首页 > 最新文献

Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Gestalt Psychology 格式塔心理学
Pub Date : 2020-03-31 DOI: 10.4135/9781412963947.n203
H. Gundlach
Gestalt psychology is an holistic approach to psychology launched in 1910 by three psychologists: Max Wertheimer, Wolfgang Köhler, and Kurt Koffka. It was conceived to oppose elementary or atomistic psychology, the conception that psychical processes consist of elements whose associations produce the contents experienced in the mind or soul. Instead, Gestalt psychology holds that configurations or, in German, Gestalten, not these hypothetical elements, are the primary material underlying experience. Beginning with research in perception, the Gestalt approach was soon applied to other fields of psychology. Gestalt theory, inspired by field theories in physics, tried to lay a common groundwork for psychology, physiology, and physics. The Gestalt movement originated in Germany, but the three protagonists for personal and political reasons resettled in the United States where the movement became an important force combatting the dominance of behaviorism. The Gestalt approach was especially fruitful in empirical psychology, but it did not fulfill the promise of turning psychology into a unified science based on a common theoretical ground.
格式塔心理学是1910年由三位心理学家马克斯·韦特海默、沃尔夫冈Köhler和库尔特·科夫卡提出的一种整体心理学方法。它被设想为反对基本或原子心理学,即心理过程由元素组成的概念,这些元素的联系产生了在思想或灵魂中体验的内容。相反,格式塔心理学认为,构型,或者在德语中,格式塔,而不是这些假设的元素,是潜在体验的主要材料。从对知觉的研究开始,格式塔方法很快被应用到心理学的其他领域。格式塔理论受到物理学场论的启发,试图为心理学、生理学和物理学奠定共同的基础。格式塔运动起源于德国,但三位主角由于个人和政治原因在美国定居,在那里该运动成为对抗行为主义主导地位的重要力量。格式塔方法在经验心理学中尤其富有成效,但它并没有实现将心理学转变为基于共同理论基础的统一科学的承诺。
{"title":"Gestalt Psychology","authors":"H. Gundlach","doi":"10.4135/9781412963947.n203","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412963947.n203","url":null,"abstract":"Gestalt psychology is an holistic approach to psychology launched in 1910 by three psychologists: Max Wertheimer, Wolfgang Köhler, and Kurt Koffka. It was conceived to oppose elementary or atomistic psychology, the conception that psychical processes consist of elements whose associations produce the contents experienced in the mind or soul. Instead, Gestalt psychology holds that configurations or, in German, Gestalten, not these hypothetical elements, are the primary material underlying experience. Beginning with research in perception, the Gestalt approach was soon applied to other fields of psychology. Gestalt theory, inspired by field theories in physics, tried to lay a common groundwork for psychology, physiology, and physics. The Gestalt movement originated in Germany, but the three protagonists for personal and political reasons resettled in the United States where the movement became an important force combatting the dominance of behaviorism. The Gestalt approach was especially fruitful in empirical psychology, but it did not fulfill the promise of turning psychology into a unified science based on a common theoretical ground.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"58 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"129996015","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
History of Evidence-Based Practice 循证实践的历史
Pub Date : 2020-02-28 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.633
S. Lilienfeld, C. Basterfield
Evidence-based therapies stemmed from the movement toward evidence-based medicine, and later, evidence-based practice (EBP) in psychology and allied fields. EBP reflects a progressive historical shift from naïve empiricism, which is based on raw and untutored observations of patient change, to systematic empiricism, which refines and hones such observations with the aid of systematic research techniques. EBP traces its roots in part to the development of methods of randomization in the early 20th century. In American psychology, EBP has traditionally been conceptualized as a three-legged stool comprising high-quality treatment outcome evidence, clinical expertise, and patient preferences and values. The research leg of the stool is typically operationalized in terms of a hierarchy of evidentiary certainty, with randomized controlled trials and meta-analyses of such trials toward the apex. The most influential operationalization of the EBP research leg is the effort to identify empirically supported treatments, which are psychotherapies that have been demonstrated to work for specific psychological conditions. Still, EBP remains scientifically controversial in many quarters, and some critics have maintained that the research base underpinning it is less compelling than claimed by its proponents.
