首页 > 最新文献

Evolutionary Human Sciences最新文献

英文 中文
Coevolution of norm psychology and cooperation through exapted conformity. 规范心理的共同进化和通过外显一致性进行的合作。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-24 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.37
Yuta Kido, Masanori Takezawa

People willingly follow norms and values, often incurring material costs. This behaviour supposedly stems from evolved norm psychology, contributing to large-scale cooperation among humans. It has been argued that cooperation is influenced by two types of norms: injunctive and descriptive. This study theoretically explores the socialisation of humans under these norms. Our agent-based model simulates scenarios where diverse agents with heterogeneous norm psychologies engage in collective action to maximise their utility functions that capture three motives: gaining material payoff, following injunctive and descriptive norms. Multilevel selective pressure drives the evolution of norm psychology that affects the utility function. Further, we develop a model with exapted conformity, assuming selective advantage for descriptive norm psychology. We show that norm psychology can evolve via cultural group selection. We then identify two normative conditions that favour the evolution of norm psychology, and therefore cooperation: injunctive norms promoting punitive behaviour and descriptive norms. Furthermore, we delineate different characteristics of cooperative societies under these two conditions and explore the potential for a macro transition between them. Together, our results validate the emergence of large-scale cooperative societies through social norms and suggest complementary roles that conformity and punishment play in human prosociality.

人们心甘情愿地遵循规范和价值观,往往为此付出物质代价。据称,这种行为源于进化的规范心理学,促成了人类之间的大规模合作。有观点认为,合作受到两类规范的影响:强制规范和描述性规范。本研究从理论上探讨了人类在这些规范下的社会化过程。我们基于代理的模型模拟了具有不同规范心理的不同代理参与集体行动的情景,以最大化他们的效用函数,这些效用函数捕捉了三种动机:获得物质报酬、遵循强制规范和描述性规范。多层次的选择压力驱动着规范心理的演变,从而影响效用函数。此外,我们假设描述性规范心理具有选择性优势,从而建立了一个具有外显顺应性的模型。我们证明,规范心理可以通过文化群体选择而进化。然后,我们确定了有利于规范心理进化并进而有利于合作的两个规范条件:促进惩罚性行为的强制规范和描述性规范。此外,我们还描述了这两种条件下合作社会的不同特征,并探讨了它们之间宏观过渡的可能性。总之,我们的研究结果验证了大规模合作社会的出现是通过社会规范实现的,并提出了一致性和惩罚性在人类亲社会性中的互补作用。
{"title":"Coevolution of norm psychology and cooperation through exapted conformity.","authors":"Yuta Kido, Masanori Takezawa","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.37","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.37","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People willingly follow norms and values, often incurring material costs. This behaviour supposedly stems from evolved norm psychology, contributing to large-scale cooperation among humans. It has been argued that cooperation is influenced by two types of norms: injunctive and descriptive. This study theoretically explores the socialisation of humans under these norms. Our agent-based model simulates scenarios where diverse agents with heterogeneous norm psychologies engage in collective action to maximise their utility functions that capture three motives: gaining material payoff, following injunctive and descriptive norms. Multilevel selective pressure drives the evolution of norm psychology that affects the utility function. Further, we develop a model with exapted conformity, assuming selective advantage for descriptive norm psychology. We show that norm psychology can evolve via cultural group selection. We then identify two normative conditions that favour the evolution of norm psychology, and therefore cooperation: injunctive norms promoting punitive behaviour and descriptive norms. Furthermore, we delineate different characteristics of cooperative societies under these two conditions and explore the potential for a macro transition between them. Together, our results validate the emergence of large-scale cooperative societies through social norms and suggest complementary roles that conformity and punishment play in human prosociality.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e35"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11503932/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142509686","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Salience of infectious diseases did not increase xenophobia during the COVID-19 pandemic. 在 COVID-19 大流行期间,传染病的显著性并没有增加仇外心理。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-18 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.28
Lei Fan, Joshua M Tybur, Paul A M Van Lange

Multiple proposals suggest that xenophobia increases when infectious disease threats are salient. The current longitudinal study tested this hypothesis by examining whether and how anti-immigrant sentiments varied in the Netherlands across four time points during the COVID-19 pandemic (May 2020, February 2021, October 2021 and June 2022 through Flycatcher.eu). The results revealed that (1) anti-immigrant sentiments were no higher in early assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations and deaths were high, than in later assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations were low, and (2) within-person changes in explicit disease concerns and disgust sensitivity did not relate to anti-immigrant sentiments, although stable individual differences in disgust sensitivity did. These findings suggest that anecdotal accounts of increased xenophobia during the pandemic did not generalize to the population sampled from here. They also suggest that not all increases in ecological pathogen threats and disease salience increase xenophobia.

