首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Collective remembering of Confucianism in Chinese language textbooks 语文教材中儒家思想的集体记忆
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/1834490921993511
Tian Xie, Shuang Chen, Dong Wang, James H. Liu
This study focuses on the collective remembering of an ancient system of meaning, examining content and changes in the construction of Confucianism in Chinese textbooks. The data consists of 84 editions of Chinese language teaching textbooks published by the People’s Education Press from 1949 to 2019. Content analysis shows that Confucianism is and was barely represented in this corpus. Thematic analysis shows that: (i) Only Confucius and Mencius were recognized as Confucian masters. (ii) Representations of Confucianism in the textbooks come from The Analects, Mencius, The Book of Rites, and The Book of Poetry, all of which are more than 2000 years old. (iii) Except for the 1970s, Confucianism was represented in a positive or at least neutral way. (iv) Confucianism is represented in a distant, abstract, decontextualized, and apolitical way, disconnected from students’ daily life. This gives insight into how a core representation stripped of its peripheral elements can lose meaning, and lose its normative influence on behavior. Contributions to collective memory, and implications for how to edit Chinese language textbooks to be more engaging are discussed.
本研究的重点是集体记忆一个古老的意义体系,考察中国教科书中儒家思想的内容和建构变化。该数据由人民教育出版社1949年至2019年出版的84版语文教学教材组成。内容分析表明,儒家思想在这一语料库中几乎没有代表性。主位分析表明:(一)只有孔子和孟子被公认为儒家大师。(ii)儒家思想在教科书中的代表来自《论语》、《孟子》、《礼记》和《诗经》,它们都有2000多年的历史。(iii)除1970年代外,儒家思想以积极或至少中立的方式表现。(iv)儒家思想以一种遥远、抽象、去文本化和非政治化的方式表现,与学生的日常生活脱节。这让我们深入了解了一个核心表征如果去掉其外围元素,会如何失去意义,并失去其对行为的规范影响。讨论了对集体记忆的贡献,以及如何编辑汉语教材以使其更具吸引力的启示。
{"title":"Collective remembering of Confucianism in Chinese language textbooks","authors":"Tian Xie, Shuang Chen, Dong Wang, James H. Liu","doi":"10.1177/1834490921993511","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1834490921993511","url":null,"abstract":"This study focuses on the collective remembering of an ancient system of meaning, examining content and changes in the construction of Confucianism in Chinese textbooks. The data consists of 84 editions of Chinese language teaching textbooks published by the People’s Education Press from 1949 to 2019. Content analysis shows that Confucianism is and was barely represented in this corpus. Thematic analysis shows that: (i) Only Confucius and Mencius were recognized as Confucian masters. (ii) Representations of Confucianism in the textbooks come from The Analects, Mencius, The Book of Rites, and The Book of Poetry, all of which are more than 2000 years old. (iii) Except for the 1970s, Confucianism was represented in a positive or at least neutral way. (iv) Confucianism is represented in a distant, abstract, decontextualized, and apolitical way, disconnected from students’ daily life. This gives insight into how a core representation stripped of its peripheral elements can lose meaning, and lose its normative influence on behavior. Contributions to collective memory, and implications for how to edit Chinese language textbooks to be more engaging are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1834490921993511","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49014305","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Designing a Preliminary Model of Coaching Pedagogy for Synchronous Collaborative Online Learning 为同步协作在线学习设计一个初步的辅导教学模型
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/1834490921991430
P. Timonen, H. Ruokamo
In recent years, webinar platforms have been broadly utilized in online learning where students meet one another synchronously online. This research’s underlying value is its recognition of the utmost importance of the awareness that online learning is a social process, as is all learning. This study aims to find out what kinds of synchronous collaborative online coaching pedagogy models have been used in previous research and proceeds to construct a preliminary pedagogical model for a coaching pedagogy for synchronous collaborative online learning (CPSCOL). The methods comprise a systematic literature review and qualitative-data and theory-driven content analysis. Through the systematic literature review, peer-reviewed articles spanning 2014–2018 are carefully examined. The results identify the following pedagogical framework, theory, and model combinations for synchronous collaborative online learning: the Community of Inquiry framework, including social, cognitive, and teaching presence; social presence in conjunction with the media synchronicity theory or the broaden-and-build theory, or the 4E Learning Cycle (engagement, exploration, explanation, and extension); no specific pedagogy; problem-based learning with Community of Inquiry framework or FISh (focus, investigate, and share); collaborative learning and collaborative learning connected to social presence; Carpe Diem with the Five-Step Model; and coaching pedagogy. The preliminary results indicate a scarcity of research on synchronous coaching pedagogy in online education. Consequently, the CPSCOL model for collaborative online learning, including cognitive, social, and teaching presence, is introduced to formulate a new perspective regarding webinar pedagogy. The process of learners, skills, and competences should factor in the pedagogical methods designed by a coach (teacher), and the results show that webinar pedagogy enables and enhances active collaborative learning and knowledge construction in groups. In addition, 18 CPSCOL principles of practice have been developed to support the practical implementation of the CPSCOL model.