循证疗法起源于循证医学的运动,以及后来心理学和相关领域的循证实践(EBP)。EBP反映了一个渐进的历史转变,从naïve经验主义,这是基于对病人变化的原始和未经指导的观察,到系统经验主义,这是在系统研究技术的帮助下改进和磨练这些观察。EBP的部分根源可以追溯到20世纪早期随机化方法的发展。在美国心理学中,EBP传统上被概念化为一个三脚凳,包括高质量的治疗结果证据、临床专业知识和患者的偏好和价值观。凳子的研究腿通常是根据证据确定性的层次结构来操作的,随机对照试验和对这些试验的荟萃分析接近顶点。EBP研究分支最有影响力的运作是努力确定经验支持的治疗方法,这些治疗方法是已被证明对特定心理状况有效的心理疗法。尽管如此,EBP在许多方面在科学上仍然存在争议,一些批评者坚持认为,支持它的研究基础并不像其支持者声称的那样令人信服。
{"title":"History of Evidence-Based Practice","authors":"S. Lilienfeld, C. Basterfield","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.633","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.633","url":null,"abstract":"Evidence-based therapies stemmed from the movement toward evidence-based medicine, and later, evidence-based practice (EBP) in psychology and allied fields. EBP reflects a progressive historical shift from naïve empiricism, which is based on raw and untutored observations of patient change, to systematic empiricism, which refines and hones such observations with the aid of systematic research techniques. EBP traces its roots in part to the development of methods of randomization in the early 20th century. In American psychology, EBP has traditionally been conceptualized as a three-legged stool comprising high-quality treatment outcome evidence, clinical expertise, and patient preferences and values. The research leg of the stool is typically operationalized in terms of a hierarchy of evidentiary certainty, with randomized controlled trials and meta-analyses of such trials toward the apex. The most influential operationalization of the EBP research leg is the effort to identify empirically supported treatments, which are psychotherapies that have been demonstrated to work for specific psychological conditions. Still, EBP remains scientifically controversial in many quarters, and some critics have maintained that the research base underpinning it is less compelling than claimed by its proponents.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"92 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127131657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Animal Cognition 动物认知
Pub Date : 2020-02-28 DOI: 10.4135/9781412956321.n31
Sarah Krichbaum, Adam Davila, Lucia Lazarowski, J. Katz
The contemporary field of animal cognition began over 150 years ago when Charles Darwin posed questions regarding the abilities of the animal mind. Animal cognition is a science dedicated to understanding the processes and mechanisms that allow nonhumans to think and behave. The techniques that are used and the species that are studied are diverse. The historical questions originally proposed by ethologist Nikolas Tinbergen as a framework for studying animal behavior remain at the core of the field. These questions are reviewed along with the domains and methods of animal cognition with a focus on concept learning, memory, and canine cognition. Finally, ideas on how a field rich in tradition and methodological strength should proceed in the future are presented.
当代动物认知领域始于150多年前,当时查尔斯·达尔文(Charles Darwin)提出了有关动物思维能力的问题。动物认知是一门致力于理解非人类思考和行为的过程和机制的科学。所使用的技术和研究的物种是多种多样的。动物行为学家Nikolas Tinbergen最初提出的作为研究动物行为框架的历史问题仍然是该领域的核心。这些问题与动物认知的领域和方法一起进行了回顾,重点是概念学习,记忆和犬类认知。最后,对这一具有丰富传统和方法论力量的领域今后应如何发展提出了看法。
{"title":"Animal Cognition","authors":"Sarah Krichbaum, Adam Davila, Lucia Lazarowski, J. Katz","doi":"10.4135/9781412956321.n31","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412956321.n31","url":null,"abstract":"The contemporary field of animal cognition began over 150 years ago when Charles Darwin posed questions regarding the abilities of the animal mind. Animal cognition is a science dedicated to understanding the processes and mechanisms that allow nonhumans to think and behave. The techniques that are used and the species that are studied are diverse. The historical questions originally proposed by ethologist Nikolas Tinbergen as a framework for studying animal behavior remain at the core of the field. These questions are reviewed along with the domains and methods of animal cognition with a focus on concept learning, memory, and canine cognition. Finally, ideas on how a field rich in tradition and methodological strength should proceed in the future are presented.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"10 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114483627","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cultural Competence 文化能力
Pub Date : 2020-02-28 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.567
Soon Ang, K. Ng, Thomas Rockstuhl
Cultural competence refers to an individual’s potential to function effectively in intercultural situations. The myriad conceptualizations of cultural competence can be broadly classified as intercultural traits (enduring personal characteristics that describe what a person typically does in intercultural situations); attitudes (perceptions and evaluations of other cultures); and capabilities (what a person can do to function effectively in intercultural contexts). In terms of empirical evidence, a review of existing report-based instruments (i.e., measures that involve self- or observer-perceptions of cultural competence) shows that only three instruments (Cultural Intelligence Survey, CQS; Multicultural Personality Questionnaire, MPQ; and Intercultural Adjustment Potential Scale, ICAPS) demonstrate strong psychometric properties and incremental predictive validity across cultures. Notably, the CQS has the most extensive evidence on its predictive validity. The field is also seeing an emergence of performance-based measures of cultural competence in the form of situation judgment tests. Finally, there is considerable research on interventions to grow cultural competence and intelligence in individuals. Meta-analyses and systematic reviews generally concluded that training enhances the development of cultural competence and intercultural effectiveness. Effect sizes, however, vary depending on training and trainee characteristics. The field of cultural competence is at an exciting nexus of globalization, increasing diversification within nations, and technological advancements. We suggest that future research should (1) extend our conceptualization of cultural competence to include managing vertical differences rooted in power and status disparity; (2) expand our measurement from psychometric approaches to the use of multimodal analytics; and (3) expand our criterion space of cultural learning.
