首页 > 最新文献

Evolutionary Human Sciences最新文献

英文 中文
The cultural evolution of teaching. 教学的文化演变。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-12 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.14
Eva Brandl, Ruth Mace, Cecilia Heyes

Teaching is an important process of cultural transmission. Some have argued that human teaching is a cognitive instinct - a form of 'natural cognition' centred on mindreading, shaped by genetic evolution for the education of juveniles, and with a normative developmental trajectory driven by the unfolding of a genetically inherited predisposition to teach. Here, we argue instead that human teaching is a culturally evolved trait that exhibits characteristics of a cognitive gadget. Children learn to teach by participating in teaching interactions with socialising agents, which shape their own teaching practices. This process hijacks psychological mechanisms involved in prosociality and a range of domain-general cognitive abilities, such as reinforcement learning and executive function, but not a suite of cognitive adaptations specifically for teaching. Four lines of evidence converge on this hypothesis. The first, based on psychological experiments in industrialised societies, indicates that domain-general cognitive processes are important for teaching. The second and third lines, based on naturalistic and experimental research in small-scale societies, indicate marked cross-cultural variation in mature teaching practice and in the ontogeny of teaching among children. The fourth line indicates that teaching has been subject to cumulative cultural evolution, i.e. the gradual accumulation of functional changes across generations.

教学是一个重要的文化传承过程。有人认为,人类教学是一种认知本能--一种以读心术为核心的 "自然认知 "形式,由基因进化形成,用于青少年教育,其规范发展轨迹由基因遗传的教学倾向驱动。在这里,我们反过来认为,人类的教学是一种文化进化的特征,表现出认知小工具的特点。儿童通过参与与社会化主体的教学互动来学习教学,社会化主体塑造了他们自己的教学实践。这一过程劫持了涉及亲社会性的心理机制和一系列领域通用的认知能力,如强化学习和执行功能,但并没有劫持一套专门用于教学的认知适应能力。这一假设有四种证据。第一个证据基于工业化社会的心理实验,表明领域一般认知过程对教学非常重要。第二和第三条线索基于小规模社会的自然和实验研究,表明成熟的教学实践和儿童教学的本体存在明显的跨文化差异。第四条线索表明,教学经历了累积性的文化演变,即世代相传的功能变化的逐步积累。
{"title":"The cultural evolution of teaching.","authors":"Eva Brandl, Ruth Mace, Cecilia Heyes","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2023.14","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2023.14","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Teaching is an important process of cultural transmission. Some have argued that human teaching is a cognitive instinct - a form of 'natural cognition' centred on mindreading, shaped by genetic evolution for the education of juveniles, and with a normative developmental trajectory driven by the unfolding of a genetically inherited predisposition to teach. Here, we argue instead that human teaching is a culturally evolved trait that exhibits characteristics of a cognitive gadget. Children learn to teach by participating in teaching interactions with socialising agents, which shape their own teaching practices. This process hijacks psychological mechanisms involved in prosociality and a range of domain-general cognitive abilities, such as reinforcement learning and executive function, but not a suite of cognitive adaptations specifically for teaching. Four lines of evidence converge on this hypothesis. The first, based on psychological experiments in industrialised societies, indicates that domain-general cognitive processes are important for teaching. The second and third lines, based on naturalistic and experimental research in small-scale societies, indicate marked cross-cultural variation in mature teaching practice and in the ontogeny of teaching among children. The fourth line indicates that teaching has been subject to cumulative cultural evolution, i.e. the gradual accumulation of functional changes across generations.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e14"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426124/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10395064","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The shape of lipsmacking: socio-emotional regulation in bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus). 咂嘴的形状:胡须卷尾猴(Sapajus libidinosus)的社会情绪调节。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-05-12 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.10
Natalia Albuquerque, Carine Savalli, Marina Belli, Ana Clara Varella, Beatriz Felício, Juliana França, Patrícia Izar

Capuchin monkeys have rich social relationships and from very young ages they participate in complex interactions with members of their group. Lipsmacking behaviour, which involves at least two individuals in socially mediated interactions, may tell about processes that maintain, accentuate or attenuate emotional exchanges in monkeys. Lipsmacking is a facial expression associated with the establishment and maintenance of affiliative interactions, following under the 'emotional regulation' umbrella, which accounts for the ability to manage behavioural responses. We investigated behaviours related to the emitter and to the receiver (infant) of lipsmacking to answer the question of how lipsmacking occurs. In capuchin monkeys, lipsmacking has been previously understood solely as a face-to-face interaction. Our data show that emitters are engaged with infants, looking longer towards their face and seeking eye contact during the display. However, receivers spend most of the time looking away from the emitter and stay in no contact for nearly half of the time. From naturalistic observations of wild infant capuchin monkeys from Brazil we found that lipsmacking is not restricted to mutual gaze, meaning there are other mechanisms in place than previously known. Our results open paths to new insights about the evolution of socio-emotional displays in primates.

