首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity最新文献

英文 中文
Mental Practice in the Intermanual Transfer of Motor Skills 手间运动技能转移中的心理练习
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2010-09-18 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1052
Keith R. Lohse, Alice F. Healy, David Sherwood
The current study compared intermanual transfer for two different handwriting tasks (familiar letters and novel symbols), following both mental and physical practice. There was substantial transfer from practice with the dominant hand to the nondominant one in both time to produce a character and size of the character produced, but there was no transfer in the reverse direction (even for novel symbols). Most importantly, there was significant transfer as a result of mental practice in production time comparable to physical practice. However, there was no transfer from mental practice when measuring character size. During mental practice, task-level variables still had significant effects whereas effector-level variables did not. Thus, asymmetrical transfer as a result of mental practice is posited to result from the transfer of task-level processes but not effector-level processes.
目前的研究比较了两种不同的书写任务(熟悉的字母和新的符号)的手间传递,同时进行了心理和身体练习。在产生一个字符的时间和字符的大小上,从惯用手到非惯用手的练习都有大量的转移,但在相反的方向上没有转移(即使是新的符号)。最重要的是,与体力练习相比,脑力练习在生产时间上产生了显著的转移。然而,在测量字符大小时,没有从心理练习中转移。在心理练习过程中,任务水平变量仍有显著影响,而效应水平变量则无显著影响。因此,不对称迁移作为心理实践的结果被假定为来自任务级过程的迁移,而不是效应级过程的迁移。
{"title":"Mental Practice in the Intermanual Transfer of Motor Skills","authors":"Keith R. Lohse, Alice F. Healy, David Sherwood","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1052","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1052","url":null,"abstract":"The current study compared intermanual transfer for two different handwriting tasks (familiar letters and novel symbols), following both mental and physical practice. There was substantial transfer from practice with the dominant hand to the nondominant one in both time to produce a character and size of the character produced, but there was no transfer in the reverse direction (even for novel symbols). Most importantly, there was significant transfer as a result of mental practice in production time comparable to physical practice. However, there was no transfer from mental practice when measuring character size. During mental practice, task-level variables still had significant effects whereas effector-level variables did not. Thus, asymmetrical transfer as a result of mental practice is posited to result from the transfer of task-level processes but not effector-level processes.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1052","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 16
Associative and Dissociative Imagery Effects on Perceived Exertion and Task Duration 联想和分离意象对知觉劳动和任务持续时间的影响
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2010-07-01 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1044
S. Razon, Itay Basevitch, Edson Filho, William M. Land, Brooke Thompson, M. Biermann, G. Tenenbaum
The study was designed to examine the effects of associative and dissociative imagery interventions on reported ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) and task-duration across a handgrip-squeezing task. Sixty adults (Mage = 22.19 years) were randomly assigned to three groups: associative imagery, dissociative imagery, and control (non-imagery). Participants were instructed to perform a 30% maximal handgrip-squeezing task until volitional fatigue. During the squeezing task, RPE and attention allocation were measured every 30 seconds. A series of RM MANOVA procedures revealed that (a) RPE increased linearly across all three groups as a function of increased effort output, (b) as compared to control participants, RPE was lower in participants using either associative or dissociative imagery, and (c) as compared to control participants, participants using either imagery remained longer on task. While the effects of imagery use on RPE and task-duration were descriptively evident, not all effects were significant. Future studies must examine imagery applications for tasks that vary in workload intensities. Findings shed light on interventions that can possibly render exercise experience more pleasant and less exertive for the general population.
