首页 > 最新文献

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Preexposure to running attenuates rats' running-based flavour avoidance: Testing associative blocking with a cover cues or context change. 暴露于奔跑中会减弱大鼠基于奔跑的味道回避:用掩护线索或情境变化测试联想阻断。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241237646
Sadahiko Nakajima

Voluntary running in activity wheels by rats leads to a Pavlovian conditioned aversion to the flavour consumed immediately before the running, causing the rats to avoid that flavour. This learning process, known as running-based flavour avoidance learning (FAL), is weakened when the rats have had repeated exposure to the wheels before. According to the associative account, the association between the background context and running established during the preexposure phase blocks the conditioning of the target flavour because the running is highly predictable by the background context from the outset of the FAL phase. Experiments 1 and 2 examined this account by introducing another flavour as a cue signalling wheel access during the preexposure phase. In the framework of the associative account, the introduction of this cue should impede the formation of the context-running association during the preexposure phase, thereby hindering the contextual blocking of aversive conditioning for the target flavour in the FAL phase. This would result in unweakened FAL. Although the results of Experiment 1 align with this prediction, in Experiment 2, when highly distinct flavours were used as the target and second cues, the preexposure effect was not eliminated. This contradicts the predictions of the associative account, indicating that Experiment 1 may have been influenced by stimulus generalisation. In Experiment 3, changing background contexts between the preexposure and FAL phases had no impact on the preexposure effect, contrary to the predictions of the associative account. In general, the associative account was not supported.

大鼠在活动轮中的自愿奔跑会导致对奔跑前食用的味道产生巴甫洛夫条件反射,从而使大鼠回避该味道。这种学习过程被称为 "基于跑步的味道回避学习(FAL)",当大鼠之前反复接触过车轮时,这种学习过程就会减弱。根据联想理论,在暴露前阶段建立的背景环境与奔跑之间的联想会阻碍目标味道的条件反射,因为在FAL阶段一开始,奔跑就很容易被背景环境预测到。实验1和2通过在暴露前阶段引入另一种味道作为车轮进入的线索,对这一解释进行了检验。在联想理论的框架下,引入这种线索应该会阻碍在暴露前阶段形成情境运行联想,从而阻碍在FAL阶段对目标味道的厌恶性条件反射的情境阻断。这将导致FAL未被削弱。实验1的结果与这一预测一致,但在实验2中,当使用高度不同的味道作为目标和第二线索时,暴露前效应并没有消除。这与联想理论的预测相矛盾,表明实验 1 可能受到了刺激泛化的影响。在实验 3 中,在暴露前和 FAL 阶段之间改变背景情境对暴露前效应没有影响,这与联想理论的预测相反。总的来说,联想理论没有得到支持。
{"title":"Preexposure to running attenuates rats' running-based flavour avoidance: Testing associative blocking with a cover cues or context change.","authors":"Sadahiko Nakajima","doi":"10.1177/17470218241237646","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241237646","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Voluntary running in activity wheels by rats leads to a Pavlovian conditioned aversion to the flavour consumed immediately before the running, causing the rats to avoid that flavour. This learning process, known as running-based flavour avoidance learning (FAL), is weakened when the rats have had repeated exposure to the wheels before. According to the associative account, the association between the background context and running established during the preexposure phase blocks the conditioning of the target flavour because the running is highly predictable by the background context from the outset of the FAL phase. Experiments 1 and 2 examined this account by introducing another flavour as a cue signalling wheel access during the preexposure phase. In the framework of the associative account, the introduction of this cue should impede the formation of the context-running association during the preexposure phase, thereby hindering the contextual blocking of aversive conditioning for the target flavour in the FAL phase. This would result in unweakened FAL. Although the results of Experiment 1 align with this prediction, in Experiment 2, when highly distinct flavours were used as the target and second cues, the preexposure effect was not eliminated. This contradicts the predictions of the associative account, indicating that Experiment 1 may have been influenced by stimulus generalisation. In Experiment 3, changing background contexts between the preexposure and FAL phases had no impact on the preexposure effect, contrary to the predictions of the associative account. In general, the associative account was not supported.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"459-473"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139940704","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting the influence of phonological similarity on cognate processing: Evidence from Cantonese-Japanese bilinguals. 表达:重新审视语音相似性对认知加工的影响:来自粤日双语者的证据。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241242631
Brian W L Wong, Shawn Hemelstrand, Tomohiro Inoue

