首页 > 最新文献

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The influence of peripheral information on a proactive process during multitasking. 多任务处理过程中外围信息对主动过程的影响。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-30 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231195198
Tsukasa Kimura, Tomoya Kawashima

The aim of this study was to examine whether peripheral information facilitates proactive processes during multitasking. For this purpose, peripheral information was presented regularly during multitasking and its effects on the performance of a tracking task (main task: reactive process) and a discrimination task (sub-task: proactive process) were examined. Experiment 1 presented peripheral information (white circles) in the same sensory modality (visual) as the information used for multitasking and the number of circle presentations was manipulated. In Experiment 2, a pure tone (auditory) was presented as peripheral information. We found that, in both experiments, the difficulty of the tracking task influenced discrimination performance, showing that as the difficulty of the tracking task (reactive process) increased, more cognitive resources were consumed in the tracking task, resulting in a decrease in cognitive resources available for the discrimination task (proactive process). In addition, regular presentation of peripheral information facilitated discrimination task performance in both experiments. Interestingly, this peripheral information also facilitated the tracking task performance (reactive process) even if the tracking task was difficult. Moreover, this promoting effect of the peripheral information occurred regardless of the sensory modality. This study revealed that processing of peripheral information facilitates the proactive process even if more cognitive resources are consumed, and that this facilitating effect does not conflict with multitasking and provides a margin of cognitive resources and also facilitates the reactive process. Our results provide evidence of how peripheral information and cognitive resources are used during multitasking.

本研究旨在探讨外围信息是否有助于多任务处理过程中的主动过程。为此,在多任务处理过程中定期呈现外围信息,并考察其对追踪任务(主任务:反应过程)和辨别任务(子任务:主动过程)的影响。实验 1 采用与多任务所用信息相同的感官模式(视觉)呈现外围信息(白色圆圈),并对圆圈呈现的次数进行了控制。在实验 2 中,纯音(听觉)作为外围信息呈现。我们发现,在这两项实验中,追踪任务的难度都会影响分辨成绩,这表明随着追踪任务(反应过程)难度的增加,追踪任务会消耗更多的认知资源,从而导致可用于分辨任务(主动过程)的认知资源减少。此外,在这两项实验中,定期呈现外围信息有助于提高辨别任务的成绩。有趣的是,即使追踪任务很困难,外围信息也能促进追踪任务的完成(反应过程)。此外,外围信息的这种促进作用与感觉模式无关。这项研究表明,即使消耗了更多的认知资源,外围信息的处理也会促进主动过程,而且这种促进作用与多任务处理并不冲突,它提供了认知资源的余量,同时也促进了反应过程。我们的研究结果为多任务处理过程中如何利用外围信息和认知资源提供了证据。
{"title":"The influence of peripheral information on a proactive process during multitasking.","authors":"Tsukasa Kimura, Tomoya Kawashima","doi":"10.1177/17470218231195198","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231195198","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The aim of this study was to examine whether peripheral information facilitates proactive processes during multitasking. For this purpose, peripheral information was presented regularly during multitasking and its effects on the performance of a tracking task (main task: reactive process) and a discrimination task (sub-task: proactive process) were examined. Experiment 1 presented peripheral information (white circles) in the same sensory modality (visual) as the information used for multitasking and the number of circle presentations was manipulated. In Experiment 2, a pure tone (auditory) was presented as peripheral information. We found that, in both experiments, the difficulty of the tracking task influenced discrimination performance, showing that as the difficulty of the tracking task (reactive process) increased, more cognitive resources were consumed in the tracking task, resulting in a decrease in cognitive resources available for the discrimination task (proactive process). In addition, regular presentation of peripheral information facilitated discrimination task performance in both experiments. Interestingly, this peripheral information also facilitated the tracking task performance (reactive process) even if the tracking task was difficult. Moreover, this promoting effect of the peripheral information occurred regardless of the sensory modality. This study revealed that processing of peripheral information facilitates the proactive process even if more cognitive resources are consumed, and that this facilitating effect does not conflict with multitasking and provides a margin of cognitive resources and also facilitates the reactive process. Our results provide evidence of how peripheral information and cognitive resources are used during multitasking.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1352-1362"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10472870","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Post-migration living difficulties and poor mental health associated with increased interpretation bias for threat. 移民后生活困难和心理健康状况不佳与威胁解释偏差增加有关。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-08-11 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231191442
Anastasia Vikhanova, Marc S Tibber, Isabelle Mareschal