近年来,网络研讨会平台已被广泛用于在线学习,学生们可以在网上同步见面。这项研究的潜在价值在于,它认识到在线学习和所有学习一样是一个社会过程,这一点至关重要。本研究旨在了解在先前的研究中使用了哪些类型的同步协作在线辅导教学法模型,并构建了同步协作在线学习辅导教学法(CPSCOL)的初步教学法模型。方法包括系统的文献综述、定性数据和理论驱动的内容分析。通过系统的文献综述,仔细审查了2014-2018年的同行评审文章。研究结果确定了以下同步协作在线学习的教学框架、理论和模型组合:探究社区框架,包括社交、认知和教学存在;与媒体同步性理论或拓宽与构建理论或4E学习周期(参与、探索、解释和扩展)相结合的社会存在;没有具体的教学法;基于问题的学习,采用调查社区框架或FISh(关注、调查和分享);协作学习和与社会存在相关的协作学习;用五步模型处理Diem;以及指导教学法。初步结果表明,在线教育中同步辅导教学法的研究较少。因此,引入了协作在线学习的CPSCOL模型,包括认知、社交和教学存在,以形成关于网络研讨会教学法的新视角。学习者的过程、技能和能力应纳入教练(教师)设计的教学方法中,结果表明,网络研讨会教学法能够促进并加强小组中的积极协作学习和知识构建。此外,还制定了18项CPSCOL实践原则,以支持CPSCOL模式的实际实施。
{"title":"Designing a Preliminary Model of Coaching Pedagogy for Synchronous Collaborative Online Learning","authors":"P. Timonen, H. Ruokamo","doi":"10.1177/1834490921991430","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1834490921991430","url":null,"abstract":"In recent years, webinar platforms have been broadly utilized in online learning where students meet one another synchronously online. This research’s underlying value is its recognition of the utmost importance of the awareness that online learning is a social process, as is all learning. This study aims to find out what kinds of synchronous collaborative online coaching pedagogy models have been used in previous research and proceeds to construct a preliminary pedagogical model for a coaching pedagogy for synchronous collaborative online learning (CPSCOL). The methods comprise a systematic literature review and qualitative-data and theory-driven content analysis. Through the systematic literature review, peer-reviewed articles spanning 2014–2018 are carefully examined. The results identify the following pedagogical framework, theory, and model combinations for synchronous collaborative online learning: the Community of Inquiry framework, including social, cognitive, and teaching presence; social presence in conjunction with the media synchronicity theory or the broaden-and-build theory, or the 4E Learning Cycle (engagement, exploration, explanation, and extension); no specific pedagogy; problem-based learning with Community of Inquiry framework or FISh (focus, investigate, and share); collaborative learning and collaborative learning connected to social presence; Carpe Diem with the Five-Step Model; and coaching pedagogy. The preliminary results indicate a scarcity of research on synchronous coaching pedagogy in online education. Consequently, the CPSCOL model for collaborative online learning, including cognitive, social, and teaching presence, is introduced to formulate a new perspective regarding webinar pedagogy. The process of learners, skills, and competences should factor in the pedagogical methods designed by a coach (teacher), and the results show that webinar pedagogy enables and enhances active collaborative learning and knowledge construction in groups. In addition, 18 CPSCOL principles of practice have been developed to support the practical implementation of the CPSCOL model.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47920543","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Effects of age on memory for pragmatic implications in advertising 年龄对广告语用含义记忆的影响
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/18344909211000452
Jing Yu, Xue-Rui Peng, Ming Yan
People employ automatic inferential processing when confronting pragmatically implied claims in advertising. However, whether comprehension and memorization of pragmatic implications differ between young and older adults is unclear. In the present study, we used eye-tracking technology to investigate online cognitive processes during reading of misleading advertisements. We found an interaction between age and advertising content, manifested as our older participants generated higher misleading rates in health-related than in health-irrelevant products, whereas this content-bias did not appear in their younger counterparts. Eye movement data further showed that the older adults spent more time processing critical claims for the health-related products than for the health-irrelevant products. Moreover, the correlations between fixation duration on pragmatic implications and misleading rates showed opposite trends in the two groups. The eye-tracking evidence novelly suggests that young and older adults may adopt different information processing strategies to comprehend pragmatic implications in advertising: More reading possibly enhances young adults’ gist memory whereas it facilitates older adults’ verbatim memory instead.