文化能力是指个人在跨文化环境中有效发挥作用的潜力。文化能力的无数概念可以大致归类为跨文化特征(描述一个人在跨文化情境中典型行为的持久的个人特征);态度(对其他文化的看法和评价);以及能力(一个人在跨文化背景下能做些什么来有效地工作)。就经验证据而言,对现有基于报告的工具(即涉及自我或观察者对文化能力的感知的措施)的审查表明,只有三种工具(文化智力调查,CQS;多元文化人格问卷;和跨文化适应潜力量表(ICAPS)显示出强大的心理测量特性和跨文化的增量预测效度。值得注意的是,CQS在预测有效性方面拥有最广泛的证据。该领域还出现了以情境判断测试形式的基于表现的文化能力衡量标准。最后,关于提高个人文化能力和智力的干预措施也有相当多的研究。荟萃分析和系统综述普遍认为,培训促进了文化能力和跨文化有效性的发展。然而,效应大小取决于培训和受训者的特征。文化能力领域正处于全球化、国家内部日益多样化和技术进步的激动人心的联系之中。我们建议未来的研究应该:(1)扩展我们对文化能力的概念,包括管理植根于权力和地位差异的垂直差异;(2)将我们的测量方法从心理测量法扩展到使用多模态分析;(3)拓展我们的文化学习规范空间。
{"title":"Cultural Competence","authors":"Soon Ang, K. Ng, Thomas Rockstuhl","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.567","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.567","url":null,"abstract":"Cultural competence refers to an individual’s potential to function effectively in intercultural situations. The myriad conceptualizations of cultural competence can be broadly classified as intercultural traits (enduring personal characteristics that describe what a person typically does in intercultural situations); attitudes (perceptions and evaluations of other cultures); and capabilities (what a person can do to function effectively in intercultural contexts). In terms of empirical evidence, a review of existing report-based instruments (i.e., measures that involve self- or observer-perceptions of cultural competence) shows that only three instruments (Cultural Intelligence Survey, CQS; Multicultural Personality Questionnaire, MPQ; and Intercultural Adjustment Potential Scale, ICAPS) demonstrate strong psychometric properties and incremental predictive validity across cultures. Notably, the CQS has the most extensive evidence on its predictive validity. The field is also seeing an emergence of performance-based measures of cultural competence in the form of situation judgment tests. Finally, there is considerable research on interventions to grow cultural competence and intelligence in individuals. Meta-analyses and systematic reviews generally concluded that training enhances the development of cultural competence and intercultural effectiveness. Effect sizes, however, vary depending on training and trainee characteristics. The field of cultural competence is at an exciting nexus of globalization, increasing diversification within nations, and technological advancements. We suggest that future research should (1) extend our conceptualization of cultural competence to include managing vertical differences rooted in power and status disparity; (2) expand our measurement from psychometric approaches to the use of multimodal analytics; and (3) expand our criterion space of cultural learning.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"70 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"129936349","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Transformative-Activist and Social Justice Approaches to the History of Psychology 心理学史的变革-活动家和社会正义方法
Pub Date : 2020-02-28 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.466
A. Stetsenko
The history of psychology is characterized by unparalleled complexity of its methodology and uniquely ambiguous subject matter closely entangled with issues of power, social justice, and ethics. This complexity requires inordinate levels of reflexivity and conceptual sophistication. In effect, a historian of psychology needs to explicate no less than one’s worldview—a broad position as to how people are situated in the world, relate to, change, and get to know it, and how knowledge develops through time—all coupled with one’s broad sociopolitical ethos. Traditional histories of psychology have operated with an astonishing lack of reflection about these issues. One of many deplorable results is that psychology still grapples with its racist and sexist legacies and lacks awareness of social injustices in existence today. The recently emerging approaches have begun to remedy this situation by focusing on situated practices of knowledge production. This article addresses how human agency can be integrated into these approaches, while focusing on knowledge production as not only situated in context but also, and critically, as a world-forming and history-making process. In tackling the shortcomings of relational approaches including social constructionism, the transformative activist stance approach draws on Marxist philosophy and epistemology—infused with insights from Vygotsky’s psychology and other critical theories of resistance. The core point is that knowledge is achieved in and through collaborative community practices realized by individually unique contributions as these come to embody and enact, in an inseparable blend, both cultural-historical contexts and unique commitments and agency of community members. The acts of being-doing-knowing are non-neutral, transformative processes that produce the world, its history and also people themselves, all realized in the process of taking up the