Previous research has found associations between mental health difficulties and interpretation biases, including heightened interpretation of threat from neutral or ambiguous stimuli. Building on this research, we explored associations between interpretation biases (positive and negative) and three constructs that have been linked to migrant experience: mental health symptoms (Global Severity Index [GSI]), Post-Migration Living Difficulties (PMLD), and Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire (PEDQ). Two hundred thirty students who identified as first- (n = 94) or second-generation ethnic minority migrants (n = 68), and first-generation White migrants (n = 68) completed measures of GSI, PEDQ, and PMLD. They also performed an interpretation bias task using Point Light Walkers (PLW), dynamic stimuli with reduced visual input that are easily perceived as humans performing an action. Five categories of PLW were used: four that clearly depicted human forms undertaking positive, neutral, negative, or ambiguous actions, and a fifth that involved scrambled animations with no clear action or form. Participants were asked to imagine their interaction with the stimuli and rate their friendliness (positive interpretation bias) and aggressiveness (interpretation bias for threat). We found that the three groups differed on PEDQ and PMLD, with no significant differences in GSI, and the three measured were positively correlated. Poorer mental health and increased PMLD were associated with a heightened interpretation for threat of scrambled animations only. These findings have implications for understanding of the role of threat biases in mental health and the migrant experience.

以往的研究发现,心理健康困难与解释偏差之间存在关联,包括对中性或模棱两可的刺激产生的威胁的解释增强。在此研究的基础上,我们探讨了解释偏差(积极和消极)与三个与移民经历相关的因素之间的联系:心理健康症状(全球严重程度指数[GSI])、移民后生活困难(PMLD)和感知民族歧视问卷(PEDQ)。230 名被认定为第一代(94 人)或第二代少数民族移民(68 人)以及第一代白人移民(68 人)的学生完成了 GSI、PEDQ 和 PMLD 的测量。他们还使用点光步行器(PLW)完成了一项解释偏差任务,点光步行器是一种视觉输入减少的动态刺激物,很容易被认为是正在做动作的人。他们使用了五类点光步行器:四类清晰地描绘了人类正在做出积极、中性、消极或模棱两可的动作,第五类则是没有明确动作或形态的乱码动画。参与者被要求想象他们与刺激物的互动,并对刺激物的友好性(积极解释偏差)和攻击性(威胁解释偏差)进行评分。我们发现,三组人在 PEDQ 和 PMLD 方面存在差异,而在 GSI 方面没有显著差异,并且三者呈正相关。较差的心理健康状况和较高的 PMLD 仅与对乱码动画的威胁性解释增强有关。这些发现对于理解威胁偏差在心理健康和移民经历中的作用具有重要意义。
{"title":"Post-migration living difficulties and poor mental health associated with increased interpretation bias for threat.","authors":"Anastasia Vikhanova, Marc S Tibber, Isabelle Mareschal","doi":"10.1177/17470218231191442","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231191442","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has found associations between mental health difficulties and interpretation biases, including heightened interpretation of threat from neutral or ambiguous stimuli. Building on this research, we explored associations between interpretation biases (positive and negative) and three constructs that have been linked to migrant experience: mental health symptoms (Global Severity Index [GSI]), Post-Migration Living Difficulties (PMLD), and Perceived Ethnic Discrimination Questionnaire (PEDQ). Two hundred thirty students who identified as first- (<i>n</i> = 94) or second-generation ethnic minority migrants (<i>n</i> = 68), and first-generation White migrants (<i>n</i> = 68) completed measures of GSI, PEDQ, and PMLD. They also performed an interpretation bias task using Point Light Walkers (PLW), dynamic stimuli with reduced visual input that are easily perceived as humans performing an action. Five categories of PLW were used: four that clearly depicted human forms undertaking positive, neutral, negative, or ambiguous actions, and a fifth that involved scrambled animations with no clear action or form. Participants were asked to imagine their interaction with the stimuli and rate their friendliness (positive interpretation bias) and aggressiveness (interpretation bias for threat). We found that the three groups differed on PEDQ and PMLD, with no significant differences in GSI, and the three measured were positively correlated. Poorer mental health and increased PMLD were associated with a heightened interpretation for threat of scrambled animations only. These findings have implications for understanding of the role of threat biases in mental health and the migrant experience.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1154-1168"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11103921/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9974077","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of the design of mental rotation trials on performance and possible differences between sexes: A theoretical review and experimental investigation. 心理旋转试验的设计对表现的影响以及性别之间可能存在的差异:理论综述和实验研究。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-13 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231200127
Leonardo Jost, Petra Jansen