卷尾猴具有丰富的社会关系,从很小的时候起,它们就与群体成员进行复杂的互动。咂嘴行为至少涉及两个个体的社会中介性互动,它可能揭示了猴子维持、加强或削弱情感交流的过程。咂嘴是一种与建立和维持附属性互动有关的面部表情,属于 "情绪调节 "的范畴,它说明了管理行为反应的能力。我们研究了与咂嘴的发出者和接收者(婴儿)有关的行为,以回答咂嘴是如何发生的这一问题。在卷尾猴中,咂嘴以前被理解为一种面对面的互动。我们的数据显示,在咂嘴过程中,发出者会与婴幼儿互动,看向婴幼儿脸部的时间更长,并寻求目光接触。然而,接受者的大部分时间都远离发出者,近一半的时间没有与发出者接触。通过对巴西野生卷尾猴婴幼儿的自然观察,我们发现咂嘴并不局限于相互凝视,这意味着还存在其他机制。我们的研究结果为深入了解灵长类动物社会情感表现的进化开辟了新的道路。
{"title":"The shape of lipsmacking: socio-emotional regulation in bearded capuchin monkeys (<i>Sapajus libidinosus</i>).","authors":"Natalia Albuquerque, Carine Savalli, Marina Belli, Ana Clara Varella, Beatriz Felício, Juliana França, Patrícia Izar","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2023.10","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2023.10","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Capuchin monkeys have rich social relationships and from very young ages they participate in complex interactions with members of their group. Lipsmacking behaviour, which involves at least two individuals in socially mediated interactions, may tell about processes that maintain, accentuate or attenuate emotional exchanges in monkeys. Lipsmacking is a facial expression associated with the establishment and maintenance of affiliative interactions, following under the 'emotional regulation' umbrella, which accounts for the ability to manage behavioural responses. We investigated behaviours related to the emitter and to the receiver (infant) of lipsmacking to answer the question of how lipsmacking occurs. In capuchin monkeys, lipsmacking has been previously understood solely as a face-to-face interaction. Our data show that emitters are engaged with infants, looking longer towards their face and seeking eye contact during the display. However, receivers spend most of the time looking away from the emitter and stay in no contact for nearly half of the time. From naturalistic observations of wild infant capuchin monkeys from Brazil we found that lipsmacking is not restricted to mutual gaze, meaning there are other mechanisms in place than previously known. Our results open paths to new insights about the evolution of socio-emotional displays in primates.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e16"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-05-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426065/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10019108","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gendered conflict in the human family. 人类家庭中的性别冲突。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-24 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.8
David W Lawson, Sarah Alami, Oluwaseyi Dolapo Somefun

Sexual conflict is a thriving area of animal behaviour research. Yet parallel research in the evolutionary human sciences remains underdeveloped and has become mired by controversy. In this special collection, we aim to invigorate the study of fitness-relevant conflicts between women and men, advocating for three synergistic research priorities. First, we argue that a commitment to diversity is required to innovate the field, achieve ethical research practice, and foster fruitful dialogue with neighbouring social sciences. Accordingly, we have prioritised issues of diversity as editors, aiming to stimulate new connections and perspectives. Second, we call for greater recognition that human sex/gender roles and accompanying conflict behaviours are both subject to natural selection and culturally determined. This motivates our shift in terminology from sexual to gendered conflict when addressing human behaviour, countering stubborn tendencies to essentialise differences between women and men and directing attention to the role of cultural practices, normative sanctions and social learning in structuring conflict battlegrounds. Finally, we draw attention to contemporary policy concerns, including the wellbeing consequences of marriage practices and the gendered implications of market integration. Focus on these themes, combined with attendance to the dangers of ethnocentrism, promises to inform culturally sensitive interventions promoting gender equality worldwide.