人们在面对广告中的语用暗示时会采用自动推理处理。然而,年轻人和老年人对语用含义的理解和记忆是否不同尚不清楚。在本研究中,我们使用眼动追踪技术来调查阅读误导性广告的在线认知过程。我们发现年龄和广告内容之间存在交互作用,表现为我们的老年参与者在与健康相关的产品中产生的误导率高于在与健康无关的产品中,而这种内容偏见在年轻参与者中没有出现。眼动数据进一步显示,与健康无关的产品相比,老年人花更多的时间处理与健康相关的产品的关键索赔。此外,对语用含义的注视持续时间和误导率之间的相关性在两组中显示出相反的趋势。眼动追踪证据新颖地表明,年轻人和老年人可能会采用不同的信息处理策略来理解广告中的语用含义:更多的阅读可能会增强年轻人的基本记忆,而相反,它会促进老年人的逐字记忆。
{"title":"Effects of age on memory for pragmatic implications in advertising","authors":"Jing Yu, Xue-Rui Peng, Ming Yan","doi":"10.1177/18344909211000452","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/18344909211000452","url":null,"abstract":"People employ automatic inferential processing when confronting pragmatically implied claims in advertising. However, whether comprehension and memorization of pragmatic implications differ between young and older adults is unclear. In the present study, we used eye-tracking technology to investigate online cognitive processes during reading of misleading advertisements. We found an interaction between age and advertising content, manifested as our older participants generated higher misleading rates in health-related than in health-irrelevant products, whereas this content-bias did not appear in their younger counterparts. Eye movement data further showed that the older adults spent more time processing critical claims for the health-related products than for the health-irrelevant products. Moreover, the correlations between fixation duration on pragmatic implications and misleading rates showed opposite trends in the two groups. The eye-tracking evidence novelly suggests that young and older adults may adopt different information processing strategies to comprehend pragmatic implications in advertising: More reading possibly enhances young adults’ gist memory whereas it facilitates older adults’ verbatim memory instead.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/18344909211000452","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41755848","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychometric Properties of the Chinese Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire: An Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling Study 中国人伯蒙-沃斯特Alexithymia问卷的心理测量特性:一项探索性结构方程建模研究
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/1834490921991429
Zhihao Wang, Ting Wang, K. Goerlich, R. J. Pitliya, B. Bermond, A. Aleman, Pengfei Xu, Yuejia Luo
The Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20) has been widely used to assess alexithymia. The Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ) assesses two additional features of alexithymia—the affective factors of emotionalizing and fantasizing, which are not included in the TAS-20. However, there is currently no Chinese version of the BVAQ. Here, the authors collected data from 439 college students (293 females, aged 17–27, mean ± SD = 20.25 ± 1.88) to evaluate the psychometric properties for a Chinese BVAQ translation. Exploratory structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis provided satisfactory validity and acceptable reliability for a six-factor first-order solution of a 35-item Chinese BVAQ. This adaptation retained the five original BVAQ factors (identifying, analyzing, verbalizing, emotionalizing, and fantasizing) and further specified the factor of identifying (successful identifying and unsuccessful identifying feelings). The authors also found a two-factor second-order model of cognitive and affective components for alexithymia in the Chinese population. Higher correlations with the TAS-20 were observed for identifying, analyzing, and verbalizing feelings (0.34 ∼ 0.61) relative to fantasizing and emotionalizing (0.02 ∼ −0.05). These results support the construct validity of the adaptation. This work provides a reliable and valid Chinese adaptation of the BVAQ.