Sex differences in mental rotation performance are one of the largest in cognitive psychology. Men outperform women by up to 1 SD in psychometric mental rotation tests, but it is often neglected that there are no or only small sex differences for chronometric tests. As both tests are supposed to measure the same ability, we suspect some features of the tests themselves to affect sex differences in performance. Following a theoretical review of the test features, we evaluate the effects of the number of possible answer alternatives, whether they are presented as pairwise mirrored, and their interaction on sex differences in mental rotation performance. In an online experiment, 838 German-speaking participants, 421 women, 417 men, Mage =  42.58 (SD = 12.54) years, solved four blocks of mental rotation trials with two or eight alternatives, which were either pairwise mirrored or not. The results show that that the overall performance was lower for more alternatives and for mixed alternatives but not for their interaction. We could not determine explanations for sex differences as we did not observe meaningful sex differences at all. Possible reasons include the differences between men and women in age and education. This study suggests that the differences between tests affect performance. Sex differences, however, need more investigation, including possible effects and interactions of the test design, education, and age.

心理旋转表现的性别差异是认知心理学中最大的差异之一。在心理测量心理旋转测试中,男性比女性高出1 SD,但人们经常忽视的是,在时间测量测试中没有或只有很小的性别差异。由于这两项测试都应该测量相同的能力,我们怀疑测试本身的一些特征会影响表现中的性别差异。在对测试特征进行理论回顾后,我们评估了可能的答案替代品的数量、它们是否以成对镜像的方式呈现,以及它们的相互作用对心理旋转表现中的性别差异的影响。在一项在线实验中,838名讲德语的参与者,421名女性,417名男性,Mage =  42.58(标准差 = 12.54)年,用两个或八个备选方案解决了四个心理旋转试验块,这些备选方案要么成对镜像,要么不镜像。结果表明,对于更多的替代品和混合的替代品,总体性能较低,但对于它们的相互作用则不然。我们无法确定性别差异的解释,因为我们根本没有观察到有意义的性别差异。可能的原因包括男女在年龄和教育方面的差异。这项研究表明,测试之间的差异会影响表现。然而,性别差异需要更多的调查,包括测试设计、教育和年龄可能产生的影响和相互作用。
{"title":"The influence of the design of mental rotation trials on performance and possible differences between sexes: A theoretical review and experimental investigation.","authors":"Leonardo Jost, Petra Jansen","doi":"10.1177/17470218231200127","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231200127","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sex differences in mental rotation performance are one of the largest in cognitive psychology. Men outperform women by up to 1 <i>SD</i> in psychometric mental rotation tests, but it is often neglected that there are no or only small sex differences for chronometric tests. As both tests are supposed to measure the same ability, we suspect some features of the tests themselves to affect sex differences in performance. Following a theoretical review of the test features, we evaluate the effects of the number of possible answer alternatives, whether they are presented as pairwise mirrored, and their interaction on sex differences in mental rotation performance. In an online experiment, 838 German-speaking participants, 421 women, 417 men, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> =  42.58 (<i>SD</i> = 12.54) years, solved four blocks of mental rotation trials with two or eight alternatives, which were either pairwise mirrored or not. The results show that that the overall performance was lower for more alternatives and for mixed alternatives but not for their interaction. We could not determine explanations for sex differences as we did not observe meaningful sex differences at all. Possible reasons include the differences between men and women in age and education. This study suggests that the differences between tests affect performance. Sex differences, however, need more investigation, including possible effects and interactions of the test design, education, and age.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1250-1271"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11103899/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10115251","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influence of the physical effort of reminder-setting on strategic offloading of delayed intentions. 提醒设置的物理努力对延迟意图的战略卸载的影响。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-23 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231199977
Gavin Chiu, Sam J Gilbert

Intention offloading involves using external reminders such as diaries, to-do lists, and digital alerts to help us remember delayed intentions. Recent studies have provided evidence for various cognitive and metacognitive factors that guide intention offloading, but little research has investigated the physical cost of reminder-setting itself. Here, we present two pre-registered experiments investigating how the cost of physical effort associated with reminder-setting influences strategic intention offloading under different levels of memory load. At all memory loads, reminder-setting was reduced when it was more effortful. The ability to set reminders allowed participants to compensate for the influence of memory load on accuracy in the low-effort condition; this effect was attenuated in the high-effort condition. In addition, there was evidence that participants with less confidence in their memory abilities were more likely to set reminders. Contrary to prediction, physical effort had the greatest effect on reminder-setting at intermediate memory loads. We speculate that the physical costs of reminder-setting might have the greatest impact when participants are uncertain about their strategy choice. These results demonstrate the importance of physical effort as one of the factors relevant to cost-benefit decision-making about cognitive offloading strategies.

