun4/), and (5) unrelated pseudowords (*/tai3/-/lun4/). We found a facilitation effect in target words with pseudoword primes that share the segmental syllable but contrast in tones (*/lun3/-/lun4/). Moreover, no evident form priming effect was observed in target words primed by real words with only tone contrasts (/lun2/-/lun4/). These results suggest that the recognition of a tone word is influenced by the representational level of tone accessed by the prime word. The distinctive priming patterns between real-word and pseudoword primes are best explained by the connectionist models of tone-word recognition, which assume a hierarchical representation of lexical tone.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1125-1135"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10297175","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Are lexical representations graded or discrete? 词汇表征是分级的还是离散的?
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-31 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231187027
Leon Li, Andrés Buxó-Lugo, Cassandra L Jacobs, L Robert Slevc

Most research on mental lexical representations (lemmas) assumes they are discrete and correspond in number to a word's number of distinct meanings. Thus, homophones (bat), whose meanings are unrelated, have separate lemmas for each meaning (one for baseball bat, another for flying bat), whereas polysemes (paper), whose senses are related, have shared lemmas (the same lemma for printer paper and term paper). However, most aspects of cognition are thought to be graded, not discrete; could lemmas be graded too? We conducted a preregistered picture-word interference study with pictures of words whose meanings ranged from unrelated (homophones) to very related (regular polysemes). Whereas semantic competitors to picture names slow picture naming, semantic competitors to non-depicted meanings of homophones facilitate naming, suggesting distinct lemmas for homophones' meanings. We predicted that competitors to non-depicted senses of polysemes would slow naming, as polysemes' depicted and non-depicted senses presumably share a lemma. Crucially, we aimed to examine the transition from facilitation to inhibition: two groupings (where competitors to non-depicted senses led to facilitation for words with two lemmas but inhibition for words with one lemma) would imply that lemmas are indeed discrete. But a transition that varies continuously by sense relatedness would imply that lemmas are graded. Unexpectedly, competitors to non-depicted senses of both homophones and polysemes facilitated naming. Although these results do not indicate whether lemmas are graded or discrete, they do inform a long-standing question on the nature of polysemes, supporting a multiple-lemma (vs. core-lemma) account.

关于心理词汇表征(词素)的大多数研究都假定它们是离散的,其数量与一个词的不同意义数量相对应。因此,词义互不相关的同音词(蝙蝠)在每个词义上都有单独的词表(棒球棒有一个词表,飞行蝙蝠有另一个词表),而词义相关的多义词(纸张)则有共同的词表(打印机纸张和术语纸张有相同的词表)。然而,人们认为认知的大多数方面都是分级的,而不是离散的;那么词素是否也可以分级呢?我们进行了一项预先登记的图片-词语干扰研究,研究对象是词义从不相干(同音词)到非常相关(规则多义词)的词语图片。图片名称的语义竞争者会减慢图片命名的速度,而同音词的非描述性含义的语义竞争者则会促进命名,这表明同音词的含义有不同的词素。我们预测,多义词的非描绘义竞争者会减慢命名速度,因为多义词的描绘义和非描绘义可能共享一个词素。最重要的是,我们的目标是研究从促进到抑制的过渡:两种分组(对非描绘义的竞争者会导致有两个词素的词的促进,而对有一个词素的词的抑制)意味着词素确实是离散的。但是,根据词义相关性不断变化的过渡则意味着词性是分级的。意想不到的是,同音词和多义词的非描绘义竞争者都有助于命名。尽管这些结果并不能说明词素是分级的还是离散的,但它们确实为一个关于多词素性质的长期问题提供了信息,支持了多词素(相对于核心词素)的观点。
{"title":"Are lexical representations graded or discrete?","authors":"Leon Li, Andrés Buxó-Lugo, Cassandra L Jacobs, L Robert Slevc","doi":"10.1177/17470218231187027","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231187027","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most research on mental lexical representations (lemmas) assumes they are discrete and correspond in number to a word's number of distinct meanings. Thus, homophones (<i>bat</i>), whose meanings are unrelated, have separate lemmas for each meaning (one for <i>baseball bat</i>, another for <i>flying bat</i>), whereas polysemes (<i>paper</i>), whose senses are related, have shared lemmas (the same lemma for <i>printer paper</i> and <i>term paper</i>). However, most aspects of cognition are thought to be graded, not discrete; could lemmas be graded too? We conducted a preregistered picture-word interference study with pictures of words whose meanings ranged from unrelated (homophones) to very related (regular polysemes). Whereas semantic competitors to picture names slow picture naming, semantic competitors to non-depicted meanings of homophones <i>facilitate</i> naming, suggesting distinct lemmas for homophones' meanings. We predicted that competitors to non-depicted senses of polysemes would slow naming, as polysemes' depicted and non-depicted senses presumably share a lemma. Crucially, we aimed to examine the transition from facilitation to inhibition: two groupings (where competitors to non-depicted senses led to facilitation for words with two lemmas but inhibition for words with one lemma) would imply that lemmas are indeed discrete. But a transition that varies continuously by sense relatedness would imply that lemmas are graded. Unexpectedly, competitors to non-depicted senses of both homophones and polysemes facilitated naming. Although these results do not indicate whether lemmas are graded or discrete, they do inform a long-standing question on the nature of polysemes, supporting a multiple-lemma (vs. core-lemma) account.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"909-923"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9894624","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of irrelevant emotionally negative stimuli on early and late retrospective metacognitive judgements. 无关情绪性负面刺激对早期和晚期回溯性元认知判断的影响。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-10 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231191516
Marie Geurten, Patrick Lemaire