多伦多述情障碍量表20 (TAS-20)被广泛用于述情障碍的评估。Bermond-Vorst述情障碍问卷(BVAQ)评估述情障碍的两个额外特征——情感化和幻想的情感因素,这些因素没有包括在TAS-20中。然而,目前还没有中文版的BVAQ。本文收集439名大学生(女性293名,年龄17-27岁,平均±SD = 20.25±1.88)的心理测量数据,对汉语BVAQ翻译的心理测量特性进行评价。探索性结构方程建模和验证性因子分析对35题中国人BVAQ的六因子一阶解提供了满意的效度和可接受的信度。该改编保留了原BVAQ的5个因子(识别、分析、语言化、情感化和幻想),并进一步细化了识别因子(成功识别和不成功识别情感)。作者还发现了中国人群述情障碍的认知和情感成分的双因素二阶模型。与幻想和情感化(0.02 ~ - 0.05)相比,在识别、分析和表达感觉方面,与TAS-20的相关性更高(0.34 ~ 0.61)。这些结果支持了适应性的构念效度。这项工作提供了一个可靠有效的BVAQ的中文改编。
{"title":"Psychometric Properties of the Chinese Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire: An Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling Study","authors":"Zhihao Wang, Ting Wang, K. Goerlich, R. J. Pitliya, B. Bermond, A. Aleman, Pengfei Xu, Yuejia Luo","doi":"10.1177/1834490921991429","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1834490921991429","url":null,"abstract":"The Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20) has been widely used to assess alexithymia. The Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ) assesses two additional features of alexithymia—the affective factors of emotionalizing and fantasizing, which are not included in the TAS-20. However, there is currently no Chinese version of the BVAQ. Here, the authors collected data from 439 college students (293 females, aged 17–27, mean ± SD = 20.25 ± 1.88) to evaluate the psychometric properties for a Chinese BVAQ translation. Exploratory structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis provided satisfactory validity and acceptable reliability for a six-factor first-order solution of a 35-item Chinese BVAQ. This adaptation retained the five original BVAQ factors (identifying, analyzing, verbalizing, emotionalizing, and fantasizing) and further specified the factor of identifying (successful identifying and unsuccessful identifying feelings). The authors also found a two-factor second-order model of cognitive and affective components for alexithymia in the Chinese population. Higher correlations with the TAS-20 were observed for identifying, analyzing, and verbalizing feelings (0.34 ∼ 0.61) relative to fantasizing and emotionalizing (0.02 ∼ −0.05). These results support the construct validity of the adaptation. This work provides a reliable and valid Chinese adaptation of the BVAQ.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1834490921991429","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47954942","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Vertical position can affect categorization of the rich and the poor in Chinese culture 纵向位置可以影响中国文化中贫富的分类
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/18344909211025705
Qiong Li, Chenquan Deng, Bin Zuo, Xiaobin Zhang
This study explored whether vertical position affects social categorization of the rich and the poor. Experiment 1 used high- and low-income occupations as stimuli, and found participants categorized high-income occupations faster when they were presented in the top vertical position compared to the bottom vertical position. In Experiment 2, participants responded using either the “up” or “down” key to categorize high- and low-income occupations, and responded faster to high-income occupations with the “up” key and low-income occupations with the “down” key. In Experiment 3, names identified as belonging to either rich or poor individuals were presented at the top or bottom of a screen, and the results were the same as in Experiments 1 and 2. These findings suggest that social categorization based on wealth involved perceptual simulations of vertical position, and that vertical position affects the social categorization of the rich and the poor.
