首页 > 最新文献

Vision (Switzerland)最新文献

英文 中文
The Influence of Tear Film Quality on Visual Function. 泪膜质量对视觉功能的影响
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.3390/vision8010008
Snježana Kaštelan, Ksenija Gabrić, Maša Mikuličić, Danijela Mrazovac Zimak, Mirela Karabatić, Antonela Gverović Antunica

Background: The prevalence of dry eye disease (DED) is increasing globally, resulting in a variety of eye symptoms characterized by discomfort and visual disturbances. The accurate diagnosis of the disease is often challenging and complex, requiring specialized diagnostic tools. This study aimed to investigate the impact of tear film instability on visual function and to evaluate the value of post-blink blur time (PBBT) as an alternative method for assessing tear film stability.

Methods: The study included 62 subjects: 31 with subjective symptoms of DED (Group A) and a control group consisting of 31 healthy participants (Group B). Symptoms were assessed using the standard Schein questionnaire, supplemented with additional questions. PBBT was measured using standard Snellen charts to investigate a potential association between PBBT and tear film dysfunction. Additional clinical assessments included tear film break-up time (TBUT).

Results: Statistically significant differences were observed in the average values of PBBT and TBUT between the examined groups. The average PBBT was 8.95 ± 5.38 s in the group with DED and 14.66 ± 10.50 s in the control group, p < 0.001. Group A exhibited an average TBUT of 4.77 ± 2.37 s, while Group B had a TBUT of 7.63 ± 3.25 s, p < 0.001. Additionally, a strong positive correlation was identified between PBBT and TBUT values (r = 0.455; p < 0.001).

Conclusions: The research confirms that tear film stability has an important role in the refraction of light and the maintenance of optical quality of vision. PBBT could potentially function as an objective and clinically significant screening test for DED.

背景:干眼症(DED)的发病率在全球范围内呈上升趋势,导致各种以不适和视力障碍为特征的眼部症状。对该疾病的准确诊断往往具有挑战性和复杂性,需要专门的诊断工具。本研究旨在调查泪膜不稳定性对视觉功能的影响,并评估眨眼后模糊时间(PBBT)作为评估泪膜稳定性的替代方法的价值:研究包括 62 名受试者:方法:研究包括 62 名受试者:31 名有 DED 主观症状的受试者(A 组)和由 31 名健康受试者组成的对照组(B 组)。采用标准的 Schein 问卷评估症状,并补充了其他问题。使用标准斯奈伦图表测量 PBBT,以研究 PBBT 与泪膜功能障碍之间的潜在联系。其他临床评估包括泪膜破裂时间(TBUT):受检组之间的 PBBT 和 TBUT 平均值存在明显统计学差异。DED 组的平均 PBBT 为 8.95 ± 5.38 秒,对照组为 14.66 ± 10.50 秒,P < 0.001。A 组的平均 TBUT 为 4.77 ± 2.37 秒,B 组为 7.63 ± 3.25 秒,P < 0.001。此外,PBBT 值与 TBUT 值之间存在很强的正相关性(r = 0.455;p < 0.001):研究证实,泪膜稳定性在光线折射和维持视力光学质量方面起着重要作用。PBBT 有可能成为一种客观且具有临床意义的 DED 筛选测试。
{"title":"The Influence of Tear Film Quality on Visual Function.","authors":"Snježana Kaštelan, Ksenija Gabrić, Maša Mikuličić, Danijela Mrazovac Zimak, Mirela Karabatić, Antonela Gverović Antunica","doi":"10.3390/vision8010008","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision8010008","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>The prevalence of dry eye disease (DED) is increasing globally, resulting in a variety of eye symptoms characterized by discomfort and visual disturbances. The accurate diagnosis of the disease is often challenging and complex, requiring specialized diagnostic tools. This study aimed to investigate the impact of tear film instability on visual function and to evaluate the value of post-blink blur time (PBBT) as an alternative method for assessing tear film stability.</p><p><strong>Methods: </strong>The study included 62 subjects: 31 with subjective symptoms of DED (Group A) and a control group consisting of 31 healthy participants (Group B). Symptoms were assessed using the standard Schein questionnaire, supplemented with additional questions. PBBT was measured using standard Snellen charts to investigate a potential association between PBBT and tear film dysfunction. Additional clinical assessments included tear film break-up time (TBUT).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Statistically significant differences were observed in the average values of PBBT and TBUT between the examined groups. The average PBBT was 8.95 ± 5.38 s in the group with DED and 14.66 ± 10.50 s in the control group, <i>p</i> < 0.001. Group A exhibited an average TBUT of 4.77 ± 2.37 s, while Group B had a TBUT of 7.63 ± 3.25 s, <i>p</i> < 0.001. Additionally, a strong positive correlation was identified between PBBT and TBUT values (r = 0.455; <i>p</i> < 0.001).</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>The research confirms that tear film stability has an important role in the refraction of light and the maintenance of optical quality of vision. PBBT could potentially function as an objective and clinically significant screening test for DED.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10974998/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140294801","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Default Reference Frames for Angular Expansion in the Perception of Visual Direction. 视觉方向感知中角度扩展的默认参照框架
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-02-21 DOI: 10.3390/vision8010007
Prince U D Tardeh, Crystal Xu, Frank H Durgin