该研究旨在检验联想和分离意象干预对握力任务中感知用力(RPE)评分和任务持续时间的影响。60名成年人(年龄22.19岁)被随机分为三组:联想意象组、分离意象组和对照组(非意象组)。参与者被要求执行30%的最大握力任务,直到意志疲劳。在挤压过程中,每隔30秒测量一次RPE和注意力分配。一系列RM -方差分析程序显示:(A) RPE随努力输出的增加而线性增加,(b)与对照组相比,使用联想意象或分离意象的参与者的RPE较低,(c)与对照组相比,使用任何一种意象的参与者在任务上停留的时间更长。虽然图像使用对RPE和任务持续时间的影响是描述性的,但并非所有的影响都是显著的。未来的研究必须检查图像应用于不同工作量强度的任务。研究结果揭示了一些干预措施,这些干预措施可能会让普通人群的运动体验更愉快,更少费力。
{"title":"Associative and Dissociative Imagery Effects on Perceived Exertion and Task Duration","authors":"S. Razon, Itay Basevitch, Edson Filho, William M. Land, Brooke Thompson, M. Biermann, G. Tenenbaum","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1044","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1044","url":null,"abstract":"The study was designed to examine the effects of associative and dissociative imagery interventions on reported ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) and task-duration across a handgrip-squeezing task. Sixty adults (Mage = 22.19 years) were randomly assigned to three groups: associative imagery, dissociative imagery, and control (non-imagery). Participants were instructed to perform a 30% maximal handgrip-squeezing task until volitional fatigue. During the squeezing task, RPE and attention allocation were measured every 30 seconds. A series of RM MANOVA procedures revealed that (a) RPE increased linearly across all three groups as a function of increased effort output, (b) as compared to control participants, RPE was lower in participants using either associative or dissociative imagery, and (c) as compared to control participants, participants using either imagery remained longer on task. While the effects of imagery use on RPE and task-duration were descriptively evident, not all effects were significant. Future studies must examine imagery applications for tasks that vary in workload intensities. Findings shed light on interventions that can possibly render exercise experience more pleasant and less exertive for the general population.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1044","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732728","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 25
Mental Imagery Research in Physical Education 体育教学中的心理意象研究
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2010-03-26 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1045
N. Hall, G. Fishburne
The purpose of this article is to present a framework for researching mental imagery use in school-based physical education. Mental imagery use has been researched quite extensively over the past 50 years in the areas of motor learning and sport psychology. Much has been learned regarding the benefits of using mental imagery to improve learning and performance. This article has drawn parallels between school-based physical education and the areas of sport psychology and motor learning. Based on findings in the areas of motor learning and sport psychology, and the similarities drawn between school-based physical education and these two areas, it could be expected that mental imagery use in school-based physical education has the potential to produce many benefits in learning and performance for both students and teachers. However, to date there has been very little research conducted with regard to mental imagery use and physical education. This article provides a research framework identifying questions that need to be addressed in order to more fully understand the potential imagery use has for both students and teachers in school-based physical education.
本文的目的是提出一个研究心理意象在校本体育教学中的应用的框架。在过去的50年里,心理意象的使用在运动学习和运动心理学领域得到了相当广泛的研究。关于使用心理意象来提高学习和表现的好处,人们已经了解了很多。这篇文章将学校体育教育与运动心理学和运动学习领域进行了比较。根据运动学习和运动心理学领域的研究结果,以及校本体育与这两个领域之间的相似性,可以预期,在校本体育中使用心理意象对学生和教师的学习和表现都有可能产生许多好处。然而,到目前为止,关于心理意象的使用和体育教育的研究很少。本文提供了一个研究框架,确定了需要解决的问题,以便更充分地了解学校体育教学中学生和教师使用图像的潜在影响。
{"title":"Mental Imagery Research in Physical Education","authors":"N. Hall, G. Fishburne","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1045","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1045","url":null,"abstract":"The purpose of this article is to present a framework for researching mental imagery use in school-based physical education. Mental imagery use has been researched quite extensively over the past 50 years in the areas of motor learning and sport psychology. Much has been learned regarding the benefits of using mental imagery to improve learning and performance. This article has drawn parallels between school-based physical education and the areas of sport psychology and motor learning. Based on findings in the areas of motor learning and sport psychology, and the similarities drawn between school-based physical education and these two areas, it could be expected that mental imagery use in school-based physical education has the potential to produce many benefits in learning and performance for both students and teachers. However, to date there has been very little research conducted with regard to mental imagery use and physical education. This article provides a research framework identifying questions that need to be addressed in order to more fully understand the potential imagery use has for both students and teachers in school-based physical education.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1045","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732753","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 16
A Field Test of the Influence of Pre-Game Imagery on Basketball Free Throw Shooting 赛前意象对篮球罚球命中率影响的现场试验
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2010-01-25 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1042
Phillip G Post, C. Wrisberg, Stephen Mullins
This field study examined the influence of a pre-game imagery intervention implemented by a high school coach on the free throw shooting performance of his girls' basketball team. The coach conducted a systematic guided imagery exercise prior to half of the team's games over the course of an entire season. The coach employed the intervention with the whole team using a reversal design similar to the replication-reversal design used in some single-subject research (Hume, Martin, Gonzalez, Cracklen, & Genthon, 1985). A 2 x 2 chi-square analysis was used to evaluate the observed and expected frequencies of made and missed free throw shots under the two conditions (imagery and no-imagery). The results revealed a significantly higher than expected number of free throws made in games preceded by the intervention (p < .001). Social validation questionnaires completed by 11 of the 16 players suggested that the players devoted significant attention to the intervention and that they perceived it to be effective.