本研究探讨垂直位置是否会影响社会的贫富分类。实验1采用高收入和低收入职业作为刺激,发现被试对高收入职业的分类速度要快于对低收入职业的分类速度。在实验2中,参与者使用“上”键或“下”键对高收入和低收入职业进行分类,并且对高收入职业使用“上”键和低收入职业使用“下”键的反应更快。在实验3中,被识别为属于富人或穷人的名字分别出现在屏幕的顶部或底部,结果与实验1和2相同。这些发现表明,基于财富的社会分类涉及对垂直位置的感知模拟,垂直位置影响富人和穷人的社会分类。
{"title":"Vertical position can affect categorization of the rich and the poor in Chinese culture","authors":"Qiong Li, Chenquan Deng, Bin Zuo, Xiaobin Zhang","doi":"10.1177/18344909211025705","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/18344909211025705","url":null,"abstract":"This study explored whether vertical position affects social categorization of the rich and the poor. Experiment 1 used high- and low-income occupations as stimuli, and found participants categorized high-income occupations faster when they were presented in the top vertical position compared to the bottom vertical position. In Experiment 2, participants responded using either the “up” or “down” key to categorize high- and low-income occupations, and responded faster to high-income occupations with the “up” key and low-income occupations with the “down” key. In Experiment 3, names identified as belonging to either rich or poor individuals were presented at the top or bottom of a screen, and the results were the same as in Experiments 1 and 2. These findings suggest that social categorization based on wealth involved perceptual simulations of vertical position, and that vertical position affects the social categorization of the rich and the poor.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/18344909211025705","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45787954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
COVID-19-related conspiracy theories in China 中国的新冠肺炎阴谋论
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/18344909211034928
Xue Wang, Shijiang Zuo, Hoi‐Wing Chan, C. Chiu, Ying-yi Hong
Many COVID-19 conspiracy theories implicate China and its agents, whether implicitly or explicitly, as conspirators with potentially malicious intent behind the current pandemic. We set out to explore whether Chinese people believe in pandemic-related conspiracy theories, and if so, how do their secure (in-group identification) and defensive (collective narcissism) in-group positivity predict their conspiracy beliefs. We hypothesized that national identification would negatively predict the tendency to attribute responsibility to an in-group, thus predicting less risk-rejection conspiracy theory beliefs (e.g., COVID-19 is a hoax). In contrast, national collective narcissism would positively predict the tendency to attribute responsibility for the pandemic to an out-group, which in turn would validate conspiracy theories that acknowledge the risk of the pandemic (e.g., COVID-19 is a bioweapon). To test these predictions, we collected data in China (n = 1,200) in April 2020. Supporting our predictions, national identification was negatively associated with risk-rejection conspiracy beliefs via in-group attribution, whereas national collective narcissism was positively associated with risk-acceptance conspiracy beliefs via out-group attribution.
许多COVID-19阴谋论暗示中国及其代理人,无论是含蓄地还是明确地,都是当前大流行背后具有潜在恶意的阴谋者。我们开始探索中国人是否相信与大流行相关的阴谋论,如果是这样,他们的安全(群体内认同)和防御(集体自恋)群体内积极性如何预测他们的阴谋论。我们假设,国家认同会对将责任归咎于内部群体的倾向产生负面预测,从而预测更少的风险拒绝阴谋论信仰(例如,COVID-19是一场骗局)。相比之下,国家集体自恋会积极预测将大流行的责任归咎于外部群体的倾向,这反过来又会验证承认大流行风险的阴谋论(例如,COVID-19是一种生物武器)。为了验证这些预测,我们于2020年4月在中国收集了数据(n = 1200)。支持我们的预测,国家认同通过群体内归因与风险拒绝阴谋信念负相关,而国家集体自恋通过群体外归因与风险接受阴谋信念正相关。
{"title":"COVID-19-related conspiracy theories in China","authors":"Xue Wang, Shijiang Zuo, Hoi‐Wing Chan, C. Chiu, Ying-yi Hong","doi":"10.1177/18344909211034928","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/18344909211034928","url":null,"abstract":"Many COVID-19 conspiracy theories implicate China and its agents, whether implicitly or explicitly, as conspirators with potentially malicious intent behind the current pandemic. We set out to explore whether Chinese people believe in pandemic-related conspiracy theories, and if so, how do their secure (in-group identification) and defensive (collective narcissism) in-group positivity predict their conspiracy beliefs. We hypothesized that national identification would negatively predict the tendency to attribute responsibility to an in-group, thus predicting less risk-rejection conspiracy theory beliefs (e.g., COVID-19 is a hoax). In contrast, national collective narcissism would positively predict the tendency to attribute responsibility for the pandemic to an out-group, which in turn would validate conspiracy theories that acknowledge the risk of the pandemic (e.g., COVID-19 is a bioweapon). To test these predictions, we collected data in China (n = 1,200) in April 2020. Supporting our predictions, national identification was negatively associated with risk-rejection conspiracy beliefs via in-group attribution, whereas national collective narcissism was positively associated with risk-acceptance conspiracy beliefs via out-group attribution.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/18344909211034928","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46017976","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