Prior work has shown that perceived angular elevation relative to a visible horizon/ground plane is exaggerated with a gain of about 1.5. Here, we investigated whether estimates of angular elevation remain exaggerated when no such visual gravitational reference is provided. This was investigated using a series of five experiments, with most using a novel apparatus to view a large field-of-view stereoscopic virtual environment while lying supine, looking straight up. Magnitude estimation methods were used as well as psychometric matches to internal standards with a total of 133 human participants. Generally, it was found that the exaggerated scaling of elevation seemed to be a default for 3D space, even if testing was performed in virtual environments that were nearly empty. Indeed, for supine observers, a strong exaggeration was found even for azimuthal judgments, which is consistent with the idea that, when looking upward, all deviations are in elevation. This suggests that the overarching gravitational frame often serves as a default reference frame.

先前的研究表明,相对于可见地平线/地平面的感知角仰角会被夸大,增益约为 1.5。在这里,我们研究了在没有这种视觉重力参照物的情况下,对角度仰角的估计是否仍然会被夸大。我们使用了一系列共五项实验来研究这一问题,其中大部分实验使用了一种新颖的仪器,让实验者仰卧直视大视场立体虚拟环境。实验中使用了幅度估计方法以及与内部标准相匹配的心理测量方法,共有 133 人参加了实验。一般来说,即使测试是在几乎空无一物的虚拟环境中进行的,夸张的仰角缩放似乎也是三维空间的默认设置。事实上,对于仰卧的观察者来说,即使是方位角的判断也会出现强烈的夸大,这与仰视时所有偏差都是仰角的观点是一致的。这表明,总体重力框架通常充当默认参考框架。
{"title":"Default Reference Frames for Angular Expansion in the Perception of Visual Direction.","authors":"Prince U D Tardeh, Crystal Xu, Frank H Durgin","doi":"10.3390/vision8010007","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision8010007","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior work has shown that perceived angular elevation relative to a visible horizon/ground plane is exaggerated with a gain of about 1.5. Here, we investigated whether estimates of angular elevation remain exaggerated when no such visual gravitational reference is provided. This was investigated using a series of five experiments, with most using a novel apparatus to view a large field-of-view stereoscopic virtual environment while lying supine, looking straight up. Magnitude estimation methods were used as well as psychometric matches to internal standards with a total of 133 human participants. Generally, it was found that the exaggerated scaling of elevation seemed to be a default for 3D space, even if testing was performed in virtual environments that were nearly empty. Indeed, for supine observers, a strong exaggeration was found even for azimuthal judgments, which is consistent with the idea that, when looking upward, all deviations are in elevation. This suggests that the overarching gravitational frame often serves as a default reference frame.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10976159/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140294800","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Kinematic Assessment of Fine Motor Skills in Children: Comparison of a Kinematic Approach and a Standardized Test. 儿童精细运动技能的运动学评估:运动学方法与标准化测试的比较。
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-02-17 DOI: 10.3390/vision8010006
Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo, Taylor A Brin, Benjamin Thompson, Lisa W T Christian

Deficits in fine motor skills have been reported in some children with neurodevelopmental disorders such as amblyopia or strabismus. Therefore, monitoring the development of motor skills and any potential improvement due to therapy is an important clinical goal. The aim of this study was to test the feasibility of performing a kinematic assessment within an optometric setting using inexpensive, portable, off-the-shelf equipment. The study also assessed whether kinematic data could enhance the information provided by a routine motor function screening test (the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, MABC). Using the MABC-2, upper limb dexterity was measured in a cohort of 47 typically developing children (7-15 years old), and the Leap motion capture system was used to record hand kinematics while children performed a bead-threading task. Two children with a history of amblyopia were also tested to explore the utility of a kinematic assessment in a clinical population. For the typically developing children, visual acuity and stereoacuity were within the normal range; however, the average standardized MABC-2 scores were lower than published norms. Comparing MABC-2 and kinematic measures in the two children with amblyopia revealed that both assessments provide convergent results and revealed deficits in fine motor control. In conclusion, kinematic assessment can augment standardized tests of fine motor skills in an optometric setting and may be useful for measuring visuomotor function and monitoring treatment outcomes in children with binocular vision anomalies.