意向卸载包括使用日记、待办事项列表和数字提醒等外部提醒来帮助我们记住延迟的意向。最近的研究为指导意图转移的各种认知和元认知因素提供了证据,但很少有研究调查提醒设置本身的物理成本。在这里,我们提出了两个预先注册的实验,研究在不同的内存负载水平下,与提醒设置相关的体力劳动成本如何影响策略意图卸载。在所有内存负载下,提醒设置在更努力时都会减少。设置提醒的能力允许参与者在低努力条件下补偿记忆负荷对准确性的影响;这种效应在高强度条件下减弱。此外,有证据表明,对自己的记忆能力信心不足的参与者更有可能设置提醒。与预测相反,在中等记忆负荷下,体力劳动对提醒设置的影响最大。我们推测,当参与者对自己的策略选择不确定时,设置提醒的物理成本可能会产生最大的影响。这些结果表明,体力劳动作为认知卸载策略成本效益决策的相关因素之一的重要性。
{"title":"Influence of the physical effort of reminder-setting on strategic offloading of delayed intentions.","authors":"Gavin Chiu, Sam J Gilbert","doi":"10.1177/17470218231199977","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231199977","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intention offloading involves using external reminders such as diaries, to-do lists, and digital alerts to help us remember delayed intentions. Recent studies have provided evidence for various cognitive and metacognitive factors that guide intention offloading, but little research has investigated the physical cost of reminder-setting itself. Here, we present two pre-registered experiments investigating how the cost of physical effort associated with reminder-setting influences strategic intention offloading under different levels of memory load. At all memory loads, reminder-setting was reduced when it was more effortful. The ability to set reminders allowed participants to compensate for the influence of memory load on accuracy in the low-effort condition; this effect was attenuated in the high-effort condition. In addition, there was evidence that participants with less confidence in their memory abilities were more likely to set reminders. Contrary to prediction, physical effort had the greatest effect on reminder-setting at intermediate memory loads. We speculate that the physical costs of reminder-setting might have the greatest impact when participants are uncertain about their strategy choice. These results demonstrate the importance of physical effort as one of the factors relevant to cost-benefit decision-making about cognitive offloading strategies.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1295-1311"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11103908/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10111466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stability of individual differences in implicitly guided attention. 内隐引导注意个体差异的稳定性
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-02 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231196463
Chen Chen, Vanessa G Lee

Daily activities often occur in familiar environments, affording us an opportunity to learn. Laboratory studies have shown that people readily acquire an implicit spatial preference for locations that frequently contained a search target in the past. These studies, however, have focused on group characteristics, downplaying the significance of individual differences. In a pre-registered study, we examined the stability of individual differences in two variants of an implicit location probability learning (LPL) task. We tested the possibility that individual differences were stable in variants that shared the same search process, but not in variants involving different search processes. In Experiment 1, participants performed alternating blocks of T-among-Ls and 5-among-2s search tasks. Unbeknownst to them, the search target appeared disproportionately often in one region of space; the high-probability regions differed between the two tasks. LPL transferred between the two tasks. In addition, individuals who showed greater LPL in the T-task also did so in the 5-task and vice versa. In Experiment 2, participants searched for either a camouflaged-T against background noise or a well-segmented T among well-segmented Ls. These two tasks produced task-specific learning that did not transfer between tasks. Moreover, individual differences in learning did not correlate between tasks. Thus, LPL is associated with stable individual differences across variants, but only when the variants share common search processes.