world, rather than passively copying it or coping with it. And since reality is in-the-making by people themselves, knowing is about creating the world and knowing it in the very act of bringing about transformative and creative change. Thus, the historicity and situativity of knowledge are ascertained alongside a focus on its ineluctable fusion with an activist, future-oriented, political-ethical stance. Therefore, the critical challenge for the history of psychology is to understand producers of knowledge in their role of actors in the drama of life (rather than only of ideas), that is, as agents of history- and world-making, while also engaging in self-reflection on the historians’ own role in these processes, in order to practice history in responsive and responsible, that is, activist ways.
心理学的历史以其方法论的空前复杂性和与权力、社会正义和伦理问题紧密纠缠在一起的独特的模棱两可的主题为特征。这种复杂性需要高度的反身性和概念复杂性。实际上,一个心理学历史学家需要解释的不仅仅是自己的世界观——一个关于人们如何在世界上生存、与世界联系、改变和了解世界,以及知识如何随着时间发展的广泛立场——所有这些都与一个人广泛的社会政治气质相结合。传统的心理学史对这些问题的反思少得惊人。许多令人遗憾的结果之一是,心理学仍然在努力解决其种族主义和性别歧视的遗留问题,并且缺乏对当今存在的社会不公正的认识。最近出现的方法已经开始通过关注知识生产的情境实践来纠正这种情况。本文将讨论如何将人的能动性整合到这些方法中,同时将重点放在知识生产上,不仅将其置于环境中,而且批判性地将其作为世界形成和历史创造的过程。在解决包括社会建构主义在内的关系方法的缺陷时,变革的行动主义立场方法借鉴了马克思主义哲学和认识论,并注入了维果茨基心理学和其他抵抗批判理论的见解。核心观点是,知识是通过协作的社区实践获得的,这些实践是通过个人独特的贡献实现的,因为这些贡献在不可分割的混合中体现和制定,包括文化历史背景和社区成员的独特承诺和代理。“存在-行动-认知”的行为是非中性的、变革的过程,它产生了世界、世界的历史和人自己,所有这些都是在接受世界的过程中实现的,而不是被动地复制世界或应对世界。因为现实是由人们自己创造的,所以了解就是创造世界,并且在带来变革和创造性变化的过程中了解它。因此,知识的历史性和情境性被确定,同时关注其与积极的、面向未来的、政治伦理立场的不可避免的融合。因此,心理学史的关键挑战是理解知识的生产者在生活戏剧中的角色(而不仅仅是思想),也就是说,作为历史和世界创造的代理人,同时也要对历史学家自己在这些过程中的角色进行自我反思,以便以回应和负责任的方式实践历史,也就是说,积极的方式。
{"title":"Transformative-Activist and Social Justice Approaches to the History of Psychology","authors":"A. Stetsenko","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.466","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.466","url":null,"abstract":"The history of psychology is characterized by unparalleled complexity of its methodology and uniquely ambiguous subject matter closely entangled with issues of power, social justice, and ethics. This complexity requires inordinate levels of reflexivity and conceptual sophistication. In effect, a historian of psychology needs to explicate no less than one’s worldview—a broad position as to how people are situated in the world, relate to, change, and get to know it, and how knowledge develops through time—all coupled with one’s broad sociopolitical ethos. Traditional histories of psychology have operated with an astonishing lack of reflection about these issues. One of many deplorable results is that psychology still grapples with its racist and sexist legacies and lacks awareness of social injustices in existence today. The recently emerging approaches have begun to remedy this situation by focusing on situated practices of knowledge production. This article addresses how human agency can be integrated into these approaches, while focusing on knowledge production as not only situated in context but also, and critically, as a world-forming and history-making process. In tackling the shortcomings of relational approaches including social constructionism, the transformative activist stance approach draws on Marxist philosophy and epistemology—infused with insights from Vygotsky’s psychology and other critical theories of resistance. The core point is that knowledge is achieved in and through collaborative community practices realized by individually unique contributions as these come to embody and enact, in an inseparable blend, both cultural-historical contexts and unique commitments and agency of community members. The acts of being-doing-knowing are non-neutral, transformative processes that produce the world, its history and also people themselves, all realized in the process of taking up the world, rather than passively copying it or coping with it. And since reality is in-the-making by people themselves, knowing is about creating the world and knowing it in the very act of bringing about transformative and creative change. Thus, the historicity and situativity of knowledge are ascertained alongside a focus on its ineluctable fusion with an activist, future-oriented, political-ethical stance. Therefore, the critical challenge for the history of psychology is to understand producers of knowledge in their role of actors in the drama of life (rather than only of ideas), that is, as agents of history- and world-making, while also engaging in self-reflection on the historians’ own role in these processes, in order to practice history in responsive and responsible, that is, activist ways.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"116 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"115547068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Animal Learning and Cognition 动物学习与认知
Pub Date : 2020-01-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.644
M. Beran