据报道,一些患有弱视或斜视等神经发育障碍的儿童存在精细运动技能缺陷。因此,监测运动技能的发展以及治疗可能带来的改善是一项重要的临床目标。本研究旨在测试在验光环境中使用廉价、便携、现成设备进行运动学评估的可行性。研究还评估了运动学数据是否能增强常规运动功能筛查测试(儿童运动评估电池,MABC)所提供的信息。研究使用 MABC-2 对 47 名发育正常的儿童(7-15 岁)的上肢灵活性进行了测量,并使用 Leap 运动捕捉系统记录了儿童执行穿珠任务时的手部运动学数据。此外,还对两名有弱视病史的儿童进行了测试,以探索运动学评估在临床人群中的实用性。对于发育正常的儿童来说,视敏度和立体敏锐度都在正常范围内;但是,MABC-2 的平均标准化得分却低于已公布的标准值。对两名弱视儿童的 MABC-2 和运动学测量结果进行比较后发现,这两种评估结果具有趋同性,并显示出他们在精细运动控制方面存在缺陷。总之,运动学评估可以增强验光环境中精细运动技能的标准化测试,并可用于测量双眼视力异常儿童的视觉运动功能和监测治疗效果。
{"title":"Kinematic Assessment of Fine Motor Skills in Children: Comparison of a Kinematic Approach and a Standardized Test.","authors":"Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo, Taylor A Brin, Benjamin Thompson, Lisa W T Christian","doi":"10.3390/vision8010006","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision8010006","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Deficits in fine motor skills have been reported in some children with neurodevelopmental disorders such as amblyopia or strabismus. Therefore, monitoring the development of motor skills and any potential improvement due to therapy is an important clinical goal. The aim of this study was to test the feasibility of performing a kinematic assessment within an optometric setting using inexpensive, portable, off-the-shelf equipment. The study also assessed whether kinematic data could enhance the information provided by a routine motor function screening test (the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, MABC). Using the MABC-2, upper limb dexterity was measured in a cohort of 47 typically developing children (7-15 years old), and the Leap motion capture system was used to record hand kinematics while children performed a bead-threading task. Two children with a history of amblyopia were also tested to explore the utility of a kinematic assessment in a clinical population. For the typically developing children, visual acuity and stereoacuity were within the normal range; however, the average standardized MABC-2 scores were lower than published norms. Comparing MABC-2 and kinematic measures in the two children with amblyopia revealed that both assessments provide convergent results and revealed deficits in fine motor control. In conclusion, kinematic assessment can augment standardized tests of fine motor skills in an optometric setting and may be useful for measuring visuomotor function and monitoring treatment outcomes in children with binocular vision anomalies.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10885072/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mimicking Facial Expressions Facilitates Working Memory for Stimuli in Emotion-Congruent Colours. 模仿面部表情能促进对情绪一致颜色刺激的工作记忆
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2024-01-30 DOI: 10.3390/vision8010004
Thaatsha Sivananthan, Steven B Most, Kim M Curby

It is one thing for everyday phrases like "seeing red" to link some emotions with certain colours (e.g., anger with red), but can such links measurably bias information processing? We investigated whether emotional face information (angry/happy/neutral) held in visual working memory (VWM) enhances memory for shapes presented in a conceptually consistent colour (red or green) (Experiment 1). Although emotional information held in VWM appeared not to bias memory for coloured shapes in Experiment 1, exploratory analyses suggested that participants who physically mimicked the face stimuli were better at remembering congruently coloured shapes. Experiment 2 confirmed this finding by asking participants to hold the faces in mind while either mimicking or labelling the emotional expressions of face stimuli. Once again, those who mimicked the expressions were better at remembering shapes with emotion-congruent colours, whereas those who simply labelled them were not. Thus, emotion-colour associations appear powerful enough to guide attention, but-consistent with proposed impacts of "embodied emotion" on cognition-such effects emerged when emotion processing was facilitated through facial mimicry.