多项研究表明,当传染病威胁突出时,仇外心理会增加。本纵向研究通过考察 COVID-19 大流行期间荷兰四个时间点(2020 年 5 月、2021 年 2 月、2021 年 10 月和 2022 年 6 月,通过 Flycatcher.eu)的反移民情绪是否以及如何变化来验证这一假设。结果显示:(1) 在 COVID-19 住院人数和死亡人数较高的早期评估中,反移民情绪并不比在 COVID-19 住院人数较低的后期评估中高;(2) 明确的疾病担忧和厌恶敏感度的人际变化与反移民情绪无关,但厌恶敏感度的稳定个体差异与反移民情绪有关。这些研究结果表明,关于大流行期间仇外心理增加的传闻并没有普遍适用于此处的抽样人群。这些研究还表明,并非所有生态病原体威胁和疾病显著性的增加都会增加仇外心理。
{"title":"Salience of infectious diseases did not increase xenophobia during the COVID-19 pandemic.","authors":"Lei Fan, Joshua M Tybur, Paul A M Van Lange","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.28","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.28","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multiple proposals suggest that xenophobia increases when infectious disease threats are salient. The current longitudinal study tested this hypothesis by examining whether and how anti-immigrant sentiments varied in the Netherlands across four time points during the COVID-19 pandemic (May 2020, February 2021, October 2021 and June 2022 through Flycatcher.eu). The results revealed that (1) anti-immigrant sentiments were no higher in early assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations and deaths were high, than in later assessments, when COVID-19 hospitalizations were low, and (2) within-person changes in explicit disease concerns and disgust sensitivity did not relate to anti-immigrant sentiments, although stable individual differences in disgust sensitivity did. These findings suggest that anecdotal accounts of increased xenophobia during the pandemic did not generalize to the population sampled from here. They also suggest that not all increases in ecological pathogen threats and disease salience increase xenophobia.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e34"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11514650/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142523269","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the expensive-tissue hypothesis' prediction of inter-tissue competition using causal modelling with latent variables. 利用潜变量因果模型检验 "昂贵组织假说 "对组织间竞争的预测。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-10-14 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.26
Meghan Shirley Bezerra, Samuli Helle, Kiran K Seunarine, Owen J Arthurs, Simon Eaton, Jane E Williams, Chris A Clark, Jonathan C K Wells

The expensive-tissue hypothesis (ETH) posited a brain-gut trade-off to explain how humans evolved large, costly brains. Versions of the ETH interrogating gut or other body tissues have been tested in non-human animals, but not humans. We collected brain and body composition data in 70 South Asian women and used structural equation modelling with instrumental variables, an approach that handles threats to causal inference including measurement error, unmeasured confounding and reverse causality. We tested a negative, causal effect of the latent construct 'nutritional investment in brain tissues' (MRI-derived brain volumes) on the construct 'nutritional investment in lean body tissues' (organ volume and skeletal muscle). We also predicted a negative causal effect of the brain latent on fat mass. We found negative causal estimates for both brain and lean tissue (-0.41, 95% CI, -1.13, 0.23) and brain and fat (-0.56, 95% CI, -2.46, 2.28). These results, although inconclusive, are consistent with theory and prior evidence of the brain trading off with lean and fat tissues, and they are an important step in assessing empirical evidence for the ETH in humans. Analyses using larger datasets, genetic data and causal modelling are required to build on these findings and expand the evidence base.