The influences of shared orthography, semantics, and phonology on bilingual cognate processing have been investigated extensively. However, mixed results have been found regarding the effects of phonological similarity on L2 cognate processing. In addition, most existing studies examining the influence of phonological similarity on cognate processing have been conducted on alphabetic scripts, in which phonology and orthography are always associated. Hence, in this study, we recruited Cantonese-Japanese bilinguals who used two logographic scripts, traditional Chinese and Japanese Kanji, to examine the influence of phonological similarity on L2 cognate lexical decision. Importantly, these scripts allow the manipulation of phonological similarity using identical characters across both languages. In addition, we examined how word frequency and L2 proficiency modulate cognate processing. Results showed that although word frequency and L2 proficiency played important roles in cognate processing, there was minimal overall influence of phonological similarity on cognate lexical decision. The latter finding suggests that theoretical models of bilingual word recognition may need to be refined to enhance our understanding of cognate processing regarding the role of phonology among diverse bilingual populations.

关于共同正字法、语义学和语音学对二语认知加工的影响,已有大量研究。然而,关于语音相似性对第二语言认知加工的影响,研究结果喜忧参半。此外,现有的大多数研究都是针对字母文字进行的,其中语音和正字法总是相关联的。因此,在本研究中,我们招募了使用繁体中文和日文汉字这两种逻各斯文字的粤日双语者,以考察语音相似性对 L2 同源词决策的影响。重要的是,这些脚本允许在两种语言中使用相同的字符来操纵语音相似性。此外,我们还研究了词频和 L2 熟练程度如何调节认知加工。结果表明,虽然词频和第二语言能力在认知加工中起着重要作用,但语音相似性对认知词汇决策的总体影响却微乎其微。后一项发现表明,我们可能需要完善二语词汇识别的理论模型,以加深我们对不同二语人群中语音作用的认知加工的理解。
{"title":"Revisiting the influence of phonological similarity on cognate processing: Evidence from Cantonese-Japanese bilinguals.","authors":"Brian W L Wong, Shawn Hemelstrand, Tomohiro Inoue","doi":"10.1177/17470218241242631","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241242631","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The influences of shared orthography, semantics, and phonology on bilingual cognate processing have been investigated extensively. However, mixed results have been found regarding the effects of phonological similarity on L2 cognate processing. In addition, most existing studies examining the influence of phonological similarity on cognate processing have been conducted on alphabetic scripts, in which phonology and orthography are always associated. Hence, in this study, we recruited Cantonese-Japanese bilinguals who used two logographic scripts, traditional Chinese and Japanese Kanji, to examine the influence of phonological similarity on L2 cognate lexical decision. Importantly, these scripts allow the manipulation of phonological similarity using identical characters across both languages. In addition, we examined how word frequency and L2 proficiency modulate cognate processing. Results showed that although word frequency and L2 proficiency played important roles in cognate processing, there was minimal overall influence of phonological similarity on cognate lexical decision. The latter finding suggests that theoretical models of bilingual word recognition may need to be refined to enhance our understanding of cognate processing regarding the role of phonology among diverse bilingual populations.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"619-637"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140137144","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
EXPRESS: No evidence for the efficiency of the eye-tracking-based RMET version at detecting differences of mind reading abilities across psychological traits.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251326569
Bertrand Beffara, Marina Veyrie, Laura Mauduit, Lara Bardi, Irene Cristofori

The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test" (RMET) is one of the most used tests of theory of mind. Its principle is to match an emotion word to the corresponding face image. The performance at this test has been associated with multiple psychological variables including personality, loneliness and empathy. Recently, however, the validity of the RMET has been questioned. An alternative version of the test has been tested using eye-tracking (Russell et al., 2021) in addition to manual responses and was hypothesized to be more sensitive. Here, we put this hypothesis to the test by attempting to reproduce already-assessed correlational results between the performance at the classical RMET and the self-reported personality, loneliness and empathy, now using eye-gaze as an RMET performance index. Despite a marked eye-gaze bias towards the face image corresponding to the target word, the eye-gaze pattern correlated with none of the self-reported psychological variables. This result highlights the interest in using eye-tracking for theory of mind tests, while questioning the robustness of the association between psychological variables and RMET performance, and the validity of the RMET itself.