像 "看到红色 "这样的日常用语将某些情绪与某些颜色联系起来(例如,愤怒与红色)是一回事,但这种联系是否会对信息处理产生可测量的偏差呢?我们研究了视觉工作记忆(VWM)中的情绪面孔信息(愤怒/快乐/中性)是否会增强对以概念一致的颜色(红色或绿色)呈现的形状的记忆(实验 1)。尽管在实验 1 中,视觉工作记忆中的情绪信息似乎并不偏向对彩色形状的记忆,但探索性分析表明,身体模仿面部刺激的参与者更善于记忆颜色一致的形状。实验 2 通过要求被试在模仿或标注人脸刺激的情绪表情时牢记人脸,证实了这一发现。同样,那些模仿表情的人更善于记住带有情绪一致颜色的形状,而那些仅仅标注了表情的人则不然。因此,情绪与颜色的关联似乎足以引导注意力,但与所提出的 "具身情绪 "对认知的影响相一致的是,当通过面部模仿来促进情绪处理时,这种影响就会出现。
{"title":"Mimicking Facial Expressions Facilitates Working Memory for Stimuli in Emotion-Congruent Colours.","authors":"Thaatsha Sivananthan, Steven B Most, Kim M Curby","doi":"10.3390/vision8010004","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision8010004","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is one thing for everyday phrases like \"seeing red\" to link some emotions with certain colours (e.g., anger with red), but can such links measurably bias information processing? We investigated whether emotional face information (angry/happy/neutral) held in visual working memory (VWM) enhances memory for shapes presented in a conceptually consistent colour (red or green) (Experiment 1). Although emotional information held in VWM appeared not to bias memory for coloured shapes in Experiment 1, exploratory analyses suggested that participants who physically mimicked the face stimuli were better at remembering congruently coloured shapes. Experiment 2 confirmed this finding by asking participants to hold the faces in mind while either mimicking or labelling the emotional expressions of face stimuli. Once again, those who mimicked the expressions were better at remembering shapes with emotion-congruent colours, whereas those who simply labelled them were not. Thus, emotion-colour associations appear powerful enough to guide attention, but-consistent with proposed impacts of \"embodied emotion\" on cognition-such effects emerged when emotion processing was facilitated through facial mimicry.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"8 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10885052/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139933331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spatial Attention in Visual Working Memory Strengthens Feature-Location Binding. 视觉工作记忆中的空间注意加强了特征-位置绑定
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-12-18 DOI: 10.3390/vision7040079
Juyeon Joe, Min-Shik Kim

There is a debate about whether working memory (WM) representations are individual features or bound objects. While spatial attention is reported to play a significant role in feature binding, little is known about the role of spatial attention in WM. To address this gap, the current study required participants to maintain multiple items in their WM and employed a memory-driven attention capture paradigm. Spatial attention in WM was manipulated by presenting an exogenous cue at one of the locations that memory items had occupied. The effects of spatial attention on attention guidance in visual search (Experiment 1) and memory performance (Experiments 1 and 2) were explored. The results show that WM-driven attention guidance did not vary based on whether the search features came from the same object in WM; instead, it depended on the number of features, regardless of their source object. In memory tasks, the cued object outperformed the uncued object. Specifically, the test item was better rejected when the features were mis-bound in the cued location than in the uncued location. These findings suggest that memory-driven attention guidance is feature-based, and spatial attention in WM helps bind features into object structures based on location.

关于工作记忆(WM)表征是个体特征还是绑定对象的问题一直存在争议。据报道,空间注意在特征绑定中起着重要作用,但人们对空间注意在工作记忆中的作用却知之甚少。为了填补这一空白,本研究要求参与者在其 WM 中保持多个项目,并采用了记忆驱动的注意力捕捉范式。通过在记忆项目所占据的位置之一呈现外源线索来操纵 WM 中的空间注意。实验探讨了空间注意对视觉搜索(实验 1)和记忆表现(实验 1 和 2)中注意引导的影响。结果表明,WM 驱动的注意引导并不因搜索特征是否来自 WM 中的同一对象而变化;相反,它取决于特征的数量,而与特征的来源对象无关。在记忆任务中,提示对象的表现优于未提示对象。具体来说,当特征被错误地绑定在提示位置时,测试项目的拒绝率要好于未被提示的位置。这些发现表明,记忆驱动的注意引导是以特征为基础的,WM 中的空间注意有助于根据位置将特征绑定到对象结构中。
{"title":"Spatial Attention in Visual Working Memory Strengthens Feature-Location Binding.","authors":"Juyeon Joe, Min-Shik Kim","doi":"10.3390/vision7040079","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision7040079","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is a debate about whether working memory (WM) representations are individual features or bound objects. While spatial attention is reported to play a significant role in feature binding, little is known about the role of spatial attention in WM. To address this gap, the current study required participants to maintain multiple items in their WM and employed a memory-driven attention capture paradigm. Spatial attention in WM was manipulated by presenting an exogenous cue at one of the locations that memory items had occupied. The effects of spatial attention on attention guidance in visual search (Experiment 1) and memory performance (Experiments 1 and 2) were explored. The results show that WM-driven attention guidance did not vary based on whether the search features came from the same object in WM; instead, it depended on the number of features, regardless of their source object. In memory tasks, the cued object outperformed the uncued object. Specifically, the test item was better rejected when the features were mis-bound in the cued location than in the uncued location. These findings suggest that memory-driven attention guidance is feature-based, and spatial attention in WM helps bind features into object structures based on location.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"7 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10748131/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832018","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revealing the Concealed: Alternatives to Random Dots for Stereograms. 揭示隐藏的东西:立体图随机点的替代方案。
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-12-17 DOI: 10.3390/vision7040078
Nicholas J Wade