昂贵组织假说(ETH)假定了大脑与肠道之间的权衡,以解释人类如何进化出庞大而昂贵的大脑。针对肠道或其他身体组织的昂贵组织假说版本已在非人类动物身上进行过测试,但尚未在人类身上进行过测试。我们收集了 70 名南亚女性的大脑和身体成分数据,并使用了带有工具变量的结构方程模型,这种方法可以处理因果推断所面临的威胁,包括测量误差、未测量混杂因素和反向因果关系。我们检验了 "脑组织营养投资"(核磁共振成像得出的脑容量)这一潜在结构对 "瘦身组织营养投资"(器官体积和骨骼肌)这一结构的负向因果效应。我们还预测了大脑潜构对脂肪量的负因果效应。我们发现大脑和瘦身组织(-0.41,95% CI,-1.13,0.23)以及大脑和脂肪(-0.56,95% CI,-2.46,2.28)的因果关系估计值均为负值。这些结果虽然尚无定论,但与大脑与瘦肉和脂肪组织交换的理论和先前的证据是一致的,它们是评估人类 ETH 经验证据的重要一步。需要利用更大的数据集、遗传数据和因果模型进行分析,以巩固这些发现并扩大证据基础。
{"title":"Testing the expensive-tissue hypothesis' prediction of inter-tissue competition using causal modelling with latent variables.","authors":"Meghan Shirley Bezerra, Samuli Helle, Kiran K Seunarine, Owen J Arthurs, Simon Eaton, Jane E Williams, Chris A Clark, Jonathan C K Wells","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.26","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.26","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The expensive-tissue hypothesis (ETH) posited a brain-gut trade-off to explain how humans evolved large, costly brains. Versions of the ETH interrogating gut or other body tissues have been tested in non-human animals, but not humans. We collected brain and body composition data in 70 South Asian women and used structural equation modelling with instrumental variables, an approach that handles threats to causal inference including measurement error, unmeasured confounding and reverse causality. We tested a negative, causal effect of the latent construct 'nutritional investment in brain tissues' (MRI-derived brain volumes) on the construct 'nutritional investment in lean body tissues' (organ volume and skeletal muscle). We also predicted a negative causal effect of the brain latent on fat mass. We found negative causal estimates for both brain and lean tissue (-0.41, 95% CI, -1.13, 0.23) and brain and fat (-0.56, 95% CI, -2.46, 2.28). These results, although inconclusive, are consistent with theory and prior evidence of the brain trading off with lean and fat tissues, and they are an important step in assessing empirical evidence for the ETH in humans. Analyses using larger datasets, genetic data and causal modelling are required to build on these findings and expand the evidence base.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e33"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-10-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11514623/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142523270","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Kin selection as a modulator of human handedness: sex-specific, parental and parent-of-origin effects. 亲属选择是人类手性的调节器:性别特异性、父母和原生父母的影响。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-08-27 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.24
Bing Dong, Silvia Paracchini, Andy Gardner

The frequency of left-handedness in humans is ~10% worldwide and slightly higher in males than females. Twin and family studies estimate the heritability of human handedness at around 25%. The low but substantial frequency of left-handedness has been suggested to imply negative frequency-dependent selection, e.g. owing to a 'surprise' advantage of left-handers in combat against opponents more used to fighting right-handers. Because such game-theoretic hypotheses involve social interaction, here we perform an analysis of the evolution of handedness based on kin-selection, which is understood to play a major role in the evolution of social behaviour generally. We show that: (1) relatedness modulates the balance of right-handedness vs. left-handedness, according to whether left-handedness is marginally selfish vs. marginally altruistic; (2) sex differences in relatedness to social partners may drive sex differences in handedness; (3) differential relatedness of parents and offspring may generate parent-offspring conflict and sexual conflict leading to the evolution of maternal and paternal genetic effects in relation to handedness; and (4) differential relatedness of maternal-origin vs. paternal-origin genes may generate intragenomic conflict leading to the evolution of parent-of-origin-specific gene effects - such as 'genomic imprinting' - and associated maladaptation.