Comparative psychology is the study of behavior and cognition across species. In recent decades, much of this research has focused on cognitive capacities that are well studied in humans. This approach provides comparative perspectives on the evolution of these cognitive capacities. Although in many areas humans shows distinct aspects of various cognitive processes, it is clear that for most major topics in human cognition, important and illustrative data are available from studies with other animals. Moreover, these areas of investigation increasingly show continuities between the behavior of other species and human behavior. Several of these cognitive processes, including concept and category learning, numerical cognition, memory, mental time travel and prospective cognition, metacognition, and language learning, highlight these continuities and demonstrate the richness of mental lives in other animals. Nonhuman animals can discriminate between categories of perceptual and conceptual classes, they can form concepts, and they can use those concepts to guide decision making and choice behavior. Other species can engage in rudimentary numerical cognition, and more importantly share with humans certain core quantitative abilities for the approximate representation of magnitude and number. Nonhuman animals share many phenomena of memory that are well-recognized in humans, and in some cases may even share the capacity to mentally re-experience the past and to anticipate and plan for the future. In some cases, some species may even reflect on their own knowledge states, memory accessibility, and perceptual acuity as they make metacognitive judgments. And, studies of animal communication provided the basis for intensive assessments of language-like behavior in certain species. Taken together, these results argue much more for continuity than discontinuity. This should not be seen as a challenge to the uniqueness of human minds, but rather as a way to better understand how we became the species we are through the process of evolution.
比较心理学是跨物种行为和认知的研究。近几十年来,这方面的研究大多集中在人类的认知能力上,而人类的认知能力已经得到了很好的研究。这种方法为这些认知能力的进化提供了比较的视角。尽管人类在许多领域表现出不同的认知过程,但很明显,对于人类认知的大多数主要主题,可以从其他动物的研究中获得重要的说明性数据。此外,这些研究领域越来越多地显示出其他物种的行为与人类行为之间的连续性。其中一些认知过程,包括概念和类别学习、数字认知、记忆、心理时间旅行和前瞻性认知、元认知和语言学习,突出了这些连续性,并展示了其他动物心理生活的丰富性。非人类动物可以区分知觉类和概念类,它们可以形成概念,它们可以使用这些概念来指导决策和选择行为。其他物种可以进行基本的数字认知,更重要的是与人类共享某些核心的量化能力,以近似表示大小和数字。非人类动物具有许多人类熟知的记忆现象,在某些情况下,它们甚至具有在精神上重新体验过去、预测和计划未来的能力。在某些情况下,一些物种甚至会在做出元认知判断时反思自己的知识状态、记忆可及性和感知敏锐度。而且,对动物交流的研究为深入评估某些物种的类语言行为提供了基础。综上所述,这些结果更多地支持连续性而非非连续性。这不应该被看作是对人类思维独特性的挑战,而是一种更好地理解我们是如何通过进化过程成为我们这个物种的方式。
{"title":"Animal Learning and Cognition","authors":"M. Beran","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.644","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.644","url":null,"abstract":"Comparative psychology is the study of behavior and cognition across species. In recent decades, much of this research has focused on cognitive capacities that are well studied in humans. This approach provides comparative perspectives on the evolution of these cognitive capacities. Although in many areas humans shows distinct aspects of various cognitive processes, it is clear that for most major topics in human cognition, important and illustrative data are available from studies with other animals. Moreover, these areas of investigation increasingly show continuities between the behavior of other species and human behavior. Several of these cognitive processes, including concept and category learning, numerical cognition, memory, mental time travel and prospective cognition, metacognition, and language learning, highlight these continuities and demonstrate the richness of mental lives in other animals. Nonhuman animals can discriminate between categories of perceptual and conceptual classes, they can form concepts, and they can use those concepts to guide decision making and choice behavior. Other species can engage in rudimentary numerical cognition, and more importantly share with humans certain core quantitative abilities for the approximate representation of magnitude and number. Nonhuman animals share many phenomena of memory that are well-recognized in humans, and in some cases may even share the capacity to mentally re-experience the past and to anticipate and plan for the future. In some cases, some species may even reflect on their own knowledge states, memory accessibility, and perceptual acuity as they make metacognitive judgments. And, studies of animal communication provided the basis for intensive assessments of language-like behavior in certain species. Taken together, these results argue much more for continuity than discontinuity. This should not be seen as a challenge to the uniqueness of human minds, but rather as a way to better understand how we became the species we are through the process of evolution.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"131345750","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Development 社会发展