Investigations of stereoscopic depth perception were transformed via the use of computer-generated random-dot stereograms in the 1960s. They realized Wheatstone's wish of demonstrating binocular depth without monocular object recognition, and they have been the dominant stimulus for studying stereopsis since then. Alternative carrier patterns to random dots, based on graphics, photographs, and their combinations, are presented as anaglyphs and for free fusion. A wider range of concealed patterns can be revealed with these alternatives, and presenting them as anaglyphs can yield patterns that have visual appeal independent of the depth they conceal.

20 世纪 60 年代,计算机生成的随机点立体图改变了对立体深度知觉的研究。它们实现了惠斯通的愿望,即在不识别单眼物体的情况下演示双眼深度,自此它们成为研究立体视的主要刺激物。以图形、照片及其组合为基础的随机点的替代载体图案,以变形图和自由融合的形式呈现。使用这些替代图案可以显示出更广泛的隐藏图案,而且将它们作为变形图像显示可以产生与隐藏深度无关的具有视觉吸引力的图案。
{"title":"Revealing the Concealed: Alternatives to Random Dots for Stereograms.","authors":"Nicholas J Wade","doi":"10.3390/vision7040078","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision7040078","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Investigations of stereoscopic depth perception were transformed via the use of computer-generated random-dot stereograms in the 1960s. They realized Wheatstone's wish of demonstrating binocular depth without monocular object recognition, and they have been the dominant stimulus for studying stereopsis since then. Alternative carrier patterns to random dots, based on graphics, photographs, and their combinations, are presented as anaglyphs and for free fusion. A wider range of concealed patterns can be revealed with these alternatives, and presenting them as anaglyphs can yield patterns that have visual appeal independent of the depth they conceal.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"7 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10747603/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Brief Myofascial Intervention Modulates Visual Event-Related Potential Response to Emotional Photographic Contents: A Pilot Study. 简短的肌筋膜干预可调节对情绪化图片内容的视觉事件相关电位反应:试点研究。
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-12-13 DOI: 10.3390/vision7040077
Gabriel Byczynski, Amedeo D'Angiulli

The use of touch for the treatment of psychiatric disorders is increasingly investigated, as it is shown that cognitive symptoms can be improved by various forms of massage. To investigate if the effect of massage is measurable using classical visual event-related potential components (P1, P2, late positive potential (LPP)), we performed a preliminary study on six participants using myofascial induction massage. Participants were shown emotionally valenced or neutral images before and after a 20 min myofascial massage. We found general increases in P2 amplitude following the intervention across all conditions (both neutral and affective), indicating increased attention or salience to visual stimuli. The magnitude of change was visibly larger for unpleasant stimuli, suggesting that visual perception and attention were modulated specifically in response to unpleasant visual images. The LPP showed reductions in amplitude after myofascial massage, suggesting increased emotional modulation following intervention, as a result of possible DMN alterations, consistent with region and function. We conclude that brief myofascial intervention supports other research in the field, finding that physical touch and massage techniques can alter cognition and perception. We posit further research to investigate its future use as an intervention for both physical and cognitive modulation. Importantly, we provide preliminary evidence that the neural processes that resonate with this type of massage involve complex feedforward and backward cortical pathways, of which a significant portion participate in modulating the visual perception of external stimuli.