性冲突是动物行为研究的一个蓬勃发展的领域。然而,人类进化科学领域的平行研究仍然发展不足,并陷入了争议之中。在这本特辑中,我们旨在为男女之间与适性相关的冲突研究注入活力,倡导三个协同研究重点。首先,我们认为,要想在这一领域有所创新,实现合乎道德的研究实践,并促进与邻近社会科学的富有成效的对话,就必须致力于多元化。因此,我们将多样性问题作为编辑的优先事项,旨在激发新的联系和观点。其次,我们呼吁更多的人认识到,人类的性/性别角色和相应的冲突行为既是自然选择的结果,也是文化决定的。这促使我们在讨论人类行为时,将术语从 "性冲突 "转变为 "性别冲突",抵制将男女差异本质化的顽固倾向,并引导人们关注文化习俗、规范性制裁和社会学习在构建冲突战场中的作用。最后,我们提请大家注意当代的政策问题,包括婚姻习俗的福祉后果和市场一体化的性别影响。对这些主题的关注,再加上对种族中心主义危险的关注,有望为促进全球性别平等的文化敏感干预措施提供信息。
{"title":"Gendered conflict in the human family.","authors":"David W Lawson, Sarah Alami, Oluwaseyi Dolapo Somefun","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2023.8","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2023.8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sexual conflict is a thriving area of animal behaviour research. Yet parallel research in the evolutionary human sciences remains underdeveloped and has become mired by controversy. In this special collection, we aim to invigorate the study of fitness-relevant conflicts between women and men, advocating for three synergistic research priorities. First, we argue that a commitment to diversity is required to innovate the field, achieve ethical research practice, and foster fruitful dialogue with neighbouring social sciences. Accordingly, we have prioritised issues of diversity as editors, aiming to stimulate new connections and perspectives. Second, we call for greater recognition that human sex/gender roles and accompanying conflict behaviours are both subject to natural selection and culturally determined. This motivates our shift in terminology from sexual to gendered conflict when addressing human behaviour, countering stubborn tendencies to essentialise differences between women and men and directing attention to the role of cultural practices, normative sanctions and social learning in structuring conflict battlegrounds. Finally, we draw attention to contemporary policy concerns, including the wellbeing consequences of marriage practices and the gendered implications of market integration. Focus on these themes, combined with attendance to the dangers of ethnocentrism, promises to inform culturally sensitive interventions promoting gender equality worldwide.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e12"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426121/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10019109","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Norm violations and punishments across human societies. 人类社会中违反规范的行为和惩罚。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-13 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.7
Zachary H Garfield, Erik J Ringen, William Buckner, Dithapelo Medupe, Richard W Wrangham, Luke Glowacki

Punishments for norm violations are hypothesised to be a crucial component of the maintenance of cooperation in humans but are rarely studied from a comparative perspective. We investigated the degree to which punishment systems were correlated with socioecology and cultural history. We took data from the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample database and coded ethnographic documents from a sample of 131 largely non-industrial societies. We recorded whether punishment for norm violations concerned adultery, religion, food, rape or war cowardice and whether sanctions were reputational, physical, material or execution. We used Bayesian phylogenetic regression modelling to test for culture-level covariation. We found little evidence of phylogenetic signals in evidence for punishment types, suggesting that punishment systems change relatively quickly over cultural evolutionary history. We found evidence that reputational punishment was associated with egalitarianism and the absence of food storage; material punishment was associated with the presence of food storage; physical punishment was moderately associated with greater dependence on hunting; and execution punishment was moderately associated with social stratification. Taken together, our results suggest that the role and kind of punishment vary both by the severity of the norm violation, but also by the specific socio-economic system of the society.

对违反规范行为的惩罚被认为是维持人类合作的一个重要组成部分,但很少有人从比较的角度对其进行研究。我们研究了惩罚制度与社会生态学和文化历史的相关程度。我们从标准跨文化样本数据库中获取数据,并对 131 个非工业社会样本中的人种学文件进行编码。我们记录了对违反规范的惩罚是否涉及通奸、宗教、食物、强奸或战争懦夫,以及制裁方式是名誉制裁、身体制裁、物质制裁还是处决。我们使用贝叶斯系统发育回归模型来检验文化层面的共变。我们在惩罚类型的证据中几乎没有发现系统发育信号,这表明惩罚制度在文化进化史上的变化相对较快。我们发现有证据表明,名誉惩罚与平等主义和没有食物储存有关;物质惩罚与有食物储存有关;肉体惩罚与更多地依赖狩猎有一定关系;执行惩罚与社会分层有一定关系。总之,我们的研究结果表明,惩罚的作用和种类既取决于违反规范的严重程度,也取决于社会的具体社会经济制度。
{"title":"Norm violations and punishments across human societies.","authors":"Zachary H Garfield, Erik J Ringen, William Buckner, Dithapelo Medupe, Richard W Wrangham, Luke Glowacki","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2023.7","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2023.7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Punishments for norm violations are hypothesised to be a crucial component of the maintenance of cooperation in humans but are rarely studied from a comparative perspective. We investigated the degree to which punishment systems were correlated with socioecology and cultural history. We took data from the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample database and coded ethnographic documents from a sample of 131 largely non-industrial societies. We recorded whether punishment for norm violations concerned adultery, religion, food, rape or war cowardice and whether sanctions were reputational, physical, material or execution. We used Bayesian phylogenetic regression modelling to test for culture-level covariation. We found little evidence of phylogenetic signals in evidence for punishment types, suggesting that punishment systems change relatively quickly over cultural evolutionary history. We found evidence that reputational punishment was associated with egalitarianism and the absence of food storage; material punishment was associated with the presence of food storage; physical punishment was moderately associated with greater dependence on hunting; and execution punishment was moderately associated with social stratification. Taken together, our results suggest that the role and kind of punishment vary both by the severity of the norm violation, but also by the specific socio-economic system of the society.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e11"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426015/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10395061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Statistical signals of copying are robust to time- and space-averaging. 复制的统计信号不受时间和空间平均的影响。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-04-13 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.5
Mason Youngblood, Helena Miton, Olivier Morin