"读心测试"(RMET)是最常用的心智理论测试之一。其原理是将一个情绪词与相应的面部图像相匹配。该测试的表现与多种心理变量有关,包括人格、孤独感和同理心。然而,最近 RMET 的有效性受到了质疑。除了手动反应之外,我们还使用眼动跟踪(Russell 等人,2021 年)对该测试的另一个版本进行了测试,并假设该版本更加灵敏。在此,我们尝试重现经典 RMET 测试成绩与自我报告的人格、孤独感和同理心之间已经评估过的相关结果,并将眼动作为 RMET 成绩指数,从而对这一假设进行验证。尽管眼球明显偏向于与目标词相对应的脸部图像,但眼球注视模式与自我报告的心理变量均不相关。这一结果突显了使用眼动跟踪进行心理理论测试的意义,同时也对心理变量与 RMET 成绩之间关联的稳健性以及 RMET 本身的有效性提出了质疑。
{"title":"EXPRESS: No evidence for the efficiency of the eye-tracking-based RMET version at detecting differences of mind reading abilities across psychological traits.","authors":"Bertrand Beffara, Marina Veyrie, Laura Mauduit, Lara Bardi, Irene Cristofori","doi":"10.1177/17470218251326569","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218251326569","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The \"Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test\" (RMET) is one of the most used tests of theory of mind. Its principle is to match an emotion word to the corresponding face image. The performance at this test has been associated with multiple psychological variables including personality, loneliness and empathy. Recently, however, the validity of the RMET has been questioned. An alternative version of the test has been tested using eye-tracking (Russell et al., 2021) in addition to manual responses and was hypothesized to be more sensitive. Here, we put this hypothesis to the test by attempting to reproduce already-assessed correlational results between the performance at the classical RMET and the self-reported personality, loneliness and empathy, now using eye-gaze as an RMET performance index. Despite a marked eye-gaze bias towards the face image corresponding to the target word, the eye-gaze pattern correlated with none of the self-reported psychological variables. This result highlights the interest in using eye-tracking for theory of mind tests, while questioning the robustness of the association between psychological variables and RMET performance, and the validity of the RMET itself.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251326569"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143531865","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Word association task responses prime associations in subsequent trials. 表达:单词联想任务反应在后续试验中的首要联想。
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-03-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-24 DOI: 10.1177/17470218241239321
David Playfoot, Ondrej Burysek

The word association task has been used extensively in psychological and linguistic research as a way of measuring connections between words in the mental lexicon. Interpretation of word association data has assumed that responses represent the strongest association between cue word and response, but there is evidence that participant behaviour can be affected by task instructions and design. This study investigated whether word association responses can be primed by the participants' own response to the preceding cue-that is, whether the order in which cues are presented alters the responses that are generated. Results showed that the proportion of participants who provide a particular association (e.g., acid-RAIN) is greater when their response to the previous cue in the list is also associated with rain (e.g., parasol-UMBRELLA). The same is not true when the two cues are presented non-consecutively. Word association tasks should be administered such that the order in which cues are presented is random for every participant so as to avoid unintentional contamination of associative strength data.