利用触摸治疗精神疾病的研究越来越多,因为各种形式的按摩都能改善认知症状。为了研究按摩的效果是否可以通过经典的视觉事件相关电位成分(P1、P2、晚期正电位(LPP))来测量,我们对六名参与者进行了肌筋膜诱导按摩的初步研究。在进行 20 分钟的肌筋膜按摩之前和之后,我们向参与者展示了具有情绪价值或中性的图像。我们发现,在所有条件下(包括中性和情感性),干预后 P2 振幅普遍增加,表明对视觉刺激的注意或显著性增加。不愉快刺激的变化幅度明显更大,这表明视觉感知和注意力在不愉快的视觉图像中受到了特别的调节。肌筋膜按摩后,LPP 的振幅减小,表明干预后情绪调节增强,这可能是 DMN 改变的结果,与区域和功能一致。我们的结论是,简短的肌筋膜干预支持该领域的其他研究,发现身体触摸和按摩技术可以改变认知和感知。我们建议开展进一步的研究,调查其作为一种干预措施在身体和认知调节方面的未来用途。重要的是,我们提供的初步证据表明,与这种按摩产生共鸣的神经过程涉及复杂的前馈和后向皮层通路,其中很大一部分参与调节对外部刺激的视觉感知。
{"title":"Brief Myofascial Intervention Modulates Visual Event-Related Potential Response to Emotional Photographic Contents: A Pilot Study.","authors":"Gabriel Byczynski, Amedeo D'Angiulli","doi":"10.3390/vision7040077","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision7040077","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The use of touch for the treatment of psychiatric disorders is increasingly investigated, as it is shown that cognitive symptoms can be improved by various forms of massage. To investigate if the effect of massage is measurable using classical visual event-related potential components (P1, P2, late positive potential (LPP)), we performed a preliminary study on six participants using myofascial induction massage. Participants were shown emotionally valenced or neutral images before and after a 20 min myofascial massage. We found general increases in P2 amplitude following the intervention across all conditions (both neutral and affective), indicating increased attention or salience to visual stimuli. The magnitude of change was visibly larger for unpleasant stimuli, suggesting that visual perception and attention were modulated specifically in response to unpleasant visual images. The LPP showed reductions in amplitude after myofascial massage, suggesting increased emotional modulation following intervention, as a result of possible DMN alterations, consistent with region and function. We conclude that brief myofascial intervention supports other research in the field, finding that physical touch and massage techniques can alter cognition and perception. We posit further research to investigate its future use as an intervention for both physical and cognitive modulation. Importantly, we provide preliminary evidence that the neural processes that resonate with this type of massage involve complex feedforward and backward cortical pathways, of which a significant portion participate in modulating the visual perception of external stimuli.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"7 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10748312/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832016","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measuring the Contributions of Perceptual and Attentional Processes in the Complete Composite Face Paradigm. 测量知觉和注意过程在完全复合面孔范式中的贡献。
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-17 DOI: 10.3390/vision7040076
William Blake Erickson, Dawn R Weatherford

Theories of holistic face processing vary widely with respect to conceptualizations, paradigms, and stimuli. These divergences have left several theoretical questions unresolved. Namely, the role of attention in face perception is understudied. To rectify this gap in the literature, we combined the complete composite face task (allowing for predictions of multiple theoretical conceptualizations and connecting with a large body of research) with a secondary auditory discrimination task at encoding (to avoid a visual perceptual bottleneck). Participants studied upright, intact faces within a continuous recognition paradigm, which intermixes study and test trials at multiple retention intervals. Within subjects, participants studied faces under full or divided attention. Test faces varied with respect to alignment, congruence, and retention intervals. Overall, we observed the predicted beneficial outcomes of holistic processing (e.g., higher discriminability for Congruent, Aligned faces relative to Congruent, Misaligned faces) that persisted across retention intervals and attention. However, we did not observe the predicted detrimental outcomes of holistic processing (e.g., higher discriminability for Incongruent, Misaligned faces relative to Incongruent, Aligned faces). Because the continuous recognition paradigm exerts particularly strong demands on attention, we interpret these findings through the lens of resource dependency and domain specificity.