How Social Media Shapes One’s Public Mood: The Three-Way Interaction Effect of Sphere, Information Valence, and Justice Sensitivity 社交媒体如何塑造公众情绪:领域、信息价值和司法敏感性的三元互动效应
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/1834490921991425
Yixin Zhou, Zheng Zhang, Kexin Wang, Shuang Chen, Mingjie Zhou
Public mood is a key concept in explaining collective activity, but the way social media shapes an audience’s public mood is still not fully understood. This study aims to explore how social media posts with various characteristics change public mood. The authors asked 351 participants to read 30 microblog newsletters with a 2 × 2 between-subject design (public × private sphere; positive × negative value). The results showed that (a) positive private information decreased negative public mood, (b) positive public information decreased positive public mood rather than increasing it, and (c) negative private information reduced the positive public mood of individuals who were high in justice sensitivity. The discussion focuses on the adverse effect of overexposure to positive public information and how individuals’ means of information processing vary.
公众情绪是解释集体活动的一个关键概念,但社交媒体塑造观众公众情绪的方式仍不完全清楚。本研究旨在探讨具有不同特征的社交媒体帖子如何改变公众情绪。作者要求351名参与者阅读30篇微博时事通讯,其中2 × 2主题设计之间(公开 × 私人领域;积极乐观的 × 负值)。结果表明:(a)积极的私人信息降低了消极的公众情绪;(b)积极的公共信息降低了积极的公众情绪,而不是增加了积极的公共情绪;(c)消极的私人信息减少了司法敏感性高的人的积极的公众情绪。讨论的重点是过度接触正面公共信息的不利影响,以及个人的信息处理方式如何变化。
{"title":"How Social Media Shapes One’s Public Mood: The Three-Way Interaction Effect of Sphere, Information Valence, and Justice Sensitivity","authors":"Yixin Zhou, Zheng Zhang, Kexin Wang, Shuang Chen, Mingjie Zhou","doi":"10.1177/1834490921991425","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/1834490921991425","url":null,"abstract":"Public mood is a key concept in explaining collective activity, but the way social media shapes an audience’s public mood is still not fully understood. This study aims to explore how social media posts with various characteristics change public mood. The authors asked 351 participants to read 30 microblog newsletters with a 2 × 2 between-subject design (public × private sphere; positive × negative value). The results showed that (a) positive private information decreased negative public mood, (b) positive public information decreased positive public mood rather than increasing it, and (c) negative private information reduced the positive public mood of individuals who were high in justice sensitivity. The discussion focuses on the adverse effect of overexposure to positive public information and how individuals’ means of information processing vary.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/1834490921991425","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44309214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Numerosity representation in a deep convolutional neural network 深度卷积神经网络中的数字表示
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/18344909211012613
Cihua Zhou, Wei Xu, Yujie Liu, Zhichao Xue, Rui Chen, Ke Zhou, Jia Liu
Enumerating objects in the environment (i.e., “number sense”) is crucial for survival in many animal species, and foundational for the construction of more abstract and complex mathematical knowledge in humans. Perhaps surprisingly, deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) spontaneously emerge a similar number sense even without any explicit training for numerosity estimation. However, little is known about how the number sense emerges, and the extent to which it is comparable with human number sense. Here, we examined whether the numerosity underestimation effect, a phenomenon indicating that numerosity perception acts upon the perceptual number rather than the physical number, can be observed in DCNNs. In a typical DCNN, AlexNet, we found that number-selective units at late layers operated on the perceptual number, like humans do. More importantly, this perceptual number sense did not emerge abruptly, rather developed progressively along the hierarchy in the DCNN, shifting from the physical number sense at early layers to perceptual number sense at late layers. Our finding hence provides important implications for the neural implementation of number sense in the human brain and advocates future research to determine whether the representation of numerosity also develops gradually along the human visual stream from physical number to perceptual number.