日常活动通常发生在熟悉的环境中,这为我们提供了学习的机会。实验室研究表明,对于过去经常包含搜索目标的地点,人们很容易获得内隐的空间偏好。然而,这些研究主要关注的是群体特征,而忽略了个体差异的重要性。在一项预先登记的研究中,我们考察了内隐位置概率学习(LPL)任务的两种变体中个体差异的稳定性。我们测试了个体差异在具有相同搜索过程的变体中是否稳定,而在涉及不同搜索过程的变体中是否稳定。在实验 1 中,参与者交替完成 T-among-Ls 和 5-among-2s 搜索任务。在他们不知道的情况下,搜索目标不成比例地频繁出现在空间的某个区域;两个任务的高概率区域不同。LPL在两个任务之间发生转移。此外,在 T 任务中表现出更大 LPL 的个体在 5 任务中也表现出更大的 LPL,反之亦然。在实验 2 中,被试要么在背景噪音中寻找伪装的 T,要么在清晰分段的 L 中寻找清晰分段的 T。这两项任务产生了特定任务的学习效果,但在不同任务之间并没有发生转移。此外,学习的个体差异在不同任务之间也没有关联。因此,LPL 与不同变体之间稳定的个体差异有关,但只有当变体具有共同的搜索过程时才会如此。
{"title":"Stability of individual differences in implicitly guided attention.","authors":"Chen Chen, Vanessa G Lee","doi":"10.1177/17470218231196463","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231196463","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Daily activities often occur in familiar environments, affording us an opportunity to learn. Laboratory studies have shown that people readily acquire an implicit spatial preference for locations that frequently contained a search target in the past. These studies, however, have focused on group characteristics, downplaying the significance of individual differences. In a pre-registered study, we examined the stability of individual differences in two variants of an implicit location probability learning (LPL) task. We tested the possibility that individual differences were stable in variants that shared the same search process, but not in variants involving different search processes. In Experiment 1, participants performed alternating blocks of T-among-Ls and 5-among-2s search tasks. Unbeknownst to them, the search target appeared disproportionately often in one region of space; the high-probability regions differed between the two tasks. LPL transferred between the two tasks. In addition, individuals who showed greater LPL in the T-task also did so in the 5-task and vice versa. In Experiment 2, participants searched for either a camouflaged-T against background noise or a well-segmented T among well-segmented Ls. These two tasks produced task-specific learning that did not transfer between tasks. Moreover, individual differences in learning did not correlate between tasks. Thus, LPL is associated with stable individual differences across variants, but only when the variants share common search processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1332-1351"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10508891","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When leaders disclose uncertainty: Effects of expressing internal and external uncertainty about a decision. 当领导者披露不确定性时:表达决策的内部和外部不确定性的影响。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-31 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231204350
Erik Løhre, Karl Halvor Teigen

It is generally assumed that decision-makers appear more competent and trustworthy when exuding confidence in their choices. However, many decisions are by their nature uncertain. Is it possible for a decision-maker to admit uncertainty and still be trusted? We propose that the communicated type of uncertainty may matter. Internal uncertainty, which signals lack of knowledge or a low degree of belief, may be viewed more negatively than external uncertainty, which is associated with randomness and complexity. The results of a series of experiments suggested that people viewed leaders as more competent when they expressed uncertainty about a decision in external ("It is uncertain") rather than internal terms ("I am uncertain"), overall effect size d = 0.45 [0.16, 0.74]. Paradoxically, when asked directly, participants expressed that leaders should be open about uncertainty rather than exuding confidence and downplaying uncertainty. A final study suggested that decision makers were more willing to reveal uncertainty about a choice to others when they perceived the uncertainty as more external and less internal and expected more positive and fewer negative consequences from expressing external rather than internal uncertainty.

人们普遍认为,当决策者对自己的选择充满信心时,他们看起来更有能力,更值得信赖。然而,许多决定本质上是不确定的。决策者是否有可能承认不确定性并仍然值得信任?我们建议,沟通类型的不确定性可能很重要。内部不确定性是缺乏知识或信仰程度低的信号,可能比外部不确定性更负面,外部不确定性与随机性和复杂性有关。一系列实验的结果表明,当人们用外部术语(“这是不确定的”)而不是内部术语(“我不确定”)表达对决策的不确定性时,他们认为领导者更有能力 = 0.45[0.16,0.74]。矛盾的是,当被直接问及时,参与者表示,领导者应该对不确定性持开放态度,而不是表现出信心和淡化不确定性。最后一项研究表明,当决策者认为不确定性更多是外部的而不是内部的,并期望表达外部而不是内部不确定性会产生更多积极和更少消极的后果时,他们更愿意向他人透露选择的不确定性。
{"title":"When leaders disclose uncertainty: Effects of expressing internal and external uncertainty about a decision.","authors":"Erik Løhre, Karl Halvor Teigen","doi":"10.1177/17470218231204350","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231204350","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is generally assumed that decision-makers appear more competent and trustworthy when exuding confidence in their choices. However, many decisions are by their nature uncertain. Is it possible for a decision-maker to admit uncertainty and still be trusted? We propose that the communicated type of uncertainty may matter. Internal uncertainty, which signals lack of knowledge or a low degree of belief, may be viewed more negatively than external uncertainty, which is associated with randomness and complexity. The results of a series of experiments suggested that people viewed leaders as more competent when they expressed uncertainty about a decision in external (\"It is uncertain\") rather than internal terms (\"I am uncertain\"), overall effect size <i>d</i> = 0.45 [0.16, 0.74]. Paradoxically, when asked directly, participants expressed that leaders <i>should</i> be open about uncertainty rather than exuding confidence and downplaying uncertainty. A final study suggested that decision makers were more willing to reveal uncertainty about a choice to others when they perceived the uncertainty as more external and less internal and expected more positive and fewer negative consequences from expressing external rather than internal uncertainty.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1221-1237"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11134984/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10302040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of the Uznadze illusion is temporally dynamic in closed-loop but temporally constant in open-loop grasping. 表达:Uznadze错觉的效果在闭环中是时间动态的,但在开环抓取中是时间恒定的。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-03 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231206907
Stefano Uccelli, Nicola Bruno