Pub Date : 2020-01-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.520
R. Parke
Social development is the sub area of developmental psychology that concerns the description of children’s development of relationships with others, their understanding of the meaning of their relationships with others, and their understanding of others’ behaviors, attitudes, and intentions. The examination of the social, emotional, biological, and cognitive processes that account for these developmental changes in social development are of interest as well. The historical shifts in the understanding of social development from Darwin to the present can be traced by an examination of the major theoretical and methodological advances that have characterized this area of inquiry. The history of social development is divided into five time periods—the beginning years (1880–1915), a period of conceptual clashes (1915–1940), a period of expansion (1940–1960), an era that saw the rise of contemporary themes (1960–1985), and the current period (from 1985 to 2019). Finally, future directions and unresolved issues are noted.
社会发展是发展心理学的一个分支,它关注儿童与他人关系的发展,他们对与他人关系意义的理解,以及他们对他人行为、态度和意图的理解。对社会、情感、生物和认知过程的研究也引起了人们的兴趣,这些过程解释了社会发展中的这些发展变化。从达尔文到现在,对社会发展的理解的历史转变可以通过对这一研究领域的主要理论和方法进步的研究来追溯。社会发展史分为五个时期:初期(1880-1915)、观念冲突时期(1915-1940)、扩张时期(1940-1960)、当代主题兴起的时期(1960-1985)和当前时期(1985 - 2019)。最后,指出了未来的发展方向和尚未解决的问题。
{"title":"Social Development","authors":"R. Parke","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.520","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.520","url":null,"abstract":"Social development is the sub area of developmental psychology that concerns the description of children’s development of relationships with others, their understanding of the meaning of their relationships with others, and their understanding of others’ behaviors, attitudes, and intentions. The examination of the social, emotional, biological, and cognitive processes that account for these developmental changes in social development are of interest as well. The historical shifts in the understanding of social development from Darwin to the present can be traced by an examination of the major theoretical and methodological advances that have characterized this area of inquiry. The history of social development is divided into five time periods—the beginning years (1880–1915), a period of conceptual clashes (1915–1940), a period of expansion (1940–1960), an era that saw the rise of contemporary themes (1960–1985), and the current period (from 1985 to 2019). Finally, future directions and unresolved issues are noted.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"71 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"126043887","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Foundations of Philosophical Functionalism 哲学功能主义基础
Pub Date : 2020-01-30 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.493
Lawrence A. Shapiro
Philosophical functionalism, as distinct from the psychological school of functionalism that enjoyed popularity around the turn of the 20th century, is a theory about the nature of mental states. That is, functionalism offers an account of which conditions must be satisfied for something to count as a belief, or a desire, or a pain, or an itch, or a fear, or a memory. Functionalism is thus a metaphysical doctrine about mental states, that is, a doctrine concerning what makes something a mental state. “Metaphysical,” in this context, should not be taken to suggest anything mysterious. Chemistry is a metaphysical doctrine in just the same sense as functionalism: it is a theory that offers an account of which conditions must be satisfied for something to count as, say, a pure chemical substance rather than a mixture. As philosophical theories go, functionalism has been fantastically successful. Its contemporary form traces to seminal work that H. Putnam initiated in the 1960s, and it remains in early 21st century the most widely accepted theory of the nature of mental states among philosophers in the Anglo tradition. According to functionalism, the conditions necessary and sufficient for something to be a mental state are specified in terms of functional role. Functionalists have disagreed about the correct basis on which functional descriptions of mental states should rest, with the result that functionalism is better conceived as a family of closely related theories about the nature of mental states rather than a single uniform view. Briefly, the idea of functional role can be usefully illustrated by consideration of an artifact, such as a corkscrew, the nature of which is defined in terms of the function of removing corks. What it is to be a corkscrew is to perform this functional role. Likewise, the functionalist claims, what it is to be a mental state is to perform the functional role characteristic of a belief, or a desire, or a pain, and so on.