列举环境中的物体(即“数感”)对许多动物物种的生存至关重要,也是人类构建更抽象、更复杂的数学知识的基础。也许令人惊讶的是,深度卷积神经网络(DCNNs)即使没有任何明确的数字估计训练,也会自发地产生类似的数字感。然而,关于数字感是如何产生的,以及它与人类数字感的可比性的程度,人们知之甚少。在这里,我们研究了数字低估效应,即数字感知作用于感知数字而不是物理数字的现象,是否可以在DCNNs中观察到。在典型的DCNN AlexNet中,我们发现后期层的数字选择单元像人类一样对感知数字进行操作。更重要的是,这种感性数字感并不是突然出现的,而是沿着DCNN的层次结构逐步发展的,从早期的物理数字感转变为后期的感性数字感。因此,我们的发现为人类大脑中数感的神经实现提供了重要的启示,并倡导未来的研究,以确定数的表征是否也沿着人类视觉流从物理数到感知数逐渐发展。
{"title":"Numerosity representation in a deep convolutional neural network","authors":"Cihua Zhou, Wei Xu, Yujie Liu, Zhichao Xue, Rui Chen, Ke Zhou, Jia Liu","doi":"10.1177/18344909211012613","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/18344909211012613","url":null,"abstract":"Enumerating objects in the environment (i.e., “number sense”) is crucial for survival in many animal species, and foundational for the construction of more abstract and complex mathematical knowledge in humans. Perhaps surprisingly, deep convolutional neural networks (DCNNs) spontaneously emerge a similar number sense even without any explicit training for numerosity estimation. However, little is known about how the number sense emerges, and the extent to which it is comparable with human number sense. Here, we examined whether the numerosity underestimation effect, a phenomenon indicating that numerosity perception acts upon the perceptual number rather than the physical number, can be observed in DCNNs. In a typical DCNN, AlexNet, we found that number-selective units at late layers operated on the perceptual number, like humans do. More importantly, this perceptual number sense did not emerge abruptly, rather developed progressively along the hierarchy in the DCNN, shifting from the physical number sense at early layers to perceptual number sense at late layers. Our finding hence provides important implications for the neural implementation of number sense in the human brain and advocates future research to determine whether the representation of numerosity also develops gradually along the human visual stream from physical number to perceptual number.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1177/18344909211012613","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49021243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Examining conspiracy beliefs and COVID-19 in four countries: The role of disgust towards the political system and implications for prosocial behavior 研究四个国家的阴谋信仰和新冠肺炎:对政治制度的厌恶的作用及其对亲社会行为的影响
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/18344909211056855
Chanki Moon, G. Travaglino
Since late 2019, the coronavirus SARS-COV-2 responsible for the COVID-19 disease has continued to spread across different regions of the world. As a result, governments have been implementing measures for controlling the disease which rely on people's cooperation. In this research, we considered predictors and implications of people's beliefs that they “haven’t been told the ‘whole story’ about COVID-19.” Specifically, we examined the role of disgust towards the political system in predicting conspiratorial tendencies across four countries, in Europe (Italy and the UK), North America (the USA), and Asia (South Korea). In addition, we investigated the implications of conspiratorial beliefs for individuals’ intentions to engage in prosocial cooperative behavior. In line with the idea that feelings of disgust towards the political system may indicate that people perceive the system as violating core norms, results showed that disgust was associated with stronger conspiratorial tendencies. Individuals’ conspiratorial tendencies were in turn associated with lower intentions to help others during the pandemic. Results were broadly consistent across the countries tested. Directions for future research are discussed.