Although it is known that the availability of visual feedback modulates grasping kinematics, it is unclear whether this extends to both the early and late stages of the movement. We tackled this issue by exposing participants to the Uznadze illusion (a medium stimulus appears larger or smaller after exposure to smaller or larger inducers). After seeing smaller or larger discs, participants grasped a medium disc with (closed-loop [CL]) or without (open-loop [OL]) visual feedback. Our main aim was to assess whether the time course of the illusion from the movement onset up to the grasp differed between OL and CL. Moreover, we compared OL and CL illusory effects on maximum grip aperture (MGA) and tested whether preparation time, movement time, and time to MGA predicted illusion magnitude. Results revealed that CL illusory effects decreased over movement time, whereas OL ones remained constant. At the time of MGA, OL, and CL effects were, however, of similar size. Although OL grasps were longer to prepare and showed earlier and larger MGAs, such differences had little impact on modulating the illusion. These results suggest that the early stage of grasping is sensitive to the Uznadze illusion both under CL and OL conditions, whereas the late phase is sensitive to it only under OL conditions. We discuss these findings within the framework of theoretical models on the functional properties of the dorsal stream for visually guided actions.

尽管众所周知,无视觉抓握会影响运动学,但尚不清楚这是否会延伸到抓握的早期和晚期。我们通过让参与者暴露于Uznadze错觉来解决这个问题(在暴露于较小或较大的诱导剂后,中等刺激看起来较大或较小)。在看到更小或更大的圆盘后,参与者抓住一个具有(闭环,CL)或不具有(开环,OL)视觉的中等圆盘。主要目的是评估OL和CL从运动开始到抓握的错觉时间过程是否不同。此外,我们比较了OL和CL错觉对最大抓握孔径(MGA)的影响,并检查了准备时间、运动时间和MGA时间是否影响错觉大小。结果表明,CL错觉效应随着运动时间的推移而减少,而OL错觉效应保持不变。然而,在MGA时,OL和CL效应的大小相似。尽管OL抓握需要更长的时间来准备,并且表现出更早和更大的MGA,但这种差异对调节幻觉几乎没有影响。这些结果表明,无论是CL还是OL,抓握的早期阶段都对Uznadze错觉敏感,而晚期阶段仅对OL敏感。我们在视觉引导动作的背流功能特性的理论模型中讨论了这些发现。
{"title":"The effect of the Uznadze illusion is temporally dynamic in closed-loop but temporally constant in open-loop grasping.","authors":"Stefano Uccelli, Nicola Bruno","doi":"10.1177/17470218231206907","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231206907","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although it is known that the availability of visual feedback modulates grasping kinematics, it is unclear whether this extends to both the early and late stages of the movement. We tackled this issue by exposing participants to the Uznadze illusion (a medium stimulus appears larger or smaller after exposure to smaller or larger inducers). After seeing smaller or larger discs, participants grasped a medium disc with (closed-loop [CL]) or without (open-loop [OL]) visual feedback. Our main aim was to assess whether the time course of the illusion from the movement onset up to the grasp differed between OL and CL. Moreover, we compared OL and CL illusory effects on maximum grip aperture (MGA) and tested whether preparation time, movement time, and time to MGA predicted illusion magnitude. Results revealed that CL illusory effects decreased over movement time, whereas OL ones remained constant. At the time of MGA, OL, and CL effects were, however, of similar size. Although OL grasps were longer to prepare and showed earlier and larger MGAs, such differences had little impact on modulating the illusion. These results suggest that the early stage of grasping is sensitive to the Uznadze illusion both under CL and OL conditions, whereas the late phase is sensitive to it only under OL conditions. We discuss these findings within the framework of theoretical models on the functional properties of the dorsal stream for visually guided actions.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1238-1249"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41108862","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Performance-related feedback as a strategy to overcome spontaneous occupational stereotypes. 与绩效相关的反馈是克服自发职业定型观念的一种策略。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-08 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231196861
Eimear Finnegan, Alan Garnham, Jane Oakhill