本研究考察了一名高中教练在赛前意象干预下对女子篮球队罚球表现的影响。在整个赛季中,教练在球队一半的比赛前进行了系统的引导意象练习。教练采用与一些单受试者研究中使用的复制逆转设计相似的逆转设计对整个团队进行干预(Hume, Martin, Gonzalez, Cracklen, & Genthon, 1985)。采用2 x 2卡方分析来评估两种情况下(图像和无图像)罚球投中和失球的观察频率和预期频率。结果显示,在干预前的比赛中,罚球次数明显高于预期(p < 0.001)。16名参与者中有11名完成了社会验证问卷,这表明参与者对干预非常关注,并且他们认为干预是有效的。
{"title":"A Field Test of the Influence of Pre-Game Imagery on Basketball Free Throw Shooting","authors":"Phillip G Post, C. Wrisberg, Stephen Mullins","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1042","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1042","url":null,"abstract":"This field study examined the influence of a pre-game imagery intervention implemented by a high school coach on the free throw shooting performance of his girls' basketball team. The coach conducted a systematic guided imagery exercise prior to half of the team's games over the course of an entire season. The coach employed the intervention with the whole team using a reversal design similar to the replication-reversal design used in some single-subject research (Hume, Martin, Gonzalez, Cracklen, & Genthon, 1985). A 2 x 2 chi-square analysis was used to evaluate the observed and expected frequencies of made and missed free throw shots under the two conditions (imagery and no-imagery). The results revealed a significantly higher than expected number of free throws made in games preceded by the intervention (p < .001). Social validation questionnaires completed by 11 of the 16 players suggested that the players devoted significant attention to the intervention and that they perceived it to be effective.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1042","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732219","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 28
The Effects of Physical Practice on a Novel Mental Imagery Task as Measured by Electroencephalography (EEG) 脑电测量的体育锻炼对一种新型心理意象任务的影响
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2010-01-21 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1050
J. Gualberto Cremades, Tamesha M Hilliard, J. Gapin
Studies have shown that the use of mental imagery can improve performance (Mamassis & Doganis, 2004; Martin, Mortitz, & Hall, 1999). However, these studies only use outcome measures to show the benefits of mental imagery/practice. Researchers have not yet examined the underlying cognitive processes of imagery performance as individuals physically learn a new skill. The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of physical practice on EEG activity when imagining a dart throwing task. EEG activity from P4, P3, O2, and O1 was recorded from 30 volunteers. Participants were randomly assigned to either a physical practice condition or a non-practice condition. Ten two-second epochs were recorded and analyzed for both the pre-test and post-test. A separate 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 (Hemisphere x Sites x Test x Practice) mixed model design was conducted for each EEG frequency band (i.e., lower alpha, upper alpha, lower beta, and upper beta). Results revealed a significant interaction test by practice in the lower alpha band (p > .05). Further, there was a significant interaction test by site (p < .05) in the lower alpha band. Lastly, a significant interaction site by hemisphere was shown in the lower beta band (p < .05). The results suggest that either the task was not complex enough to detect differences or the participants did not have adequate physical practice. In addition, the occipital sites were less involved in attention and effort during the post-test imagery performance while the parietal sites were as involved as the pre-test. The imagery script used for this study emphasized feeling the movement prior to imagery performance, and this may have resulted in the parietal region being activated during the pre- and post-test. However, the occipital region at the left hemisphere was more stimulated than the right hemisphere, suggesting the cognitive processing of imagery instructions.