全世界人类左撇子的比例约为 10%,男性略高于女性。双胞胎和家族研究估计,人类左撇子的遗传率约为 25%。有人认为,左撇子出现频率低但数量可观,这意味着与频率有关的负面选择,例如,左撇子在与习惯于与右撇子作战的对手作战时具有 "出其不意 "的优势。由于这种博弈论假说涉及社会互动,因此我们在此基于亲属选择对惯用手的进化进行分析,据了解,亲属选择在一般社会行为的进化中发挥着重要作用。我们证明(1)亲缘关系会调节右手与左手的平衡,这取决于左手是否具有边际自私性与边际利他性;(2)与社会伙伴的亲缘关系中的性别差异可能会驱动手性的性别差异;(3)父母与后代的不同亲缘关系可能会产生父母与后代的冲突和性冲突,从而导致与手性有关的母系和父系遗传效应的进化;以及(4)母系起源基因与父系起源基因的不同亲缘关系可能会产生与手性有关的母系和父系遗传效应。(4) 母源基因与父源基因的不同亲缘关系可能会产生基因组内冲突,从而导致父源特异性基因效应(如 "基因组印记")的进化以及相关的适应不良。
{"title":"Kin selection as a modulator of human handedness: sex-specific, parental and parent-of-origin effects.","authors":"Bing Dong, Silvia Paracchini, Andy Gardner","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.24","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.24","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The frequency of left-handedness in humans is ~10% worldwide and slightly higher in males than females. Twin and family studies estimate the heritability of human handedness at around 25%. The low but substantial frequency of left-handedness has been suggested to imply negative frequency-dependent selection, e.g. owing to a 'surprise' advantage of left-handers in combat against opponents more used to fighting right-handers. Because such game-theoretic hypotheses involve social interaction, here we perform an analysis of the evolution of handedness based on kin-selection, which is understood to play a major role in the evolution of social behaviour generally. We show that: (1) relatedness modulates the balance of right-handedness vs. left-handedness, according to whether left-handedness is marginally selfish vs. marginally altruistic; (2) sex differences in relatedness to social partners may drive sex differences in handedness; (3) differential relatedness of parents and offspring may generate parent-offspring conflict and sexual conflict leading to the evolution of maternal and paternal genetic effects in relation to handedness; and (4) differential relatedness of maternal-origin vs. paternal-origin genes may generate intragenomic conflict leading to the evolution of parent-of-origin-specific gene effects - such as 'genomic imprinting' - and associated maladaptation.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e32"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11418076/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142308695","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of mating effort and co-residence history in step-grandparental investment. 交配努力和共同居住历史在继祖父母投资中的作用。
IF 2.6 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-05-16 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.17
Jenni E Pettay, David A Coall, Mirkka Danielsbacka, Antti O Tanskanen

The prevalence of divorce in both parental and grandparental generations has led to a rise in the number of children who now have families that include both biological and step-grandparents. Despite the thorough examination of biological grandparents' contributions in the recent literature, there remains a scarcity of studies focusing on the investment of step-grandparents. Using population-based data from a sample of 2494 parents in Germany, we assessed grandparental investment through financial support and assistance with childcare of grandparents (N = 4238) and step-grandparents (N = 486). The study revealed that step-grandparents provided lower levels of investment in their grandchildren compared with biological grandparents. Furthermore, the study identified that a longer duration of co-residence between step-grandparents and parents earlier in life did not correspond to an increase or decrease in step-grandparental investment. However, investment by separated biological grandparents increased with the increasing length of co-residence with parents. In line with the scarce literature on step-grandparental investment, these findings indicate that mating effort may be the most important motivation for step-grandparental investment.