It is well established that negative emotions influence a range of cognitive processes. How these emotions influence the metacognitive judgement individuals make about their own performance and whether this influence is similar depending on the conditions under which metacognition is assessed, however, is far less understood. The primary aim of this study was to determine whether exposure to emotional stimuli could influence metacognitive judgements made under short or long time constraints. A total sample of 144 young adults (aged 18-35 years) was recruited and asked to complete an arithmetic strategy selection task under emotional or neutral condition. Following each strategy selection trial, participants also provided a retrospective confidence judgement (RCJ). Both strategy selection and RCJ were collected under short or long time constraints (1,500 vs. 2,500 ms for strategy selection and 800 vs. 1,500 ms for RCJ). In addition to replicating previous findings showing lower rates of better strategy selection under negative emotions compared with neutral condition, an effect of negative stimuli on the accuracy of participants' confidence judgements was found, but only if participants had a short time limit to make their second-level evaluation. Such findings are consistent with the hypothesis that exposure to emotional stimuli disturbs early, but not late metacognitive processes and have important implications to further our understanding of the role of emotions on metacognition.

负面情绪会影响一系列认知过程,这一点已得到公认。然而,人们对这些情绪如何影响个体对自身表现所做的元认知判断,以及这种影响是否因元认知评估条件的不同而相似,却知之甚少。本研究的主要目的是确定暴露于情绪刺激是否会影响在短时间或长时间限制下做出的元认知判断。研究共招募了 144 名年轻成年人(18-35 岁),要求他们在情绪或中性条件下完成算术策略选择任务。在每次策略选择试验后,参与者还提供了一个回顾性信心判断(RCJ)。策略选择和 RCJ 都是在短或长的时间限制下收集的(策略选择为 1,500 vs. 2,500 毫秒,RCJ 为 800 vs. 1,500 毫秒)。除了重复了之前的研究结果,即与中性条件相比,负面情绪下的策略选择率更低之外,还发现了负面刺激对参与者信心判断准确性的影响,但仅限于参与者在短时间内做出第二级评估的情况。这些发现与受到情绪刺激会干扰早期元认知过程而非晚期元认知过程的假设一致,对我们进一步了解情绪对元认知的作用具有重要意义。
{"title":"The influence of irrelevant emotionally negative stimuli on early and late retrospective metacognitive judgements.","authors":"Marie Geurten, Patrick Lemaire","doi":"10.1177/17470218231191516","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231191516","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is well established that negative emotions influence a range of cognitive processes. How these emotions influence the metacognitive judgement individuals make about their own performance and whether this influence is similar depending on the conditions under which metacognition is assessed, however, is far less understood. The primary aim of this study was to determine whether exposure to emotional stimuli could influence metacognitive judgements made under short or long time constraints. A total sample of 144 young adults (aged 18-35 years) was recruited and asked to complete an arithmetic strategy selection task under emotional or neutral condition. Following each strategy selection trial, participants also provided a retrospective confidence judgement (RCJ). Both strategy selection and RCJ were collected under short or long time constraints (1,500 vs. 2,500 ms for strategy selection and 800 vs. 1,500 ms for RCJ). In addition to replicating previous findings showing lower rates of better strategy selection under negative emotions compared with neutral condition, an effect of negative stimuli on the accuracy of participants' confidence judgements was found, but only if participants had a short time limit to make their second-level evaluation. Such findings are consistent with the hypothesis that exposure to emotional stimuli disturbs early, but not late metacognitive processes and have important implications to further our understanding of the role of emotions on metacognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1113-1124"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9974071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1