研究表明,使用心理意象可以提高表现(Mamassis & Doganis, 2004;Martin, Mortitz, & Hall, 1999)。然而,这些研究只使用结果测量来显示心理意象/练习的好处。研究人员还没有研究过个体在学习一项新技能时意象表现的潜在认知过程。本研究的目的是确定体育锻炼对想象投掷飞镖任务时脑电图活动的影响。记录30名志愿者P4、P3、O2、O1脑电活动。参与者被随机分配到身体练习组和非练习组。记录和分析前测和后测的10个2秒epoch。对每个EEG频带(即下α、上α、下β和上β)进行单独的2 × 2 × 2 × 2(半球x部位x测试x实践)混合模型设计。结果显示,在实践中,下α波段的相互作用测试显著(p < 0.05)。此外,在低α波段存在显著的位点交互作用检验(p < 0.05)。最后,低β波段显示了显著的半球相互作用位点(p < 0.05)。结果表明,要么任务不够复杂,无法发现差异,要么参与者没有进行足够的身体练习。此外,枕区在测试后的图像表现中对注意力和努力的参与程度较低,而顶叶区在测试前的图像表现中对注意力和努力的参与程度相同。本研究使用的想象脚本强调在想象表现之前感觉运动,这可能导致顶叶区域在测试前后被激活。然而,左半球枕区比右半球受到更大的刺激,表明对图像指令的认知处理。
{"title":"The Effects of Physical Practice on a Novel Mental Imagery Task as Measured by Electroencephalography (EEG)","authors":"J. Gualberto Cremades, Tamesha M Hilliard, J. Gapin","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1050","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1050","url":null,"abstract":"Studies have shown that the use of mental imagery can improve performance (Mamassis & Doganis, 2004; Martin, Mortitz, & Hall, 1999). However, these studies only use outcome measures to show the benefits of mental imagery/practice. Researchers have not yet examined the underlying cognitive processes of imagery performance as individuals physically learn a new skill. The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of physical practice on EEG activity when imagining a dart throwing task. EEG activity from P4, P3, O2, and O1 was recorded from 30 volunteers. Participants were randomly assigned to either a physical practice condition or a non-practice condition. Ten two-second epochs were recorded and analyzed for both the pre-test and post-test. A separate 2 x 2 x 2 x 2 (Hemisphere x Sites x Test x Practice) mixed model design was conducted for each EEG frequency band (i.e., lower alpha, upper alpha, lower beta, and upper beta). Results revealed a significant interaction test by practice in the lower alpha band (p > .05). Further, there was a significant interaction test by site (p < .05) in the lower alpha band. Lastly, a significant interaction site by hemisphere was shown in the lower beta band (p < .05). The results suggest that either the task was not complex enough to detect differences or the participants did not have adequate physical practice. In addition, the occipital sites were less involved in attention and effort during the post-test imagery performance while the parietal sites were as involved as the pre-test. The imagery script used for this study emphasized feeling the movement prior to imagery performance, and this may have resulted in the parietal region being activated during the pre- and post-test. However, the occipital region at the left hemisphere was more stimulated than the right hemisphere, suggesting the cognitive processing of imagery instructions.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1050","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732351","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
A Narrative Review of the Relationships among Imagery, Exercise, and Self-Efficacy 意象、运动与自我效能感之关系述评
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2010-01-19 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1051
Lindsay Ross-Stewart, Sandra E Short, Cheryl A. Terrance
We all know physical activity is an important part of a healthy lifestyle, yet many people still live sedentary lifestyles. It is, therefore, important to develop theoretically based interventions that may serve to increase physical activity and exercise. This narrative review focuses on the relationships among imagery, self-efficacy and exercise, and makes a case for future researchers to investigate the effectiveness of exercise imagery interventions.
我们都知道体育活动是健康生活方式的重要组成部分,然而许多人仍然过着久坐不动的生活方式。因此,重要的是开发基于理论的干预措施,可能有助于增加身体活动和锻炼。本文对运动意象、自我效能感和运动之间的关系进行了综述,并为未来研究运动意象干预的有效性提供了依据。
{"title":"A Narrative Review of the Relationships among Imagery, Exercise, and Self-Efficacy","authors":"Lindsay Ross-Stewart, Sandra E Short, Cheryl A. Terrance","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1051","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1051","url":null,"abstract":"We all know physical activity is an important part of a healthy lifestyle, yet many people still live sedentary lifestyles. It is, therefore, important to develop theoretically based interventions that may serve to increase physical activity and exercise. This narrative review focuses on the relationships among imagery, self-efficacy and exercise, and makes a case for future researchers to investigate the effectiveness of exercise imagery interventions.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1051","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732381","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Paradoxical-Success Imagery in the Field 矛盾的成功意象在现场
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2010-01-07 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1046
Derek de la Peña, Anselm Khoo, Nicholas P Murray
In the current study, 27 male club-soccer players were trained with paradoxical-success imagery (de la Peña, 2009) in the field. Based on the tenets of Wegner's (1994) theory of ironic processes of mental control and previous research (e.g., de la Peña, 2009; Mellalieu et al., 2009), it was predicted that participants who imaged experiencing anxiety-related symptoms (combined with additional substantiated imagery components) prior to competing, would show a positive increase in self-confidence and interpretation of anxiety-related symptoms following training compared to pre-treatment. Hypotheses were supported in spite of an initial increase in cognitive anxiety intensity immediately following treatment. Additionally, results suggest that paradoxical-success imagery may be best suited for individuals with trait anxiety and the propensity to interpret anxiety-related symptoms as debilitative to performance. Factors contributing to the effective use of the imagery strategy as well as potential drawbacks are discussed.