哲学功能主义不同于20世纪初流行的功能主义心理学派,它是一种关于心理状态本质的理论。也就是说,功能主义提供了一种解释,说明某些东西必须满足哪些条件才能算作信仰、欲望、痛苦、瘙痒、恐惧或记忆。因此,功能主义是一种关于精神状态的形而上学学说,也就是说,是一种关于什么使某物成为精神状态的学说。在这种情况下,“形而上学”不应该被认为是暗示任何神秘的东西。化学和功能主义一样,是一种形而上学的学说。它是一种理论,它提供了一种说明,比如说,某种东西要算作纯化学物质而不是混合物,必须满足哪些条件。就哲学理论而言,功能主义取得了惊人的成功。它的当代形式可以追溯到H. Putnam在20世纪60年代发起的开创性工作,并且在21世纪初仍然是盎格鲁传统哲学家中最广泛接受的精神状态本质理论。功能主义认为,某种事物成为精神状态的必要和充分条件是通过功能角色来规定的。功能主义者对精神状态的功能描述应该建立在什么正确的基础上存在分歧,其结果是,功能主义最好被看作是关于精神状态本质的一系列密切相关的理论,而不是一个单一的统一观点。简单地说,功能性角色的概念可以通过考虑一个人工制品来有效地说明,比如开瓶器,它的性质是根据去除软木塞的功能来定义的。开瓶器就是要发挥这个功能性的作用。同样地,功能主义者声称,精神状态的定义是,履行一种信仰、一种欲望、一种痛苦等的功能性角色。
{"title":"Foundations of Philosophical Functionalism","authors":"Lawrence A. Shapiro","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.493","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.493","url":null,"abstract":"Philosophical functionalism, as distinct from the psychological school of functionalism that enjoyed popularity around the turn of the 20th century, is a theory about the nature of mental states. That is, functionalism offers an account of which conditions must be satisfied for something to count as a belief, or a desire, or a pain, or an itch, or a fear, or a memory. Functionalism is thus a metaphysical doctrine about mental states, that is, a doctrine concerning what makes something a mental state. “Metaphysical,” in this context, should not be taken to suggest anything mysterious. Chemistry is a metaphysical doctrine in just the same sense as functionalism: it is a theory that offers an account of which conditions must be satisfied for something to count as, say, a pure chemical substance rather than a mixture. As philosophical theories go, functionalism has been fantastically successful. Its contemporary form traces to seminal work that H. Putnam initiated in the 1960s, and it remains in early 21st century the most widely accepted theory of the nature of mental states among philosophers in the Anglo tradition.\u0000 According to functionalism, the conditions necessary and sufficient for something to be a mental state are specified in terms of functional role. Functionalists have disagreed about the correct basis on which functional descriptions of mental states should rest, with the result that functionalism is better conceived as a family of closely related theories about the nature of mental states rather than a single uniform view. Briefly, the idea of functional role can be usefully illustrated by consideration of an artifact, such as a corkscrew, the nature of which is defined in terms of the function of removing corks. What it is to be a corkscrew is to perform this functional role. Likewise, the functionalist claims, what it is to be a mental state is to perform the functional role characteristic of a belief, or a desire, or a pain, and so on.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"25 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"122188856","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thought Suppression 认为抑制
Pub Date : 2020-01-30 DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-08-044714-8.x5028-4
C. Purdon
{"title":"Thought Suppression","authors":"C. Purdon","doi":"10.1016/b978-0-08-044714-8.x5028-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-08-044714-8.x5028-4","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"36 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127712095","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychological Aspects of Cancer Screening 癌症筛检的心理层面
Pub Date : 2019-11-22 DOI: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.130
C. Wagner, W. Verstraete, S. Stoffel
Cancer screening aims to detect cancer before the appearance of symptoms. Applying a proactive and systematic approach, cancer screening programs invite every person in the target population automatically. Many countries have established guidelines that define criteria and principles on whether to implement screening programs for specific conditions. Despite the universal coverage of these programs, inequalities have been observed in their uptake based on various sociodemographic factors: gender, age, ethnicity, socioeconomic status (SES), educational level, and marital status. Behavioral science provides key performance indicators of these programs. Psychological factors such as perceived benefits (e.g., ability of the program to diagnose early or even prevent cancer) and barriers (e.g., opportunity costs relating to test attendance or completion), as well as people’s cancer and screening-related beliefs and perceptions of their own susceptibility to cancer, play a crucial role in cancer screening uptake. Furthermore, there is increasing awareness among professional bodies for the need to balance the public health benefits against individual costs, including financial and opportunity costs associated with participation and potential longer-term harms, such as receiving a cancer diagnosis that would never have caused any symptoms or problems). These recent developments have led to stronger emphasis on monitoring patient-reported experiences and ensuring that participation is based on informed choice. In addition, some of these issues have also been addressed by more fundamental changes to the screening paradigm such as more personalized approaches (using additional genetic and epigenetic information) to establishing eligibility criteria. The acceptability of using this information and its implication to offer more or less intensive screening and developing effective ways to understand the ability of the program to communicate this information are key challenges for the clinical, research and policy making community.