Cattle brands (ownership marks left on animals) are subject to forces influencing other graphic codes: the copying of constituent parts, pressure for distinctiveness and pressure for complexity. The historical record of cattle brands in some US states is complete owing to legal registration, providing a unique opportunity to assess how sampling processes leading to time- and space-averaging influence our ability to make inferences from limited datasets in fields like archaeology. In this preregistered study, we used a dataset of ~81,000 Kansas cattle brands (1990-2016) to explore two aspects: (1) the relative influence of copying, pressure for distinctiveness and pressure for complexity on the creation and diffusion of brand components; and (2) the effects of time- and space-averaging on statistical signals. By conducting generative inference with an agent-based model, we found that the patterns in our data are consistent with copying and pressure for intermediate complexity. In addition, by comparing mixed and structured datasets, we found that these statistical signals of copying are robust to, and possibly boosted by, time- and space-averaging.

牛烙印(动物身上留下的所有权标记)受到影响其他图形代码的各种力量的制约:对构成部分的复制、对独特性的压力和对复杂性的压力。由于进行了合法登记,美国一些州的牛烙印历史记录是完整的,这为我们提供了一个独特的机会,以评估导致时间和空间平均化的取样过程如何影响我们从考古学等领域的有限数据集中进行推论的能力。在这项预先登记的研究中,我们使用了一个包含约 81,000 个堪萨斯州牛品牌(1990-2016 年)的数据集来探索两个方面:(1) 复制、独特性压力和复杂性压力对品牌成分的创造和传播的相对影响;(2) 时间和空间平均化对统计信号的影响。通过使用基于代理的模型进行生成推理,我们发现数据中的模式与复制和对中间复杂性的压力是一致的。此外,通过对混合数据集和结构化数据集进行比较,我们发现这些复制的统计信号对时间和空间平均化具有稳健性,并且可能受到时间和空间平均化的影响。
{"title":"Statistical signals of copying are robust to time- and space-averaging.","authors":"Mason Youngblood, Helena Miton, Olivier Morin","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2023.5","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2023.5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cattle brands (ownership marks left on animals) are subject to forces influencing other graphic codes: the copying of constituent parts, pressure for distinctiveness and pressure for complexity. The historical record of cattle brands in some US states is complete owing to legal registration, providing a unique opportunity to assess how sampling processes leading to time- and space-averaging influence our ability to make inferences from limited datasets in fields like archaeology. In this preregistered study, we used a dataset of ~81,000 Kansas cattle brands (1990-2016) to explore two aspects: (1) the relative influence of copying, pressure for distinctiveness and pressure for complexity on the creation and diffusion of brand components; and (2) the effects of time- and space-averaging on statistical signals. By conducting generative inference with an agent-based model, we found that the patterns in our data are consistent with copying and pressure for intermediate complexity. In addition, by comparing mixed and structured datasets, we found that these statistical signals of copying are robust to, and possibly boosted by, time- and space-averaging.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e10"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-04-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426036/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10019114","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Agta hunter-gatherer oral microbiomes are shaped by contact network structure. Agta狩猎采集者的口腔微生物群是由接触网络结构形成的。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-23 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.4
Federico Musciotto, Begoña Dobon, Michael Greenacre, Alex Mira, Nikhil Chaudhary, Gul Deniz Salali, Pascale Gerbault, Rodolph Schlaepfer, Leonora H Astete, Marilyn Ngales, Jesus Gomez-Gardenes, Vito Latora, Federico Battiston, Jaume Bertranpetit, Lucio Vinicius, Andrea Bamberg Migliano

Here we investigate the effects of extensive sociality and mobility on the oral microbiome of 138 Agta hunter-gatherers from the Philippines. Our comparisons of microbiome composition showed that the Agta are more similar to Central African BaYaka hunter-gatherers than to neighbouring farmers. We also defined the Agta social microbiome as a set of 137 oral bacteria (only 7% of 1980 amplicon sequence variants) significantly influenced by social contact (quantified through wireless sensors of short-range interactions). We show that large interaction networks including strong links between close kin, spouses and even unrelated friends can significantly predict bacterial transmission networks across Agta camps. Finally, we show that more central individuals to social networks are also bacterial supersharers. We conclude that hunter-gatherer social microbiomes are predominantly pathogenic and were shaped by evolutionary tradeoffs between extensive sociality and disease spread.