单词联想任务作为一种测量心理词典中单词之间联系的方法,在心理学和语言学研究中被广泛使用。对词语联想数据的解释假定,反应代表了提示词语和反应之间的最强关联,但有证据表明,参与者的行为会受到任务指示和设计的影响。本研究调查了词联想反应是否会受到参与者自身对前面提示词反应的影响,也就是说,提示词的呈现顺序是否会改变所产生的反应。结果显示,当参与者对列表中前一个提示的反应也与雨(如阳伞--UMBRELLA)相关时,提供特定联想(如酸--雨)的参与者比例会更高。而当两个线索非连续出现时,情况就不一样了。在进行词语联想任务时,应确保每个被试的提示顺序都是随机的,以避免无意中污染联想强度数据。
{"title":"Word association task responses prime associations in subsequent trials.","authors":"David Playfoot, Ondrej Burysek","doi":"10.1177/17470218241239321","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218241239321","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The word association task has been used extensively in psychological and linguistic research as a way of measuring connections between words in the mental lexicon. Interpretation of word association data has assumed that responses represent the strongest association between cue word and response, but there is evidence that participant behaviour can be affected by task instructions and design. This study investigated whether word association responses can be primed by the participants' own response to the preceding cue-that is, whether the order in which cues are presented alters the responses that are generated. Results showed that the proportion of participants who provide a particular association (e.g., acid-RAIN) is greater when their response to the previous cue in the list is also associated with rain (e.g., parasol-UMBRELLA). The same is not true when the two cues are presented non-consecutively. Word association tasks should be administered such that the order in which cues are presented is random for every participant so as to avoid unintentional contamination of associative strength data.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"566-574"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11874611/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140013251","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
EXPRESS: Facial attractiveness influenced cooperative behavior in the Stag Hunt game: evidence from neural electrophysiology.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-28 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251326501
Xianjia Wang, Wei Cui, Shuochen Wang, Yang Liu, Hao Yu, Jian Song

Facial attractiveness plays a significant role in interpersonal interactions, influencing various aspects of life. This study is the first to explore, from a neurological perspective, the impact of facial attractiveness on individual cooperative behavior in the context of the Stag Hunt game. 26 participants took part in a two-person Stag Hunt experimental task while their electroencephalogram (EEG) data was recorded. Participants had to decide whether to cooperate with or to defect from a virtual partner in the game, with photos of these partners (high or low attractiveness) shown before the decision. Analysis of the behavioral data indicates that faces with high attractiveness can promote individual cooperative behavior. EEG data analysis revealed that during the facial stimulus presentation phase, low attractiveness faces elicited more negative N2 amplitudes, smaller LPP amplitudes, and larger alpha oscillations compared to high attractiveness faces. During the outcome feedback phase, high attractiveness faces elicited smaller FRN amplitudes, larger P300 amplitudes, and stronger theta oscillations than low attractiveness faces, while loss feedback elicited more negative FRN amplitudes, smaller P300 amplitudes, and larger theta oscillations than gain feedback. These findings indicate that the processing of facial attractiveness occurs early and automatically, and it also influences individuals' evaluation of behavioral outcomes.

{"title":"EXPRESS: Facial attractiveness influenced cooperative behavior in the Stag Hunt game: evidence from neural electrophysiology.","authors":"Xianjia Wang, Wei Cui, Shuochen Wang, Yang Liu, Hao Yu, Jian Song","doi":"10.1177/17470218251326501","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218251326501","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Facial attractiveness plays a significant role in interpersonal interactions, influencing various aspects of life. This study is the first to explore, from a neurological perspective, the impact of facial attractiveness on individual cooperative behavior in the context of the Stag Hunt game. 26 participants took part in a two-person Stag Hunt experimental task while their electroencephalogram (EEG) data was recorded. Participants had to decide whether to cooperate with or to defect from a virtual partner in the game, with photos of these partners (high or low attractiveness) shown before the decision. Analysis of the behavioral data indicates that faces with high attractiveness can promote individual cooperative behavior. EEG data analysis revealed that during the facial stimulus presentation phase, low attractiveness faces elicited more negative N2 amplitudes, smaller LPP amplitudes, and larger alpha oscillations compared to high attractiveness faces. During the outcome feedback phase, high attractiveness faces elicited smaller FRN amplitudes, larger P300 amplitudes, and stronger theta oscillations than low attractiveness faces, while loss feedback elicited more negative FRN amplitudes, smaller P300 amplitudes, and larger theta oscillations than gain feedback. These findings indicate that the processing of facial attractiveness occurs early and automatically, and it also influences individuals' evaluation of behavioral outcomes.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251326501"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143531864","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A megastudy of lexico-semantic effects in single-word shadowing.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-27 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251320371
Zongyu Qian, Winston D Goh