父母和祖父母两代人的离婚率都很高,导致现在有亲生父母和继祖父母的家庭的儿童人数增加。尽管最近的文献对亲生祖父母的贡献进行了深入研究,但关注继祖父母投资的研究仍然很少。我们利用德国 2494 名父母的样本数据,通过祖父母(4238 人)和继祖父母(486 人)的经济支持和育儿援助,对祖父母的投资进行了评估。研究显示,与亲生祖父母相比,继祖父母对孙辈的投资水平较低。此外,研究还发现,继祖父母和父母在生命早期共同居住的时间越长,继祖父母对孙辈的投资就越多或越少。然而,随着与父母共同居住时间的延长,分居的亲生祖父母的投资也在增加。与有关继祖父母投资的稀缺文献相一致,这些研究结果表明,交配努力可能是继祖父母投资的最重要动机。
{"title":"The role of mating effort and co-residence history in step-grandparental investment.","authors":"Jenni E Pettay, David A Coall, Mirkka Danielsbacka, Antti O Tanskanen","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.17","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.17","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The prevalence of divorce in both parental and grandparental generations has led to a rise in the number of children who now have families that include both biological and step-grandparents. Despite the thorough examination of biological grandparents' contributions in the recent literature, there remains a scarcity of studies focusing on the investment of step-grandparents. Using population-based data from a sample of 2494 parents in Germany, we assessed grandparental investment through financial support and assistance with childcare of grandparents (<i>N</i> = 4238) and step-grandparents (<i>N</i> = 486). The study revealed that step-grandparents provided lower levels of investment in their grandchildren compared with biological grandparents. Furthermore, the study identified that a longer duration of co-residence between step-grandparents and parents earlier in life did not correspond to an increase or decrease in step-grandparental investment. However, investment by separated biological grandparents increased with the increasing length of co-residence with parents. In line with the scarce literature on step-grandparental investment, these findings indicate that mating effort may be the most important motivation for step-grandparental investment.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e27"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11106544/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141076951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The floating duck syndrome: biased social learning leads to effort-reward imbalances. 浮鸭综合症:偏颇的社会学习导致努力-回报失衡。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-29 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.20
Erol Akçay, Ryotaro Ohashi

An increasingly common phenomenon in modern work and school settings is individuals taking on too many tasks and spending effort without commensurate rewards. Such an imbalance of efforts and rewards leads to myriad negative consequences, such as burnout, anxiety and disease. Here, we develop a model to explain how such effort-reward imbalances can come about as a result of biased social learning dynamics. Our model is based on a phenomenon that on some US college campuses is called 'the floating duck syndrome'. This phrase refers to the social pressure on individuals to advertise their successes but hide the struggles and the effort put in to achieve them. We show that a bias against revealing the true effort results in social learning dynamics that lead others to underestimate the difficulty of the world. This in turn leads individuals to both invest too much total effort and spread this effort over too many activities, reducing the success rate from each activity and creating effort-reward imbalances. We also consider potential ways to counteract the floating duck effect: we find that solutions other than addressing the root cause, biased observation of effort, are unlikely to work.

在现代工作和学习环境中,一个日益普遍的现象是个人承担了太多的任务,付出了努力却没有得到相应的回报。这种付出与回报的失衡会导致无数负面后果,如倦怠、焦虑和疾病。在这里,我们建立了一个模型来解释这种努力与回报的不平衡是如何由于有偏差的社会学习动力而产生的。我们的模型基于一种在美国大学校园中被称为 "浮鸭综合征 "的现象。这个短语指的是个人所面临的社会压力,即宣传自己的成功,但隐藏为取得成功而付出的奋斗和努力。我们的研究表明,对揭示真实努力的偏见会导致社会学习动力,使他人低估世界的难度。这反过来又会导致个人投入过多的总努力,并将这种努力分散到过多的活动中,从而降低每项活动的成功率,造成努力与回报的不平衡。我们还考虑了抵消浮萍效应的潜在方法:我们发现,除了解决根本原因--对努力的偏差观察--之外,其他解决方案都不太可能奏效。
{"title":"The floating duck syndrome: biased social learning leads to effort-reward imbalances.","authors":"Erol Akçay, Ryotaro Ohashi","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.20","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.20","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An increasingly common phenomenon in modern work and school settings is individuals taking on too many tasks and spending effort without commensurate rewards. Such an imbalance of efforts and rewards leads to myriad negative consequences, such as burnout, anxiety and disease. Here, we develop a model to explain how such effort-reward imbalances can come about as a result of biased social learning dynamics. Our model is based on a phenomenon that on some US college campuses is called 'the floating duck syndrome'. This phrase refers to the social pressure on individuals to advertise their successes but hide the struggles and the effort put in to achieve them. We show that a bias against revealing the true effort results in social learning dynamics that lead others to underestimate the difficulty of the world. This in turn leads individuals to both invest too much total effort and spread this effort over too many activities, reducing the success rate from each activity and creating effort-reward imbalances. We also consider potential ways to counteract the floating duck effect: we find that solutions other than addressing the root cause, biased observation of effort, are unlikely to work.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e30"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362996/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142113002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dyadic inter-group cooperation in shotgun hunting activities in a Congo Basin village. 刚果盆地村庄猎枪狩猎活动中的群体间合作。
IF 2.6 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-04-01 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.14
Vidrige H Kandza, Haneul Jang, Francy Kiabiya Ntamboudila, Sheina Lew-Levy, Adam H Boyette