在这里,我们研究了广泛的社会性和流动性对菲律宾 138 名阿格塔狩猎采集者口腔微生物组的影响。我们对微生物组组成进行的比较显示,阿格塔人与中非巴亚卡狩猎采集者的相似程度高于与邻近农民的相似程度。我们还将阿格塔人的社会微生物组定义为一组受社会接触(通过短程互动无线传感器量化)显著影响的 137 种口腔细菌(仅占 1980 个扩增子序列变异的 7%)。我们的研究表明,大型互动网络(包括近亲、配偶、甚至无血缘关系的朋友之间的紧密联系)可以显著预测 Agta 营地的细菌传播网络。最后,我们表明,社会网络中的核心个体也是细菌的超级分享者。我们的结论是,狩猎采集者的社会微生物组主要是致病性的,是在广泛的社会性和疾病传播之间的进化权衡中形成的。
{"title":"Agta hunter-gatherer oral microbiomes are shaped by contact network structure.","authors":"Federico Musciotto, Begoña Dobon, Michael Greenacre, Alex Mira, Nikhil Chaudhary, Gul Deniz Salali, Pascale Gerbault, Rodolph Schlaepfer, Leonora H Astete, Marilyn Ngales, Jesus Gomez-Gardenes, Vito Latora, Federico Battiston, Jaume Bertranpetit, Lucio Vinicius, Andrea Bamberg Migliano","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2023.4","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2023.4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Here we investigate the effects of extensive sociality and mobility on the oral microbiome of 138 Agta hunter-gatherers from the Philippines. Our comparisons of microbiome composition showed that the Agta are more similar to Central African BaYaka hunter-gatherers than to neighbouring farmers. We also defined the Agta social microbiome as a set of 137 oral bacteria (only 7% of 1980 amplicon sequence variants) significantly influenced by social contact (quantified through wireless sensors of short-range interactions). We show that large interaction networks including strong links between close kin, spouses and even unrelated friends can significantly predict bacterial transmission networks across Agta camps. Finally, we show that more central individuals to social networks are also bacterial supersharers. We conclude that hunter-gatherer social microbiomes are predominantly pathogenic and were shaped by evolutionary tradeoffs between extensive sociality and disease spread.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e9"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/3e/e0/S2513843X2300004Xa.PMC10426009.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10019112","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Socioeconomic benefits and limited parent-offspring disagreement in arranged marriages in Nepal. 尼泊尔包办婚姻中的社会经济利益和有限的父母与后代之间的分歧。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-22 eCollection Date: 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.3
Elizabeth Agey, Savannah Crippen, Alyx Wells, Parash Upreti

Mate preferences probably evolved to increase fitness; however, studies using arranged and non-arranged marriage as proxies for limited and free mate choice (respectively) do not find any reproductive differences. We explore why arranged and non-arranged marriages are an imperfect proxy for limited and free-choice matings and what fitness effects different marriage types could produce. Data from focus group discussions with men and women in Nepal show that there are three spouse choice categories with differing levels of parental influence over mate choice, reinforcing that arranged and non-arranged are not dichotomous. Discussions also show that parents and offspring, especially sons, may be more aligned in in-law/mate preferences than expected, demonstrating the need to establish clear domains of parent-offspring disagreement over spouse choice in the community before investigating fitness. Several social and financial benefits that are only available to arranged couples in this community were detected, and these benefits could compensate for any costs of not choosing a spouse independently. These benefits of arranged marriage are more salient for women than for men. These discussions indicate that predictions about the effects of spouse choice on fitness outcomes may differ for men and women and depend on community-specific socioeconomic benefits.