自2019年末以来,导致新冠肺炎疾病的冠状病毒SARS-COV-2继续在世界不同地区传播。因此,各国政府一直在实施依靠人民合作来控制这种疾病的措施。在这项研究中,我们考虑了人们认为他们“没有被告知新冠肺炎的‘全部故事’”的预测因素和影响。具体而言,我们研究了对政治制度的厌恶在预测欧洲(意大利和英国)、北美(美国)和亚洲(韩国)四个国家阴谋倾向中的作用。此外,我们还调查了阴谋信念对个体参与亲社会合作行为意图的影响。与对政治制度的厌恶感可能表明人们认为该制度违反了核心规范的观点一致,研究结果表明,厌恶与更强的阴谋倾向有关。个人的阴谋倾向反过来与在疫情期间帮助他人的意愿较低有关。测试国家的结果大致一致。讨论了未来研究的方向。
{"title":"Examining conspiracy beliefs and COVID-19 in four countries: The role of disgust towards the political system and implications for prosocial behavior","authors":"Chanki Moon, G. Travaglino","doi":"10.1177/18344909211056855","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/18344909211056855","url":null,"abstract":"Since late 2019, the coronavirus SARS-COV-2 responsible for the COVID-19 disease has continued to spread across different regions of the world. As a result, governments have been implementing measures for controlling the disease which rely on people's cooperation. In this research, we considered predictors and implications of people's beliefs that they “haven’t been told the ‘whole story’ about COVID-19.” Specifically, we examined the role of disgust towards the political system in predicting conspiratorial tendencies across four countries, in Europe (Italy and the UK), North America (the USA), and Asia (South Korea). In addition, we investigated the implications of conspiratorial beliefs for individuals’ intentions to engage in prosocial cooperative behavior. In line with the idea that feelings of disgust towards the political system may indicate that people perceive the system as violating core norms, results showed that disgust was associated with stronger conspiratorial tendencies. Individuals’ conspiratorial tendencies were in turn associated with lower intentions to help others during the pandemic. Results were broadly consistent across the countries tested. Directions for future research are discussed.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48753528","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Corrigendum to “The role of self-evaluation in predicting attitudes toward supporters of COVID-19-related conspiracy theories: A direct and a conceptual replication of Cichoka et al. (2016).” “自我评价在预测对covid -19相关阴谋论支持者的态度中的作用:Cichoka等人(2016)的直接和概念复制”的更正。
IF 2.3 3区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/18344909211064802
B. Siem, B. Kretzmeyer,, S. Sturmer
In the above-referenced paper, author name was mentioned incorrectly in the title and page 4. It should be Cichocka inspite of Cichoka. The correct title is as given below: The role of self-evaluation in predicting attitudes toward supporters of COVID-19-related conspiracy theories: A direct and a conceptual replication of Cichocka et al. (2016). On page 4, column 1, line 1, the correct statement is “With the present research we seek to further clarify the role of selfevaluation by attempting to replicate Cichocka et al.’s central findings observed with a US sample in a different national context, namely in Germany.” The online version of the paper has been corrected. © The Author(s) 2021.
在上述引用的论文中,在标题和第4页中错误地提到了作者姓名。应该是奇乔卡,尽管是奇乔卡。正确的标题如下:自我评价在预测对covid -19相关阴谋论支持者的态度中的作用:Cichocka et al.(2016)的直接和概念复制。在第4页,第1栏,第1行,正确的表述是“在目前的研究中,我们试图通过复制Cichocka等人在不同国家背景下(即德国)观察到的美国样本的中心发现,来进一步阐明自我评价的作用。”论文的在线版本已被更正。©作者2021。
{"title":"Corrigendum to “The role of self-evaluation in predicting attitudes toward supporters of COVID-19-related conspiracy theories: A direct and a conceptual replication of Cichoka et al. (2016).”","authors":"B. Siem, B. Kretzmeyer,, S. Sturmer","doi":"10.1177/18344909211064802","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/18344909211064802","url":null,"abstract":"In the above-referenced paper, author name was mentioned incorrectly in the title and page 4. It should be Cichocka inspite of Cichoka. The correct title is as given below: The role of self-evaluation in predicting attitudes toward supporters of COVID-19-related conspiracy theories: A direct and a conceptual replication of Cichocka et al. (2016). On page 4, column 1, line 1, the correct statement is “With the present research we seek to further clarify the role of selfevaluation by attempting to replicate Cichocka et al.’s central findings observed with a US sample in a different national context, namely in Germany.” The online version of the paper has been corrected. © The Author(s) 2021.","PeriodicalId":45049,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48471234","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1