This article investigates the use of performance-related feedback as a strategy for overcoming spontaneous occupational stereotyping when certain social role nouns and professional terms are read. Across two studies participants were presented with two terms: a role noun (e.g., surgeon) and a kinship term (e.g., mother) and asked to quickly decide whether both terms could refer to the same person. The feedback training involved telling participants whether their responses were correct or incorrect and providing them with their cumulative percentage correct score. In the absence of feedback, responding to stereotype-incongruent pairings was typically slower and less accurate than in stereotype-congruent and neutral conditions. However, the results demonstrated that performance significantly improved to stimuli on which participants received the feedback training (Experiment 1), and to a novel set of stimuli (Experiment 2). In addition, the effects were still evident 1 week later (Experiment 2). It is concluded that performance-related feedback is a valuable strategy for overcoming spontaneous activation of occupational stereotypes and can result in lower levels of stereotype use.

本文研究了在阅读某些社会角色名词和专业术语时,使用与表现相关的反馈作为克服自发职业刻板印象的策略。在两项研究中,研究人员向参与者展示了两个术语:一个角色名词(如外科医生)和一个亲属关系术语(如母亲),并要求参与者快速判断这两个术语是否指同一个人。反馈训练包括告诉参与者他们的回答是正确的还是错误的,并向他们提供累计正确率分数。在没有反馈的情况下,对刻板印象-不一致配对的反应通常比刻板印象-一致和中性条件下的反应更慢、更不准确。然而,实验结果表明,对于接受过反馈训练的刺激物(实验 1)和一组新刺激物(实验 2),被试的反应能力明显提高。此外,这种效果在一周后仍然明显(实验 2)。结论是,与成绩相关的反馈是克服自发激活职业刻板印象的一种有价值的策略,可以降低刻板印象的使用水平。
{"title":"Performance-related feedback as a strategy to overcome spontaneous occupational stereotypes.","authors":"Eimear Finnegan, Alan Garnham, Jane Oakhill","doi":"10.1177/17470218231196861","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231196861","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This article investigates the use of performance-related feedback as a strategy for overcoming spontaneous occupational stereotyping when certain social role nouns and professional terms are read. Across two studies participants were presented with two terms: a role noun (e.g., surgeon) and a kinship term (e.g., mother) and asked to quickly decide whether both terms could refer to the same person. The feedback training involved telling participants whether their responses were correct or incorrect and providing them with their cumulative percentage correct score. In the absence of feedback, responding to stereotype-incongruent pairings was typically slower and less accurate than in stereotype-congruent and neutral conditions. However, the results demonstrated that performance significantly improved to stimuli on which participants received the feedback training (Experiment 1), and to a novel set of stimuli (Experiment 2). In addition, the effects were still evident 1 week later (Experiment 2). It is concluded that performance-related feedback is a valuable strategy for overcoming spontaneous activation of occupational stereotypes and can result in lower levels of stereotype use.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1312-1331"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11103928/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10176157","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Face perception and facial emotional expression recognition ability: Both unique predictors of the broader autism phenotype. 面部感知和面部情绪表达识别能力:两者都是更广泛自闭症表型的独特预测因素。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231203679
Gilles E Gignac, Romina Palermo, Ellen Bothe, Dana L Walker, Jeremy B Wilmer

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and the broader autistic phenotype (BAP) have been suggested to be associated with perceptual-cognitive difficulties processing human faces. However, the empirical results are mixed, arguably, in part due to inadequate samples and analyses. Consequently, we administered the Cambridge Face Perception Test (CFPT), the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET), a vocabulary test, and the Autism Quotient (AQ) to a sample of 318 adults in the general community. Based on a disattenuated path analytic modelling strategy, we found that both face perception ability (β = -.21) and facial emotional expression recognition ability (β = -.27) predicted uniquely and significantly the Communication dimension of AQ. Vocabulary failed to yield a significant, direct effect onto the Communication dimension of the AQ. We conclude that difficulties perceiving information from the faces of others may contribute to difficulties in nonverbal communication, as conceptualised and measured within the context of BAP.