整体面部加工理论在概念化、范式和刺激方面差异很大。这些分歧留下了几个未解决的理论问题。也就是说,注意在面部知觉中的作用尚未得到充分的研究。为了弥补文献中的这一空白,我们将完整的复合面部任务(允许多种理论概念的预测并与大量研究相联系)与编码时的次级听觉辨别任务(避免视觉感知瓶颈)结合起来。参与者在连续识别范式中研究直立的、完整的面孔,该范式在多个保留间隔中混合了研究和测试试验。在研究对象中,参与者在全神贯注或分散注意力的情况下研究面部。测试面在对齐、一致性和保留间隔方面有所不同。总体而言,我们观察到整体加工的预期有益结果(例如,一致性、对齐的面孔相对于一致性、不对齐的面孔具有更高的可辨别性)在保留间隔和注意力中持续存在。然而,我们没有观察到整体加工的预期有害结果(例如,相对于不一致、对齐的面孔,不一致、不对齐的面孔具有更高的可辨别性)。由于连续识别范式对注意力的要求特别强烈,我们从资源依赖和领域特异性的角度来解释这些发现。
{"title":"Measuring the Contributions of Perceptual and Attentional Processes in the Complete Composite Face Paradigm.","authors":"William Blake Erickson, Dawn R Weatherford","doi":"10.3390/vision7040076","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision7040076","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Theories of holistic face processing vary widely with respect to conceptualizations, paradigms, and stimuli. These divergences have left several theoretical questions unresolved. Namely, the role of attention in face perception is understudied. To rectify this gap in the literature, we combined the complete composite face task (allowing for predictions of multiple theoretical conceptualizations and connecting with a large body of research) with a secondary auditory discrimination task at encoding (to avoid a visual perceptual bottleneck). Participants studied upright, intact faces within a continuous recognition paradigm, which intermixes study and test trials at multiple retention intervals. Within subjects, participants studied faces under full or divided attention. Test faces varied with respect to alignment, congruence, and retention intervals. Overall, we observed the predicted beneficial outcomes of holistic processing (e.g., higher discriminability for Congruent, Aligned faces relative to Congruent, Misaligned faces) that persisted across retention intervals and attention. However, we did not observe the predicted detrimental outcomes of holistic processing (e.g., higher discriminability for Incongruent, Misaligned faces relative to Incongruent, Aligned faces). Because the continuous recognition paradigm exerts particularly strong demands on attention, we interpret these findings through the lens of resource dependency and domain specificity.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"7 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10661262/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138177488","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Repulsive Aftereffects of Visual Space. 视觉空间的排斥效应。
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-15 DOI: 10.3390/vision7040073
Eckart Zimmermann

Prolonged exposure to a sensory stimulus induces perceptual adaptation aftereffects. Traditionally, aftereffects are known to change the appearance of stimulus features, like contrast, color, or shape. However, shifts in the spatial position of objects have also been observed to follow adaptation. Here, I demonstrate that visual adaptation produced by different adapter stimuli generates a bi-directional spatial repulsion. Observers had to judge the distance between a probe dot pair presented in the adapted region and compare them to a reference dot pair presented in a region not affected by adaptation. If the probe dot pair was present inside the adapted area, observers underestimated the distance. If, however, the dot pair straddled the adapted area, the distance was perceived as larger with a stronger distance expansion than compression. Bi-directional spatial repulsion was found with a similar magnitude for size and density adapters. Localization estimates with mouse pointing revealed that adaptation also affected absolute position judgments. Bi-directional spatial repulsion is most likely produced by the lines of adapter stimuli since single bars used as adapters were sufficient to induce spatial repulsion. Spatial repulsion was stronger for stimuli presented in the periphery. This finding explains why distance expansion is stronger than distance compression.

长时间暴露于感官刺激会引起知觉适应的后效。传统上,后遗症被认为是改变刺激特征的外观,如对比度、颜色或形状。然而,物体空间位置的变化也被观察到遵循适应。在这里,我证明了不同的适配器刺激产生的视觉适应产生双向空间排斥。观察者必须判断在适应区域中呈现的探测点对之间的距离,并将它们与未受适应影响的区域中呈现的参考点对进行比较。如果探测点对出现在适应区域内,观察者会低估距离。然而,如果点对横跨适应区域,则距离被认为更大,距离扩展比压缩更强。双向空间排斥被发现具有类似的大小和密度适配器。用鼠标指向的定位估计表明,适应也影响绝对位置判断。双向空间斥力最有可能是由适配器刺激线产生的,因为用作适配器的单根棒足以引起空间斥力。外周刺激的空间斥力更强。这一发现解释了为什么距离膨胀强于距离压缩。
{"title":"Repulsive Aftereffects of Visual Space.","authors":"Eckart Zimmermann","doi":"10.3390/vision7040073","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision7040073","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prolonged exposure to a sensory stimulus induces perceptual adaptation aftereffects. Traditionally, aftereffects are known to change the appearance of stimulus features, like contrast, color, or shape. However, shifts in the spatial position of objects have also been observed to follow adaptation. Here, I demonstrate that visual adaptation produced by different adapter stimuli generates a bi-directional spatial repulsion. Observers had to judge the distance between a probe dot pair presented in the adapted region and compare them to a reference dot pair presented in a region not affected by adaptation. If the probe dot pair was present inside the adapted area, observers underestimated the distance. If, however, the dot pair straddled the adapted area, the distance was perceived as larger with a stronger distance expansion than compression. Bi-directional spatial repulsion was found with a similar magnitude for size and density adapters. Localization estimates with mouse pointing revealed that adaptation also affected absolute position judgments. Bi-directional spatial repulsion is most likely produced by the lines of adapter stimuli since single bars used as adapters were sufficient to induce spatial repulsion. Spatial repulsion was stronger for stimuli presented in the periphery. This finding explains why distance expansion is stronger than distance compression.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"7 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10661249/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138177489","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Utilization of Mirror Visual Feedback for Upper Limb Function in Poststroke Patients: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. 利用镜像视觉反馈改善脑卒中后患者上肢功能:一项系统回顾和荟萃分析。
Q2 Medicine Pub Date : 2023-11-15 DOI: 10.3390/vision7040075
Hyunjoong Kim, Eunsang Lee, Jihye Jung, Seungwon Lee