Lexico-semantic effects in lexical decision and semantic categorisation tasks have been investigated using the megastudy approach, but not with other traditional spoken word recognition tasks. To address this gap, this megastudy examined the single-word shadowing task, where 96 native English speakers repeated aloud each word they heard as quickly and as accurately as possible. Item-level hierarchical regression and linear mixed-effects analyses produced identical results: Words with longer token duration were associated with slower response times while high-frequency and phonologically distinctive words were repeated faster. These findings were consistent with previous studies and other tasks, which suggests that lexical effects are task-general in spoken word recognition. However, after controlling for lexical variables, six semantic variables did not account for any additional unique variance in response times. These results suggest that the single-word shadowing task is heavily dependent on lexical processing and can be completed without activating semantics. Cross-task comparisons with another megastudy's data on auditory lexical decision and semantic categorisation tasks further suggest that lexical effects are task-general, while semantic richness effects are task-specific in spoken word recognition.

{"title":"A megastudy of lexico-semantic effects in single-word shadowing.","authors":"Zongyu Qian, Winston D Goh","doi":"10.1177/17470218251320371","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218251320371","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Lexico-semantic effects in lexical decision and semantic categorisation tasks have been investigated using the megastudy approach, but not with other traditional spoken word recognition tasks. To address this gap, this megastudy examined the single-word shadowing task, where 96 native English speakers repeated aloud each word they heard as quickly and as accurately as possible. Item-level hierarchical regression and linear mixed-effects analyses produced identical results: Words with longer token duration were associated with slower response times while high-frequency and phonologically distinctive words were repeated faster. These findings were consistent with previous studies and other tasks, which suggests that lexical effects are task-general in spoken word recognition. However, after controlling for lexical variables, six semantic variables did not account for any additional unique variance in response times. These results suggest that the single-word shadowing task is heavily dependent on lexical processing and can be completed without activating semantics. Cross-task comparisons with another megastudy's data on auditory lexical decision and semantic categorisation tasks further suggest that lexical effects are task-general, while semantic richness effects are task-specific in spoken word recognition.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251320371"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143066685","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
EXPRESS: Does collaborative remembering serve a directive function? Examining the influence of collaborative remembering on subsequent decision making.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-27 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251325246
Magdalena Abel

Remembering together with others can facilitate memory for previously encountered contents, but can also prompt social contagion with information not previously encountered. This study examined whether these effects of collaborative remembering might serve a directive function and guide subsequent individual decisions. Participants were tested in groups of three and completed an adapted version of a prisoner's dilemma. They initially encountered faces of different players on a screen, who cooperated with them or acted as cheaters. Some of these players were encountered by all three participants, others by single participants only. An interpolated memory test on all players was completed individually or collaboratively. During a final decision game, participants were asked to decide whether to cooperate with each player or not. Three experiments were conducted, which additionally varied encoding, the retention interval before the interpolated memory test, and format and instructions for the interpolated memory test. The results consistently showed adaptive decision making. Participants were more likely to cooperate with players who had previously cooperated with them, relative to both new players and cheaters. Interpolated collaborative remembering had no benefit, however; neither for decisions towards directly encountered players, nor for decisions towards players encountered by other participants. Effects of collaborative remembering may thus not serve a directive function and guide future behavior, or at least they may not do so in this adapted version of a prisoner's dilemma.

{"title":"EXPRESS: Does collaborative remembering serve a directive function? Examining the influence of collaborative remembering on subsequent decision making.","authors":"Magdalena Abel","doi":"10.1177/17470218251325246","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218251325246","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Remembering together with others can facilitate memory for previously encountered contents, but can also prompt social contagion with information not previously encountered. This study examined whether these effects of collaborative remembering might serve a directive function and guide subsequent individual decisions. Participants were tested in groups of three and completed an adapted version of a prisoner's dilemma. They initially encountered faces of different players on a screen, who cooperated with them or acted as cheaters. Some of these players were encountered by all three participants, others by single participants only. An interpolated memory test on all players was completed individually or collaboratively. During a final decision game, participants were asked to decide whether to cooperate with each player or not. Three experiments were conducted, which additionally varied encoding, the retention interval before the interpolated memory test, and format and instructions for the interpolated memory test. The results consistently showed adaptive decision making. Participants were more likely to cooperate with players who had previously cooperated with them, relative to both new players and cheaters. Interpolated collaborative remembering had no benefit, however; neither for decisions towards directly encountered players, nor for decisions towards players encountered by other participants. Effects of collaborative remembering may thus not serve a directive function and guide future behavior, or at least they may not do so in this adapted version of a prisoner's dilemma.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251325246"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143515775","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Eye on context: Individual differences reveal the mechanisms of statistical learning.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-26 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251320540
Artyom Zinchenko, Markus Conci, Hermann J Müller, Thomas Geyer