癌症筛查的目的是在症状出现之前发现癌症。采用主动和系统的方法,癌症筛查项目自动邀请目标人群中的每一个人。许多国家都制定了指导方针,确定了是否针对特定情况实施筛查项目的标准和原则。尽管这些计划的覆盖范围很广,但根据不同的社会人口因素,如性别、年龄、种族、社会经济地位(SES)、教育水平和婚姻状况,在接受这些计划方面存在不平等现象。行为科学为这些项目提供了关键的绩效指标。心理因素,如感知到的好处(例如,项目早期诊断甚至预防癌症的能力)和障碍(例如,与参加或完成测试有关的机会成本),以及人们对癌症和筛查相关的信念和对自己对癌症易感性的认识,在癌症筛查的接受中起着至关重要的作用。此外,专业机构越来越认识到需要平衡公共健康利益与个人成本(包括与参与相关的财务和机会成本以及潜在的长期危害,例如接受绝不会引起任何症状或问题的癌症诊断)之间的关系。这些最近的发展使人们更加重视监测患者报告的经验,并确保参与是基于知情选择。此外,其中一些问题也已经通过对筛选范例进行更根本的改变来解决,例如更个性化的方法(使用额外的遗传和表观遗传信息)来建立资格标准。使用这些信息的可接受性及其含义,提供或多或少的密集筛选,并开发有效的方法来了解项目沟通这些信息的能力,是临床,研究和政策制定社区的关键挑战。
{"title":"Psychological Aspects of Cancer Screening","authors":"C. Wagner, W. Verstraete, S. Stoffel","doi":"10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.130","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.130","url":null,"abstract":"Cancer screening aims to detect cancer before the appearance of symptoms. Applying a proactive and systematic approach, cancer screening programs invite every person in the target population automatically. Many countries have established guidelines that define criteria and principles on whether to implement screening programs for specific conditions. Despite the universal coverage of these programs, inequalities have been observed in their uptake based on various sociodemographic factors: gender, age, ethnicity, socioeconomic status (SES), educational level, and marital status. Behavioral science provides key performance indicators of these programs. Psychological factors such as perceived benefits (e.g., ability of the program to diagnose early or even prevent cancer) and barriers (e.g., opportunity costs relating to test attendance or completion), as well as people’s cancer and screening-related beliefs and perceptions of their own susceptibility to cancer, play a crucial role in cancer screening uptake. Furthermore, there is increasing awareness among professional bodies for the need to balance the public health benefits against individual costs, including financial and opportunity costs associated with participation and potential longer-term harms, such as receiving a cancer diagnosis that would never have caused any symptoms or problems). These recent developments have led to stronger emphasis on monitoring patient-reported experiences and ensuring that participation is based on informed choice. In addition, some of these issues have also been addressed by more fundamental changes to the screening paradigm such as more personalized approaches (using additional genetic and epigenetic information) to establishing eligibility criteria. The acceptability of using this information and its implication to offer more or less intensive screening and developing effective ways to understand the ability of the program to communicate this information are key challenges for the clinical, research and policy making community.","PeriodicalId":339030,"journal":{"name":"Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology","volume":"66 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-11-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"121750060","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1