自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)和更广泛的自闭症表型(BAP)已被认为与处理人脸的感知认知困难有关。然而,可以说,实证结果喜忧参半,部分原因是样本和分析不足。因此,我们对318名普通社区成年人进行了剑桥人脸感知测试(CFPT)、阅读眼睛中的想法测试(RMET)、词汇测试和自闭症商(AQ)。基于离散路径分析建模策略,我们发现两种人脸感知能力(β = -.21)和面部情绪表情识别能力(β = -.27)唯一且显著地预测了AQ的通信维度。词汇未能对AQ的沟通维度产生显著、直接的影响。我们得出的结论是,从他人脸上感知信息的困难可能会导致非言语交际的困难,这是在BAP的背景下概念化和衡量的。
{"title":"Face perception and facial emotional expression recognition ability: Both unique predictors of the broader autism phenotype.","authors":"Gilles E Gignac, Romina Palermo, Ellen Bothe, Dana L Walker, Jeremy B Wilmer","doi":"10.1177/17470218231203679","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231203679","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and the broader autistic phenotype (BAP) have been suggested to be associated with perceptual-cognitive difficulties processing human faces. However, the empirical results are mixed, arguably, in part due to inadequate samples and analyses. Consequently, we administered the Cambridge Face Perception Test (CFPT), the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET), a vocabulary test, and the Autism Quotient (AQ) to a sample of 318 adults in the general community. Based on a disattenuated path analytic modelling strategy, we found that both face perception ability (β = -.21) and facial emotional expression recognition ability (β = -.27) predicted uniquely and significantly the Communication dimension of AQ. Vocabulary failed to yield a significant, direct effect onto the Communication dimension of the AQ. We conclude that difficulties perceiving information from the faces of others may contribute to difficulties in nonverbal communication, as conceptualised and measured within the context of BAP.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1140-1153"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11103922/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10297181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reading(,) with and without commas. 阅读带逗号和不带逗号的(,)。
IF 1.7 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-06-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-03 DOI: 10.1177/17470218231200338
Bernhard Angele, Ismael Gutiérrez-Cordero, Manuel Perea, Ana Marcet

All major writing systems mandate the use of commas to separate clauses and list items. However, casual writers often omit mandatory commas. Little empirical or theoretical research has been done on the effect that omitting mandatory commas has on eye movement control during reading. We present an eye-tracking experiment in Spanish, a language with a clear standard as to mandatory comma use. Sentences were presented with or without mandatory commas while readers' eye movements were recorded. There was a local increase in the go-past time for the pre-comma region when commas were presented, which was balanced out by shorter first-pass and second-pass times on the subsequent regions. In global sentence reading time, there was no evidence for an advantage of presenting commas. These findings suggest that, even when commas are mandatory, their effect is primarily to shift when processing takes place rather than to facilitate processing overall.

所有主要的写作系统都要求使用逗号来分隔从句和列表项。然而,随意的作者经常省略强制性逗号。关于省略强制性逗号对阅读过程中眼动控制的影响,很少有实证或理论研究。我们用西班牙语做了一个眼动追踪实验,西班牙语对强制性逗号使用有明确的标准。在记录读者眼球运动的同时,呈现的句子带有或不带有强制性逗号。当出现逗号时,逗号前区域的通过时间局部增加,随后区域的第一次通过和第二次通过时间缩短抵消了这一点。在全球句子阅读时间里,没有证据表明使用逗号有好处。这些发现表明,即使逗号是强制性的,它们的作用也主要是在处理发生时发生变化,而不是促进整体处理。
{"title":"Reading(,) with and without commas.","authors":"Bernhard Angele, Ismael Gutiérrez-Cordero, Manuel Perea, Ana Marcet","doi":"10.1177/17470218231200338","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218231200338","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>All major writing systems mandate the use of commas to separate clauses and list items. However, casual writers often omit mandatory commas. Little empirical or theoretical research has been done on the effect that omitting mandatory commas has on eye movement control during reading. We present an eye-tracking experiment in Spanish, a language with a clear standard as to mandatory comma use. Sentences were presented with or without mandatory commas while readers' eye movements were recorded. There was a local increase in the go-past time for the pre-comma region when commas were presented, which was balanced out by shorter first-pass and second-pass times on the subsequent regions. In global sentence reading time, there was no evidence for an advantage of presenting commas. These findings suggest that, even when commas are mandatory, their effect is primarily to shift when processing takes place rather than to facilitate processing overall.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"1190-1200"},"PeriodicalIF":1.7,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10483827","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1