If a searched-for target object is consistently encountered within repeating spatial distractor arrangements, target detection becomes more efficient relative to nonrepeated, that is, random arrangements (contextual cueing [CC] effect). However, target location changes within otherwise unchanged distractor arrays substantially weaken the cueing effect. Previous studies reported substantial variations in individual participants' abilities to learn and relearn invariant contexts. Therefore, the current study examined how individual differences in attentional control and focus, as indexed by the well-established Stroop and Navon tasks, respectively, relate to CC in a learning phase/relocation phase design. During the visual search, we recorded behavioural reaction times (RTs) and fixation locations, the latter permitting us to decompose search RTs into search- and motor-related substages. We could thus evaluate the processes responsible for CC and the lack thereof after target relocation while also testing whether search and motor components of CC are different for individuals depending on their Stroop/Navon scores. Repeated contexts yielded faster RTs (and reduced fixation numbers), though there was a substantial decrease in cueing from learning to adaptation, consistent with previous studies. Critically, contextual learning, but not relearning, varied across individuals: participants with high-Stroop interference displayed overall larger CC during early target search, while a more local Navon task bias was associated with increased CC during later processes of target response decisions. Our results demonstrate that analysing individual differences can help validate the processes responsible for CC in search tasks, particularly distinguishing between early search and later response-related mechanisms.

{"title":"Eye on context: Individual differences reveal the mechanisms of statistical learning.","authors":"Artyom Zinchenko, Markus Conci, Hermann J Müller, Thomas Geyer","doi":"10.1177/17470218251320540","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218251320540","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>If a searched-for target object is consistently encountered within repeating spatial distractor arrangements, target detection becomes more efficient relative to nonrepeated, that is, random arrangements (contextual cueing [CC] effect). However, target location changes within otherwise unchanged distractor arrays substantially weaken the cueing effect. Previous studies reported substantial variations in individual participants' abilities to learn and relearn invariant contexts. Therefore, the current study examined how individual differences in attentional control and focus, as indexed by the well-established Stroop and Navon tasks, respectively, relate to CC in a learning phase/relocation phase design. During the visual search, we recorded behavioural reaction times (RTs) and fixation locations, the latter permitting us to decompose search RTs into search- and motor-related substages. We could thus evaluate the processes responsible for CC and the lack thereof after target relocation while also testing whether search and motor components of CC are different for individuals depending on their Stroop/Navon scores. Repeated contexts yielded faster RTs (and reduced fixation numbers), though there was a substantial decrease in cueing from learning to adaptation, consistent with previous studies. Critically, contextual learning, but not relearning, varied across individuals: participants with high-Stroop interference displayed overall larger CC during early target search, while a more local Navon task bias was associated with increased CC during later processes of target response decisions. Our results demonstrate that analysing individual differences can help validate the processes responsible for CC in search tasks, particularly distinguishing between early search and later response-related mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251320540"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143075244","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hysteresis in reach planning and spatial demonstrative choice.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-22 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251318225
Michael Long, Michael P Kaschak

Recent studies have demonstrated hysteresis in studies of syntactic choice in language production. These studies further show that the hysteresis effects observed in syntactic choice are similar to the effects observed in motor choice tasks. Here, we examine whether hysteresis can be observed for a different kind of linguistic choice, namely the choice between the spatial demonstratives this and that. We further examine whether these hysteresis effects parallel the hysteresis effects found in a similar motor task. Our results demonstrate hysteresis in both spatial demonstrative choice and motor choice. Nevertheless, there were some effects that appeared in the motor task that did not appear in the language task (e.g., a response time advantage for repeated choices).