配偶偏好的进化可能是为了提高适配性;然而,将包办婚姻和非包办婚姻分别作为有限配偶选择和自由配偶选择的替代物的研究并未发现任何生殖差异。我们探讨了为什么包办婚姻和非包办婚姻不能完美地代表有限配偶选择和自由配偶选择,以及不同的婚姻类型会产生什么样的生殖效应。与尼泊尔男性和女性进行的焦点小组讨论数据显示,有三种配偶选择类别,父母对配偶选择的影响程度各不相同,这进一步说明包办和非包办并非二分法。讨论还表明,父母和后代(尤其是儿子)在姻亲/配偶偏好上的一致性可能比预期的要高,这表明在调查合适性之前,有必要在社区中明确父母和后代在择偶问题上的分歧领域。研究发现,在这个社区中,只有包办婚姻的夫妇才能获得一些社会和经济利益,这些利益可以弥补不独立选择配偶的任何代价。包办婚姻的这些好处对女性比对男性更明显。这些讨论表明,关于择偶对健康结果的影响的预测可能对男性和女性有所不同,并取决于特定社区的社会经济利益。
{"title":"Socioeconomic benefits and limited parent-offspring disagreement in arranged marriages in Nepal.","authors":"Elizabeth Agey, Savannah Crippen, Alyx Wells, Parash Upreti","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2023.3","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2023.3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mate preferences probably evolved to increase fitness; however, studies using arranged and non-arranged marriage as proxies for limited and free mate choice (respectively) do not find any reproductive differences. We explore why arranged and non-arranged marriages are an imperfect proxy for limited and free-choice matings and what fitness effects different marriage types could produce. Data from focus group discussions with men and women in Nepal show that there are three spouse choice categories with differing levels of parental influence over mate choice, reinforcing that arranged and non-arranged are not dichotomous. Discussions also show that parents and offspring, especially sons, may be more aligned in in-law/mate preferences than expected, demonstrating the need to establish clear domains of parent-offspring disagreement over spouse choice in the community before investigating fitness. Several social and financial benefits that are only available to arranged couples in this community were detected, and these benefits could compensate for any costs of not choosing a spouse independently. These benefits of arranged marriage are more salient for women than for men. These discussions indicate that predictions about the effects of spouse choice on fitness outcomes may differ for men and women and depend on community-specific socioeconomic benefits.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e7"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2023-02-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426041/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10010664","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pathways to cultural adaptation: the coevolution of cumulative culture and social networks 文化适应的途径:累积文化与社会网络的共同进化
IF 2.6 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-02-21 DOI: 10.1101/2023.02.21.529416
M. Smolla, Erol Akçay
Humans have adapted to an immense array of ecologies by accumulating culturally transmitted knowledge and skills. Culture accumulates in at least two ways: via more distinct cultural traits, or via improvements of existing cultural trait. A trade-off is expected between these owing to the fact that social learning opportunities are finite and social learning often requires multiple exposures. Furthermore, what kind of culture accumulates depends on, and coevolves with, the social structure of societies. Here we show that the coevolution of social networks for learning and cumulative culture results in two distinct pathways to cultural adaptation: highly connected populations with high proficiency but low cultural trait diversity vs. sparsely connected populations with low proficiency but more cultural trait diversity. Importantly, we show there is a general conflict between group-level payoffs, which is maximised in highly connected groups that attain high proficiency, and individual level selection, which favours disconnection. This conflict emerges from the interaction of social learning with population structure and causes populations to cycle between the two cultural and network states. The same conflict creates a paradox where improving individual innovation rates lowers the payoffs of groups. Finally, we explore how populations navigate these two pathways in heterogeneous and changing environments, and show that high heterogeneity in payoffs and slow rate of environmental change favours high proficiency, while fast rate of environmental change favours more trait diversity. We also find that the proficiency pathway to cultural adaptation is favoured with increased population size, but only in slow changing environments. Our results uncover previously unrecognised trade-offs and tensions in the coevolutionary dynamics of cumulative culture and social structure, with broad implications for human social evolution.
人类通过积累文化传播的知识和技能,适应了大量的生态环境。文化至少以两种方式积累:通过更独特的文化特征,或通过对现有文化特征的改进。由于社会学习机会有限,社会学习往往需要多次接触,因此预计两者之间会有权衡。此外,什么样的文化积累取决于社会的社会结构,并与之共同发展。在这里,我们表明,学习和累积文化的社会网络的共同进化导致了两条不同的文化适应途径:具有高熟练度但文化特征多样性较低的高度联系人群与具有低熟练度但更具文化特征多样度的稀疏联系人群。重要的是,我们表明,在群体层面的回报和个人层面的选择之间存在普遍冲突,群体层面的收益在获得高熟练度的高度连接的群体中最大化,而个人层面的回报有利于断开连接。这种冲突源于社会学习与人口结构的互动,并导致人口在两种文化和网络状态之间循环。同样的冲突造成了一个悖论,即提高个人创新率会降低群体的回报。最后,我们探索了种群如何在异质和不断变化的环境中驾驭这两条路径,并表明收益的高度异质性和环境变化的缓慢速率有利于高熟练度,而环境变化的快速速率有利于更多的性状多样性。我们还发现,随着人口规模的增加,文化适应的熟练途径是有利的,但只有在缓慢变化的环境中。我们的研究结果揭示了累积文化和社会结构的共同进化动力学中以前未被认识到的权衡和紧张关系,对人类社会进化有着广泛的影响。
{"title":"Pathways to cultural adaptation: the coevolution of cumulative culture and social networks","authors":"M. Smolla, Erol Akçay","doi":"10.1101/2023.02.21.529416","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.02.21.529416","url":null,"abstract":"Humans have adapted to an immense array of ecologies by accumulating culturally transmitted knowledge and skills. Culture accumulates in at least two ways: via more distinct cultural traits, or via improvements of existing cultural trait. A trade-off is expected between these owing to the fact that social learning opportunities are finite and social learning often requires multiple exposures. Furthermore, what kind of culture accumulates depends on, and coevolves with, the social structure of societies. Here we show that the coevolution of social networks for learning and cumulative culture results in two distinct pathways to cultural adaptation: highly connected populations with high proficiency but low cultural trait diversity vs. sparsely connected populations with low proficiency but more cultural trait diversity. Importantly, we show there is a general conflict between group-level payoffs, which is maximised in highly connected groups that attain high proficiency, and individual level selection, which favours disconnection. This conflict emerges from the interaction of social learning with population structure and causes populations to cycle between the two cultural and network states. The same conflict creates a paradox where improving individual innovation rates lowers the payoffs of groups. Finally, we explore how populations navigate these two pathways in heterogeneous and changing environments, and show that high heterogeneity in payoffs and slow rate of environmental change favours high proficiency, while fast rate of environmental change favours more trait diversity. We also find that the proficiency pathway to cultural adaptation is favoured with increased population size, but only in slow changing environments. Our results uncover previously unrecognised trade-offs and tensions in the coevolutionary dynamics of cumulative culture and social structure, with broad implications for human social evolution.","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41830688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing evolutionary conflict theories for sexual and physical intimate partner violence in Sub-Saharan Africa. 在撒哈拉以南非洲测试性和身体亲密伴侣暴力的进化冲突理论。
IF 2.6 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2022.58
Janet A Howard, Mhairi A Gibson