在目前的研究中,27名男性俱乐部足球运动员接受了悖论成功意象的训练(de la Peña, 2009)。基于Wegner(1994)的精神控制的反讽过程理论的原则和先前的研究(例如,de la Peña, 2009;Mellalieu et al., 2009),据预测,在比赛前想象经历焦虑相关症状(结合额外的证实的图像成分)的参与者,在训练后与治疗前相比,会表现出自信和对焦虑相关症状的解释的积极增加。尽管在治疗后认知焦虑强度立即增加,但假设得到了支持。此外,结果表明,矛盾成功意象可能最适合具有特质焦虑的个体,并且倾向于将焦虑相关症状解释为衰弱的表现。讨论了影响意象策略有效使用的因素以及潜在的缺点。
{"title":"Paradoxical-Success Imagery in the Field","authors":"Derek de la Peña, Anselm Khoo, Nicholas P Murray","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1046","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1046","url":null,"abstract":"In the current study, 27 male club-soccer players were trained with paradoxical-success imagery (de la Peña, 2009) in the field. Based on the tenets of Wegner's (1994) theory of ironic processes of mental control and previous research (e.g., de la Peña, 2009; Mellalieu et al., 2009), it was predicted that participants who imaged experiencing anxiety-related symptoms (combined with additional substantiated imagery components) prior to competing, would show a positive increase in self-confidence and interpretation of anxiety-related symptoms following training compared to pre-treatment. Hypotheses were supported in spite of an initial increase in cognitive anxiety intensity immediately following treatment. Additionally, results suggest that paradoxical-success imagery may be best suited for individuals with trait anxiety and the propensity to interpret anxiety-related symptoms as debilitative to performance. Factors contributing to the effective use of the imagery strategy as well as potential drawbacks are discussed.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2010-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1046","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychometric Properties of the Revised Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-R) 修正运动意象问卷(MIQ-R)的心理测量特性
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2009-11-05 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1027
Eva V Monsma, Sandra E Short, Craig R. Hall, Melanie Gregg, Phil Sullivan
The revised Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-R: Hall & Martin, 1997) assesses visual and kinesthetic movement imagery abilities and has become a popular questionnaire in sport and exercise psychology research. The purpose of this study was to examine the psychometric properties of the MIQ-R including internal consistency, stability, factor structure, and gender invariance. There were 325 male (n = 136) and female (n = 189) athletes/dancers who participated in the study. Gender-specific internal consistency coefficients ranged from .83 to .89 and the test-retest reliabilities over a one week period for the subscales were .80 for visual and .81 for kinesthetic imagery abilities. The best model fit from confirmatory factor analyses results was for a 2-factor structure with an interrelationship between the subscales. These results support the finding that visual and kinesthetic imagery abilities are separate but related constructs. An examination of invariance by gender indicated that the model fit females better than males.