{"title":"Hysteresis in reach planning and spatial demonstrative choice.","authors":"Michael Long, Michael P Kaschak","doi":"10.1177/17470218251318225","DOIUrl":"10.1177/17470218251318225","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent studies have demonstrated hysteresis in studies of syntactic choice in language production. These studies further show that the hysteresis effects observed in syntactic choice are similar to the effects observed in motor choice tasks. Here, we examine whether hysteresis can be observed for a different kind of linguistic choice, namely the choice between the spatial demonstratives <i>this</i> and <i>that</i>. We further examine whether these hysteresis effects parallel the hysteresis effects found in a similar motor task. Our results demonstrate hysteresis in both spatial demonstrative choice and motor choice. Nevertheless, there were some effects that appeared in the motor task that did not appear in the language task (e.g., a response time advantage for repeated choices).</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251318225"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143029374","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
EXPRESS: Race and Ethnicity Categories Cue Cognitive Control as Evidenced by Transfer.
IF 1.5 3区 心理学 Q4 PHYSIOLOGY Pub Date : 2025-02-21 DOI: 10.1177/17470218251325249
Abhishek Dey, Rachel Brough, Julie Bugg

Learning-guided reactive control is the flexible biasing of attention that is triggered by external cues, such that more focused control settings are retrieved and executed in response to cues predicting higher attentional demands. We investigated whether race/ethnicity is harnessed as a cue to guide control using a social Stroop task in which participants named the race/ethnicity of a face (e.g., Asian) while ignoring a superimposed word that was congruent (e.g., Asian) or incongruent (e.g., White). In the first four experiments, we manipulated item-specific proportion congruence. Faces of some races/ethnicities (e.g., Asian; Black) were mostly congruent and faces of others (e.g., White; Latina) were mostly incongruent. We observed the item-specific proportion congruence effect showing a smaller Stroop effect for mostly incongruent faces. Critically, we found transfer of the effect to faces of each race/ethnicity that were 50% congruent, indicating control at the more abstract, category level (i.e., more focused control setting retrieved and executed for racial/ethnic categories associated with higher attentional demands). Individuating faces did not disrupt category-level control but recategorization of the faces into racially/ethnically diverse teams did, as indicated by the lack of transfer. In a final experiment, we associated proportion congruence (attentional demands) with the conjunction of two social categories (race/ethnicity and gender) and found novel evidence of conjunctive learning-guided control. The findings demonstrate that race/ethnicity (and conjunctions with gender) cues control adjustments, people transfer learned control settings to other members of race/ethnicity categories, and recategorization creates an important boundary condition for transfer.

{"title":"EXPRESS: Race and Ethnicity Categories Cue Cognitive Control as Evidenced by Transfer.","authors":"Abhishek Dey, Rachel Brough, Julie Bugg","doi":"10.1177/17470218251325249","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218251325249","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning-guided reactive control is the flexible biasing of attention that is triggered by external cues, such that more focused control settings are retrieved and executed in response to cues predicting higher attentional demands. We investigated whether race/ethnicity is harnessed as a cue to guide control using a social Stroop task in which participants named the race/ethnicity of a face (e.g., Asian) while ignoring a superimposed word that was congruent (e.g., Asian) or incongruent (e.g., White). In the first four experiments, we manipulated item-specific proportion congruence. Faces of some races/ethnicities (e.g., Asian; Black) were mostly congruent and faces of others (e.g., White; Latina) were mostly incongruent. We observed the item-specific proportion congruence effect showing a smaller Stroop effect for mostly incongruent faces. Critically, we found transfer of the effect to faces of each race/ethnicity that were 50% congruent, indicating control at the more abstract, category level (i.e., more focused control setting retrieved and executed for racial/ethnic categories associated with higher attentional demands). Individuating faces did not disrupt category-level control but recategorization of the faces into racially/ethnically diverse teams did, as indicated by the lack of transfer. In a final experiment, we associated proportion congruence (attentional demands) with the conjunction of two social categories (race/ethnicity and gender) and found novel evidence of conjunctive learning-guided control. The findings demonstrate that race/ethnicity (and conjunctions with gender) cues control adjustments, people transfer learned control settings to other members of race/ethnicity categories, and recategorization creates an important boundary condition for transfer.</p>","PeriodicalId":20869,"journal":{"name":"Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology","volume":" ","pages":"17470218251325249"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2025-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143469072","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1