Understanding the dynamics of inter-group cooperation in human adaptation has been the subject of recent empirical and theoretical studies in evolutionary anthropology, beginning to fill gaps in our knowledge of how interactions across political, economic and social domains can - and often do - lead to stable, large-scale cooperation. Here we investigate dyadic intergroup cooperation in shotgun hunting in the Republic of the Congo. In the Congo Basin, inter-group cooperation between foragers and farmers is at the centre of an exchange system maintained by traditional norms and institutions such as fictive kinship. Here, we focused on what factors predict cooperative shotgun hunting exchanges between BaYaka and Yambe. We conducted structured interviews with 48 BaYaka hunters and 18 Yambe men who organise hunts in a village along the Motaba River. We used Bayesian multilevel regression models to investigate the influence of Yambe and BaYaka attributes on probability of dyadic cooperation. We found that BaYaka men's reputations as skilled hunters and their family size each predicted cooperation in shotgun hunting, whereas there was no effect of Yambe attributes (status, wealth, family size). We discuss the results in terms of evolutionary models of men as hunters and inter-group cooperation, as well as biodiversity conservation implications.

了解人类适应过程中群体间合作的动态一直是进化人类学近期实证和理论研究的主题,这些研究开始填补我们在跨政治、经济和社会领域的互动如何能够--而且往往确实--导致稳定的大规模合作方面的知识空白。在这里,我们研究了刚果共和国猎枪狩猎中的群体间合作。在刚果盆地,狩猎者和农民之间的群体间合作是由传统规范和制度(如虚构的亲属关系)维持的交换体系的核心。在此,我们重点研究了哪些因素会影响巴雅卡人和亚姆贝人之间的猎枪狩猎合作交流。我们在莫塔巴河沿岸的一个村庄对 48 名巴亚卡猎人和 18 名组织狩猎的亚姆贝人进行了结构性访谈。我们使用贝叶斯多层次回归模型来研究 Yambe 和 BaYaka 属性对双方合作概率的影响。我们发现,巴雅卡男性作为熟练猎手的声誉和他们的家庭规模都能预测猎枪狩猎中的合作,而山地人的属性(地位、财富、家庭规模)则没有影响。我们从男性作为猎人和群体间合作的进化模型以及对生物多样性保护的影响的角度讨论了这一结果。
{"title":"Dyadic inter-group cooperation in shotgun hunting activities in a Congo Basin village.","authors":"Vidrige H Kandza, Haneul Jang, Francy Kiabiya Ntamboudila, Sheina Lew-Levy, Adam H Boyette","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.14","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2024.14","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Understanding the dynamics of inter-group cooperation in human adaptation has been the subject of recent empirical and theoretical studies in evolutionary anthropology, beginning to fill gaps in our knowledge of how interactions across political, economic and social domains can - and often do - lead to stable, large-scale cooperation. Here we investigate dyadic intergroup cooperation in shotgun hunting in the Republic of the Congo. In the Congo Basin, inter-group cooperation between foragers and farmers is at the centre of an exchange system maintained by traditional norms and institutions such as fictive kinship. Here, we focused on what factors predict cooperative shotgun hunting exchanges between BaYaka and Yambe. We conducted structured interviews with 48 BaYaka hunters and 18 Yambe men who organise hunts in a village along the Motaba River. We used Bayesian multilevel regression models to investigate the influence of Yambe and BaYaka attributes on probability of dyadic cooperation. We found that BaYaka men's reputations as skilled hunters and their family size each predicted cooperation in shotgun hunting, whereas there was no effect of Yambe attributes (status, wealth, family size). We discuss the results in terms of evolutionary models of men as hunters and inter-group cooperation, as well as biodiversity conservation implications.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e22"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11058523/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140867637","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-cousin marriage among Tsimane forager-horticulturalists during demographic transition and market integration. 人口结构转型和市场一体化期间齐马内狩猎园艺家的跨堂兄妹婚姻。
IF 2.2 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-26 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.11
Arianna Dalzero, Bret A Beheim, Hillard Kaplan, Jonathan Stieglitz, Paul L Hooper, Cody T Ross, Michael Gurven, Dieter Lukas