Mirror visual feedback (MVF), a noninvasive treatment method, is attracting attention as a possibility to promote the recovery of upper limb function in stroke patients. However, the cognitive effects of this therapy have received limited attention in the existing literature. To address this gap, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to investigate the relationship between upper limb function and cognition in stroke patients and to evaluate the effect of MVF on improving upper limb function. A comprehensive search was performed on the Embase, MEDLINE, and PubMed databases to identify original articles and clinical studies published between 2013 and 2022. Qualitative analysis was performed using the Cochrane Risk of Bias tool, and in the quantitative analysis, a random-effects model was used as the effect model, and standard mean difference (SMD) was used as the effect measure. Eight studies that met the inclusion criteria were entered in the analysis. Data extraction included an assessment tool for upper extremity function. Results of the quantitative analysis demonstrate that MVF was effective in improving upper extremity function in stroke patients (SMD = 0.94, 95% CI 0.69 to 1.20). In conclusion, this systematic review and meta-analysis provides evidence supporting the effectiveness of MVF in improving upper limb function in stroke patients. However, further studies are needed to investigate the cognitive effects of MVF and elucidate the underlying mechanisms.

镜像视觉反馈(Mirror visual feedback, MVF)作为一种无创治疗方法,有望促进脑卒中患者上肢功能的恢复,正受到人们的关注。然而,这种疗法的认知效果在现有文献中受到的关注有限。为了弥补这一空白,我们进行了系统回顾和荟萃分析,探讨脑卒中患者上肢功能与认知的关系,并评估MVF对改善上肢功能的作用。对Embase、MEDLINE和PubMed数据库进行了全面搜索,以确定2013年至2022年间发表的原始文章和临床研究。采用Cochrane Risk of Bias工具进行定性分析,定量分析采用随机效应模型作为效应模型,标准差(standard mean difference, SMD)作为效应测度。符合纳入标准的8项研究被纳入分析。数据提取包括上肢功能评估工具。定量分析结果表明,MVF可有效改善脑卒中患者上肢功能(SMD = 0.94, 95% CI 0.69 ~ 1.20)。总之,本系统综述和荟萃分析为MVF改善脑卒中患者上肢功能的有效性提供了证据。然而,MVF对认知的影响和潜在的机制还需要进一步的研究。
{"title":"Utilization of Mirror Visual Feedback for Upper Limb Function in Poststroke Patients: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis.","authors":"Hyunjoong Kim, Eunsang Lee, Jihye Jung, Seungwon Lee","doi":"10.3390/vision7040075","DOIUrl":"10.3390/vision7040075","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mirror visual feedback (MVF), a noninvasive treatment method, is attracting attention as a possibility to promote the recovery of upper limb function in stroke patients. However, the cognitive effects of this therapy have received limited attention in the existing literature. To address this gap, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis to investigate the relationship between upper limb function and cognition in stroke patients and to evaluate the effect of MVF on improving upper limb function. A comprehensive search was performed on the Embase, MEDLINE, and PubMed databases to identify original articles and clinical studies published between 2013 and 2022. Qualitative analysis was performed using the Cochrane Risk of Bias tool, and in the quantitative analysis, a random-effects model was used as the effect model, and standard mean difference (SMD) was used as the effect measure. Eight studies that met the inclusion criteria were entered in the analysis. Data extraction included an assessment tool for upper extremity function. Results of the quantitative analysis demonstrate that MVF was effective in improving upper extremity function in stroke patients (SMD = 0.94, 95% CI 0.69 to 1.20). In conclusion, this systematic review and meta-analysis provides evidence supporting the effectiveness of MVF in improving upper limb function in stroke patients. However, further studies are needed to investigate the cognitive effects of MVF and elucidate the underlying mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":36586,"journal":{"name":"Vision (Switzerland)","volume":"7 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10661272/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138177491","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Vision (Switzerland)
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1