修订后的运动意象问卷(MIQ-R: Hall & Martin, 1997)评估视觉和动觉运动意象能力,已成为运动和运动心理学研究中流行的问卷。本研究的目的是检验智商测试的内部一致性、稳定性、因素结构和性别不变性等心理测量特性。共有325名男性(n = 136)和女性(n = 189)运动员/舞者参与了这项研究。不同性别的内部一致性系数范围为0.83 ~ 0.89,在一周时间内,视觉分量表的重测信度为0.80,动觉意象能力的重测信度为0.81。验证性因子分析结果的最佳模型拟合是两个因子结构,各分量表之间存在相互关系。这些结果支持了视觉和动觉意象能力是独立但相关的结构的发现。对性别不变性的检验表明,该模型比男性更适合女性。
{"title":"Psychometric Properties of the Revised Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-R)","authors":"Eva V Monsma, Sandra E Short, Craig R. Hall, Melanie Gregg, Phil Sullivan","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1027","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1027","url":null,"abstract":"The revised Movement Imagery Questionnaire (MIQ-R: Hall & Martin, 1997) assesses visual and kinesthetic movement imagery abilities and has become a popular questionnaire in sport and exercise psychology research. The purpose of this study was to examine the psychometric properties of the MIQ-R including internal consistency, stability, factor structure, and gender invariance. There were 325 male (n = 136) and female (n = 189) athletes/dancers who participated in the study. Gender-specific internal consistency coefficients ranged from .83 to .89 and the test-retest reliabilities over a one week period for the subscales were .80 for visual and .81 for kinesthetic imagery abilities. The best model fit from confirmatory factor analyses results was for a 2-factor structure with an interrelationship between the subscales. These results support the finding that visual and kinesthetic imagery abilities are separate but related constructs. An examination of invariance by gender indicated that the model fit females better than males.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2009-11-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1027","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68731427","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 41
Impact of Differing Frequencies of PETTLEP Imagery on Netball Shooting Performance 不同频率petttlep图像对无板篮球投篮性能的影响
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2009-10-20 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1043
C. Wakefield, Dave Smith
This study examined the effects of differing frequencies of PETTLEP imagery on netball shooting performance. Thirty-two female participants were divided into four groups: PETTLEP imagery once per week (1x/wk), PETTLEP imagery twice per week (2x/wk), PETTLEP imagery three times per week (3x/wk) and a control. During the pre-test and post-test participants were required to complete a total of twenty shots from five different points within the shooting zone. They were awarded points (out of five) for each shot, giving a possible total of 100 points. The numbers of shots scored was also recorded. Following the pre-test, the imagery participants imaged the twenty shots the required number of times per week. The control group completed some netball specific stretching. Transfer tests were also completed to assess the transferability of the intervention to related tasks. Group x test ANOVAs for performance score and shots scored revealed a significant interaction effect (p<.01). Tukey tests revealed that the 3x/wk imagery group improved performance on both measures, whereas the 2x/wk, 1x/wk and control group did not. These results support the notion that PETTLEP imagery may be more effective if completed at least three times per week.
本研究考察了不同频率的petttlep图像对无板篮球投篮表现的影响。32名女性参与者被分为四组:petlep图像每周1次(1次/周),petlep图像每周2次(2次/周),petlep图像每周3次(3次/周)和对照组。在测试前和测试后,参与者被要求在射击区内的五个不同的点上完成总共20次射击。他们每一击都得一分(总分5分),总分可能为100分。射门得分的次数也被记录下来。在预测试之后,图像参与者按要求每周拍摄20张照片。对照组完成了一些篮球赛特有的拉伸运动。还完成了转移测试,以评估干预措施对相关任务的可转移性。x组性能评分和射击评分的方差分析显示交互作用显著(p< 0.01)。Tukey测试显示,每3次/周的图像组在两项指标上都有所改善,而每2次/周、每1次/周和对照组则没有。这些结果支持了petttlep图像如果每周至少完成三次可能会更有效的概念。
{"title":"Impact of Differing Frequencies of PETTLEP Imagery on Netball Shooting Performance","authors":"C. Wakefield, Dave Smith","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1043","url":null,"abstract":"This study examined the effects of differing frequencies of PETTLEP imagery on netball shooting performance. Thirty-two female participants were divided into four groups: PETTLEP imagery once per week (1x/wk), PETTLEP imagery twice per week (2x/wk), PETTLEP imagery three times per week (3x/wk) and a control. During the pre-test and post-test participants were required to complete a total of twenty shots from five different points within the shooting zone. They were awarded points (out of five) for each shot, giving a possible total of 100 points. The numbers of shots scored was also recorded. Following the pre-test, the imagery participants imaged the twenty shots the required number of times per week. The control group completed some netball specific stretching. Transfer tests were also completed to assess the transferability of the intervention to related tasks. Group x test ANOVAs for performance score and shots scored revealed a significant interaction effect (p<.01). Tukey tests revealed that the 3x/wk imagery group improved performance on both measures, whereas the 2x/wk, 1x/wk and control group did not. These results support the notion that PETTLEP imagery may be more effective if completed at least three times per week.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2009-10-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1043","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 68
Does Self-Modeling Affect Imagery Ability or Vividness? 自我塑造会影响想象能力还是生动性?