Although still prevalent in many human societies, the practice of cousin marriage has precipitously declined in populations undergoing rapid demographic and socioeconomic change. However, it is still unclear whether changes in the structure of the marriage pool or changes in the fitness-relevant consequences of cousin marriage more strongly influence the frequency of cousin marriage. Here, we use genealogical data collected by the Tsimane Health and Life History Project to show that there is a small but measurable decline in the frequency of first cross-cousin marriage since the mid-twentieth century. Such changes are linked to concomitant changes in the pool of potential spouses in recent decades. We find only very modest differences in fitness-relevant demographic measures between first cousin and non-cousin marriages. These differences have been diminishing as the Tsimane have become more market integrated. The factors that influence preferences for cousin marriage appear to be less prevalent now than in the past, but cultural inertia might slow the pace of change in marriage norms. Overall, our findings suggest that cultural changes in marriage practices reflect underlying societal changes that shape the pool of potential spouses.

虽然表亲婚姻在许多人类社会中仍然盛行,但在人口和社会经济发生快速变化的人群中,表亲婚姻已急剧减少。然而,目前还不清楚是表亲婚姻结构的变化还是表亲婚姻的适配性相关后果的变化更能影响表亲婚姻的频率。在这里,我们利用 "齐玛内健康和生活史项目"(Tsimane Health and Life History Project)收集的家谱数据表明,自二十世纪中叶以来,首次跨表亲婚姻的频率出现了小幅但可测量的下降。这种变化与近几十年来潜在配偶库的相应变化有关。我们发现,嫡亲表兄妹婚姻与非表兄妹婚姻在人口统计学相关指标上的差异非常小。随着齐马内人越来越融入市场,这些差异也在逐渐缩小。与过去相比,影响表亲婚姻偏好的因素现在似乎不那么普遍了,但文化惯性可能会减缓婚姻规范的变化速度。总之,我们的研究结果表明,婚姻习俗的文化变化反映了潜在配偶群体的社会变化。
{"title":"Cross-cousin marriage among Tsimane forager-horticulturalists during demographic transition and market integration.","authors":"Arianna Dalzero, Bret A Beheim, Hillard Kaplan, Jonathan Stieglitz, Paul L Hooper, Cody T Ross, Michael Gurven, Dieter Lukas","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.11","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.11","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although still prevalent in many human societies, the practice of cousin marriage has precipitously declined in populations undergoing rapid demographic and socioeconomic change. However, it is still unclear whether changes in the structure of the marriage pool or changes in the fitness-relevant consequences of cousin marriage more strongly influence the frequency of cousin marriage. Here, we use genealogical data collected by the Tsimane Health and Life History Project to show that there is a small but measurable decline in the frequency of first cross-cousin marriage since the mid-twentieth century. Such changes are linked to concomitant changes in the pool of potential spouses in recent decades. We find only very modest differences in fitness-relevant demographic measures between first cousin and non-cousin marriages. These differences have been diminishing as the Tsimane have become more market integrated. The factors that influence preferences for cousin marriage appear to be less prevalent now than in the past, but cultural inertia might slow the pace of change in marriage norms. Overall, our findings suggest that cultural changes in marriage practices reflect underlying societal changes that shape the pool of potential spouses.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e18"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10988167/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140858351","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Why cultural distance can promote - or impede - group-beneficial outcomes. 为什么文化距离会促进或阻碍对群体有益的结果?
IF 2.6 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-11 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.8
Bret Alexander Beheim, Adrian Viliami Bell