Intimate partner violence (IPV) refers to physical, sexual and psychological violence. Here an evolutionary approach is used to compare risk factors for male-to-female IPV perpetration, analysing physical and sexual IPV separately. Two hypotheses based on sexual conflict theory have been applied to IPV perpetration, but they remain largely untested using empirical data: (a) men perpetrate IPV in response to a perceived threat to their paternity certainty; and (b) IPV is caused by men pursuing a higher fertility optima than their partners, either within marriage (reproductive coercion) or outside marriage (paternal disinvestment). Demographic Health Survey data from couples in 12 sub-Saharan African countries (n = 25,577) were used to test these evolutionary hypotheses, using multilevel models and controlling for potential social and environmental confounds. The results show that evolutionary theory provides important insight into different risk factors by IPV type. Indicators of paternity concern are associated with an increased risk of both physical and sexual IPV, indicators of paternal disinvestment are associated with an increased risk of physical IPV only, while reproductive coercion is not associated with either IPV type. The risk factors identified here correspond with proximate-level explanations for IPV perpetration, but an evolutionary interpretation explains why these particular factors may motivate IPV in certain contexts.

亲密伴侣暴力(IPV)是指身体、性和心理暴力。在这里,一种进化的方法被用来比较男性对女性IPV犯罪的危险因素,分别分析身体和性IPV。基于性冲突理论的两种假设已被应用于IPV的实施,但它们在很大程度上尚未得到实证数据的检验:(a)男性实施IPV是为了回应对其父权确定性的感知威胁;(b) IPV是由于男子在婚姻内(生殖强迫)或婚姻外(父亲撤资)追求比其伴侣更高的最佳生育率造成的。来自12个撒哈拉以南非洲国家夫妇的人口健康调查数据(n = 25,577)被用来测试这些进化假设,使用多层次模型并控制潜在的社会和环境混杂因素。结果表明,进化理论为IPV类型不同的危险因素提供了重要的见解。父权问题的指标与身体和性IPV风险增加有关,父权撤资的指标只与身体IPV风险增加有关,而生殖强迫与这两种IPV类型都没有关系。这里确定的风险因素与IPV发生的近似解释相对应,但进化的解释解释了为什么这些特定因素可能在某些情况下激发IPV。
{"title":"Testing evolutionary conflict theories for sexual and physical intimate partner violence in Sub-Saharan Africa.","authors":"Janet A Howard,&nbsp;Mhairi A Gibson","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2022.58","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.58","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intimate partner violence (IPV) refers to physical, sexual and psychological violence. Here an evolutionary approach is used to compare risk factors for male-to-female IPV perpetration, analysing physical and sexual IPV separately. Two hypotheses based on sexual conflict theory have been applied to IPV perpetration, but they remain largely untested using empirical data: (a) men perpetrate IPV in response to a perceived threat to their paternity certainty; and (b) IPV is caused by men pursuing a higher fertility optima than their partners, either within marriage (reproductive coercion) or outside marriage (paternal disinvestment). Demographic Health Survey data from couples in 12 sub-Saharan African countries (<i>n</i> = 25,577) were used to test these evolutionary hypotheses, using multilevel models and controlling for potential social and environmental confounds. The results show that evolutionary theory provides important insight into different risk factors by IPV type. Indicators of paternity concern are associated with an increased risk of both physical and sexual IPV, indicators of paternal disinvestment are associated with an increased risk of physical IPV only, while reproductive coercion is not associated with either IPV type. The risk factors identified here correspond with proximate-level explanations for IPV perpetration, but an evolutionary interpretation explains why these particular factors may motivate IPV in certain contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e6"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426027/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10373307","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Market integration, income inequality, and kinship system among the Mosuo of China. 摩梭人的市场一体化、收入不平等与亲属制度。
IF 2.6 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2022.52
Siobhán M Mattison, Neil MacLaren, Chun-Yi Sum, Peter M Mattison, Ruizhe Liu, Mary K Shenk, Tami Blumenfield, Mingjie Su, Hui Li, Katherine Wander