Q3 Health Professions Pub Date : 2009-10-16 DOI: 10.2202/1932-0191.1035
Amanda M. Rymal, D. Ste-Marie
Research has shown imagery interventions to be important tools for learning new skills, as well as enhancing competitive performance. Moreover, imagery vividness and ability are two factors shown to contribute to their effectiveness. Therefore, learning ways to increase one's imagery vividness and ability is important. Hence, the present research examined the effects of an external stimulus (i.e., a self-modeling video) on one's imagery vividness and ability. A self-modeling video is an edited video showing the desired target behaviors; in this case it was a competitive dive. Two imagery measures (VMIQ and MIQ-R) were used to capture whether the self-modeling video would influence competitive divers' imagery vividness and ability. Seven competitive divers were administered both imagery measures at pre-test and post-test. After pre-test scores were taken, the participants' individual self-modeling videos were shown on three occasions before each competition and once at each competition. The results for the VMIQ indicated that imagery vividness when imaging the self was significantly better than when imaging others, F(1,6) = 7.44, p < 0.05, ?2 = .54. Of more importance is that the participants' imagery vividness increased after the self-modeling video had been administered for imaging one self but not for imaging others, although this only approached significance, F(1,6) = 3.70, p = .107, ?2 = .38. No significant results, however, were found for imagery ability. These findings suggest that there is potential for a self-modeling video to positively influence an athletes' imagery vividness.
研究表明,意象干预是学习新技能和提高竞争表现的重要工具。此外,意象的生动性和能力是影响其效果的两个因素。因此,学习如何提高自己的形象生动性和能力是很重要的。因此,本研究考察了外部刺激(即自我模仿视频)对人的形象生动性和能力的影响。自我建模视频是经过编辑的视频,展示期望的目标行为;在这种情况下,这是一次竞争性跳水。采用VMIQ和MIQ-R两种图像测量来捕捉自我建模视频是否会影响竞技潜水员的图像生动性和能力。7名竞技潜水员在测试前和测试后同时进行图像测量。在取得测试前分数后,参与者的个人自我建模视频在每次比赛前播放三次,每次比赛时播放一次。VMIQ测试结果显示,自我想象时的形象生动度显著优于他人想象时,F(1,6) = 7.44, p < 0.05, ?2 = 0.54。更重要的是,在自我建模视频被用于想象自己而不是想象他人之后,参与者的图像生动度增加了,尽管这只是接近显著性,F(1,6) = 3.70, p = .107, ?2 = .38。然而,在想象能力方面没有发现显著的结果。这些发现表明,自我建模视频有可能对运动员的形象生动度产生积极影响。
{"title":"Does Self-Modeling Affect Imagery Ability or Vividness?","authors":"Amanda M. Rymal, D. Ste-Marie","doi":"10.2202/1932-0191.1035","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2202/1932-0191.1035","url":null,"abstract":"Research has shown imagery interventions to be important tools for learning new skills, as well as enhancing competitive performance. Moreover, imagery vividness and ability are two factors shown to contribute to their effectiveness. Therefore, learning ways to increase one's imagery vividness and ability is important. Hence, the present research examined the effects of an external stimulus (i.e., a self-modeling video) on one's imagery vividness and ability. A self-modeling video is an edited video showing the desired target behaviors; in this case it was a competitive dive. Two imagery measures (VMIQ and MIQ-R) were used to capture whether the self-modeling video would influence competitive divers' imagery vividness and ability. Seven competitive divers were administered both imagery measures at pre-test and post-test. After pre-test scores were taken, the participants' individual self-modeling videos were shown on three occasions before each competition and once at each competition. The results for the VMIQ indicated that imagery vividness when imaging the self was significantly better than when imaging others, F(1,6) = 7.44, p < 0.05, ?2 = .54. Of more importance is that the participants' imagery vividness increased after the self-modeling video had been administered for imaging one self but not for imaging others, although this only approached significance, F(1,6) = 3.70, p = .107, ?2 = .38. No significant results, however, were found for imagery ability. These findings suggest that there is potential for a self-modeling video to positively influence an athletes' imagery vividness.","PeriodicalId":39479,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity","volume":"4 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2009-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.2202/1932-0191.1035","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"68732125","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 27
期刊
Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1