Quantifying the distance between cultural groups has received substantial recent interest. A key innovation, borrowed from population genetics, is the calculation of cultural FST (CFST) statistics on datasets of human culture. Measuring the variance between groups as a fraction of total variance, FST is theoretically important in additive models of cooperation. Consistent with this, recent empirical work has confirmed that high values of pairwise CFST (measuring cultural distance) strongly predict unwillingness to cooperate with strangers in coordination vignettes. As applications for CFST increase, however, there is greater need to understand its meaning in naturalistic situations beyond additive cooperation. Focusing on games with both positive and negative frequency dependence and high-diversity, mixed equilibria, we derive a simple relationship between FST and the evolution of group-beneficial traits across a broad spectrum of social interactions. Contrary to standard assumptions, this model shows why FST can have both positive and negative marginal effects on the spread of group-beneficial traits under certain realistic conditions. These results provide broader theoretical direction for empirical applications of CFST in the evolutionary study of culture.

量化文化群体之间的距离最近受到了广泛关注。借鉴群体遗传学的一项重要创新是计算人类文化数据集上的文化距离统计(CFST)。FST 用总方差的一部分来衡量群体间的方差,在理论上对合作的加法模型非常重要。与此相一致的是,最近的实证研究证实,成对 CFST 的高值(衡量文化距离)可强烈预测在协调小故事中与陌生人合作的意愿。然而,随着 CFST 应用的增加,人们更需要了解它在自然情况下的意义,而不仅仅是加法合作。我们以具有正负频率依赖性和高多样性混合均衡的博弈为重点,推导出了在广泛的社会互动中,FST 与群体有益特质进化之间的简单关系。与标准假设相反,这个模型显示了为什么在某些现实条件下,FST 对群体有益性状的传播既有正的边际效应,也有负的边际效应。这些结果为 CFST 在文化进化研究中的实证应用提供了更广阔的理论方向。
{"title":"Why cultural distance can promote - or impede - group-beneficial outcomes.","authors":"Bret Alexander Beheim, Adrian Viliami Bell","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.8","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Quantifying the distance between cultural groups has received substantial recent interest. A key innovation, borrowed from population genetics, is the calculation of cultural <i>F<sub>ST</sub></i> (<i>CF<sub>ST</sub></i>) statistics on datasets of human culture. Measuring the variance between groups as a fraction of total variance, <i>F<sub>ST</sub></i> is theoretically important in additive models of cooperation. Consistent with this, recent empirical work has confirmed that high values of pairwise <i>CF<sub>ST</sub></i> (measuring cultural distance) strongly predict unwillingness to cooperate with strangers in coordination vignettes. As applications for <i>CF<sub>ST</sub></i> increase, however, there is greater need to understand its meaning in naturalistic situations beyond additive cooperation. Focusing on games with both positive and negative frequency dependence and high-diversity, mixed equilibria, we derive a simple relationship between <i>F<sub>ST</sub></i> and the evolution of group-beneficial traits across a broad spectrum of social interactions. Contrary to standard assumptions, this model shows why <i>F<sub>ST</sub></i> can have both positive and <i>negative</i> marginal effects on the spread of group-beneficial traits under certain realistic conditions. These results provide broader theoretical direction for empirical applications of <i>CF<sub>ST</sub></i> in the evolutionary study of culture.</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e14"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10955364/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140185899","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Erratum: The shape of lipsmacking: socio-emotional regulation in bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) - CORRIGENDUM. 更正:咂嘴的形状:胡须卷尾猴(Sapajus libidinosus)的社会情绪调节 - CORRIGENDUM。
IF 2.6 Q1 ANTHROPOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-03-04 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2024.4
Natalia Albuquerque, Carine Savalli, Marina Belli, Ana Clara Varella, Beatriz Felício, Juliana França, Patrícia Izar

[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.10.].

[此处更正了文章 DOI:10.1017/ehs.2023.10]。
{"title":"Erratum: The shape of lipsmacking: socio-emotional regulation in bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) - CORRIGENDUM.","authors":"Natalia Albuquerque, Carine Savalli, Marina Belli, Ana Clara Varella, Beatriz Felício, Juliana França, Patrícia Izar","doi":"10.1017/ehs.2024.4","DOIUrl":"10.1017/ehs.2024.4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>[This corrects the article DOI: 10.1017/ehs.2023.10.].</p>","PeriodicalId":36414,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Human Sciences","volume":"6 ","pages":"e13"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10955351/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140185897","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolutionary Human Sciences
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1