Increased access to defensible material wealth is hypothesised to escalate inequality. Market integration, which creates novel opportunities in cash economies, provides a means of testing this hypothesis. Using demographic data collected from 505 households among the matrilineal and patrilineal Mosuo in 2017, we test whether market integration is associated with increased material wealth, whether increased material wealth is associated with wealth inequality, and whether being in a matrilineal vs. patrilineal kinship system alters the relationship between wealth and inequality. We find evidence that market integration, measured as distance to the nearest source of tourism and primary source of household income, is associated with increased household income and 'modern' asset value. Both village-level market integration and mean asset value were associated negatively, rather than positively, with inequality, contrary to predictions. Finally, income, modern wealth and inequality were higher in matrilineal communities that were located closer to the centre of tourism and where tourism has long provided a relatively stable source of income. However, we also observed exacerbated inequality with increasing farm animal value in patriliny. We conclude that the forces affecting wealth and inequality depend on local context and that the importance of local institutions is obscured by aggregate statistics drawn from modern nation states.

人们假设,获得可防御物质财富的机会增加会加剧不平等。市场一体化在现金经济中创造了新的机会,为检验这一假设提供了一种手段。本文利用2017年收集的505户摩梭人母系和父系家庭的人口统计数据,检验了市场一体化是否与物质财富增加有关,物质财富增加是否与财富不平等有关,以及母系与父系亲属制度是否改变了财富与不平等之间的关系。我们发现有证据表明,市场一体化(以与最近的旅游来源和家庭收入主要来源的距离来衡量)与家庭收入和“现代”资产价值的增加有关。与预测相反,村级市场一体化和平均资产价值都与不平等呈负相关,而不是正相关。最后,在靠近旅游业中心的母系社区,收入、现代财富和不平等程度更高,旅游业长期以来提供了相对稳定的收入来源。然而,我们也观察到,随着父权制中农场动物价值的增加,不平等加剧。我们得出的结论是,影响财富和不平等的力量取决于当地环境,而当地制度的重要性被来自现代民族国家的总体统计数据所掩盖。
{"title":"Market integration, income inequality, and kinship system among the Mosuo of China.","authors":"Siobhán M Mattison,&nbsp;Neil MacLaren,&nbsp;Chun-Yi Sum,&nbsp;Peter M Mattison,&nbsp;Ruizhe Liu,&nbsp;Mary K Shenk,&nbsp;Tami Blumenfield,&nbsp;Mingjie Su,&nbsp;Hui Li,&nbsp;Katherine Wander","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2022.52","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2022.52","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Increased access to defensible material wealth is hypothesised to escalate inequality. Market integration, which creates novel opportunities in cash economies, provides a means of testing this hypothesis. Using demographic data collected from 505 households among the matrilineal and patrilineal Mosuo in 2017, we test whether market integration is associated with increased material wealth, whether increased material wealth is associated with wealth inequality, and whether being in a matrilineal vs. patrilineal kinship system alters the relationship between wealth and inequality. We find evidence that market integration, measured as distance to the nearest source of tourism and primary source of household income, is associated with increased household income and 'modern' asset value. Both village-level market integration and mean asset value were associated negatively, rather than positively, with inequality, contrary to predictions. Finally, income, modern wealth and inequality were higher in matrilineal communities that were located closer to the centre of tourism and where tourism has long provided a relatively stable source of income. However, we also observed exacerbated inequality with increasing farm animal value in patriliny. We conclude that the forces affecting wealth and inequality depend on local context and that the importance of local institutions is obscured by aggregate statistics drawn from modern nation states.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"5 ","pages":"e4"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10426023/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10010665","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Evolutionary Human Sciences
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1