首页 > 最新文献

Spatial Cognition and Computation最新文献

英文 中文
In memoriam: Christian Freksa (1950-2020) 纪念:克里斯蒂安·弗雷克萨(1950-2020)
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2023.2270137
Thomas Barkowsky, Thora Tenbrink, Peter Haddawy, Ulrich Furbach
{"title":"In memoriam: Christian Freksa (1950-2020)","authors":"Thomas Barkowsky, Thora Tenbrink, Peter Haddawy, Ulrich Furbach","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2023.2270137","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2023.2270137","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"115 11-12","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136376803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Treat robots as humans? Perspective choice in human-human and human-robot spatial language interaction 把机器人当作人类对待?人与机器人空间语言交互中的视角选择
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-06-22 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2023.2227995
Chengli Xiao, Weishuang Wu, Junyi Zhang, Liufei Xu
ABSTRACT Spatial language interaction is critical for human-robot interactions. However, previous findings are inconsistent in people’s perspective choices for robots and humans. In two experiments, participants viewed complex spatial layouts and verbally instructed an imagined human or robot addressee to find the target location or route. In both tasks, participants only tended to describe from humans‘ but not robots’ perspective, at the cost of taking more time to generate instructions for humans than robots. The results showed that spatial language interaction is not only a spatial but also a social task, and people do not regard robots as human-like social partners.
空间语言交互是人机交互的关键。然而,之前的研究结果在人们对机器人和人类的视角选择上并不一致。在两个实验中,参与者观看复杂的空间布局,并口头指示想象中的人类或机器人收件人找到目标位置或路线。在这两项任务中,参与者都倾向于从人类而不是机器人的角度来描述,其代价是要花更多的时间来为人类而不是机器人生成指令。结果表明,空间语言交互不仅是一项空间任务,也是一项社会任务,人们并不把机器人视为类似人类的社会伙伴。
{"title":"Treat robots as humans? Perspective choice in human-human and human-robot spatial language interaction","authors":"Chengli Xiao, Weishuang Wu, Junyi Zhang, Liufei Xu","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2023.2227995","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2023.2227995","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Spatial language interaction is critical for human-robot interactions. However, previous findings are inconsistent in people’s perspective choices for robots and humans. In two experiments, participants viewed complex spatial layouts and verbally instructed an imagined human or robot addressee to find the target location or route. In both tasks, participants only tended to describe from humans‘ but not robots’ perspective, at the cost of taking more time to generate instructions for humans than robots. The results showed that spatial language interaction is not only a spatial but also a social task, and people do not regard robots as human-like social partners.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"7 1","pages":"309 - 329"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-06-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89065824","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direction information is more influential than distance information in memory for location relative to landmarks 对于相对于地标的位置,方向信息比距离信息在记忆中的影响更大
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-04-21 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2023.2204394
Darin Galyer, S. Dopkins
ABSTRACT Our results suggest that direction information is more influential than distance information in memory for the locations of objects relative to landmarks. Participants viewed target objects in conjunction with pairs of landmarks and then attempted to place the targets in the correct locations relative to the landmarks. The locations of the landmarks at test were slightly different from their locations at study. This dissociated the locations in which the targets would be placed if they were remembered solely on the basis of distance and direction, respectively. Participants placed the targets closer to the “direction-predicted” locations than to the “distance-predicted” locations.
我们的研究结果表明,方向信息比距离信息在记忆中对物体相对于地标位置的影响更大。参与者将目标物体与成对的地标结合起来,然后尝试将目标放置在相对于地标的正确位置。测试时的地标位置与学习时的位置略有不同。如果仅仅根据距离和方向来记忆目标,这就分离了目标被放置的位置。参与者将目标放置在离“方向预测”位置较近的地方,而不是离“距离预测”位置较近的地方。
{"title":"Direction information is more influential than distance information in memory for location relative to landmarks","authors":"Darin Galyer, S. Dopkins","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2023.2204394","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2023.2204394","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Our results suggest that direction information is more influential than distance information in memory for the locations of objects relative to landmarks. Participants viewed target objects in conjunction with pairs of landmarks and then attempted to place the targets in the correct locations relative to the landmarks. The locations of the landmarks at test were slightly different from their locations at study. This dissociated the locations in which the targets would be placed if they were remembered solely on the basis of distance and direction, respectively. Participants placed the targets closer to the “direction-predicted” locations than to the “distance-predicted” locations.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"27 1","pages":"293 - 308"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-04-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"81740826","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Task-dependent sketch maps 与任务相关的草图
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-02-09 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2023.2170802
J. Krukar, Antonia van Eek, A. Schwering
ABSTRACT Sketch maps are considered a reliable method for assessing spatial knowledge. However, it is unknown whether all information types in sketch maps are reconstructed with similar accuracy under differing task instructions. Here, we show that the dominant information type and metric accuracy of sketches of a single environment drawn by the same person change across drawing tasks. Results are in line with the situated nature of spatial cognition: participants produce different types of maps, and different recall errors, depending on the task at hand. Understanding variation in measures of sketch maps is critical for designing standardized and automated methods of sketch map analysis.
草图被认为是评估空间知识的可靠方法。然而,在不同的任务指令下,草图图中的所有信息类型是否都能以相似的精度重建,这是未知的。在这里,我们展示了由同一个人绘制的单一环境草图的主要信息类型和度量精度在绘制任务中发生变化。结果与空间认知的情境性质一致:参与者根据手头的任务产生不同类型的地图和不同的回忆错误。了解速写图度量的变化对于设计标准化和自动化的速写图分析方法至关重要。
{"title":"Task-dependent sketch maps","authors":"J. Krukar, Antonia van Eek, A. Schwering","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2023.2170802","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2023.2170802","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Sketch maps are considered a reliable method for assessing spatial knowledge. However, it is unknown whether all information types in sketch maps are reconstructed with similar accuracy under differing task instructions. Here, we show that the dominant information type and metric accuracy of sketches of a single environment drawn by the same person change across drawing tasks. Results are in line with the situated nature of spatial cognition: participants produce different types of maps, and different recall errors, depending on the task at hand. Understanding variation in measures of sketch maps is critical for designing standardized and automated methods of sketch map analysis.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"175 1","pages":"263 - 292"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2023-02-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79714008","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence for flexible navigation strategies during spatial learning involving path choices 空间学习中灵活导航策略的证据包括路径选择
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-12-15 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2022.2158090
Stephanie Doner, Jingyi Zheng, Andrew S. McAvan, Michael J. Starrett, R. Campbell, Delaney Sanders, Arne D. Ekstrom
ABSTRACT In a classic study in 1946, Tolman et al. found that rodents chose the optimal path to a hidden goal location when given the option to take a shortcut. Subsequent studies, though, found mixed results, including a previous study in humans showing non-optimal response strategies. Here, we tested human participants in a virtual sunburst maze identical to that employed by Tolman. Across three different experiments, we found that participants consistently employed non-optimal response strategies involving either following the encoded route or using the light as a beacon cue. Adding distal boundary cues (Experiment 2) or enhancing idiothetic cues (Experiment 3) led to the same overall results. Our findings suggest participants’ search strategies are often suboptimal when given the opportunity to spontaneously take a short cut.
在1946年的一项经典研究中,Tolman等人发现,当啮齿类动物可以选择走捷径时,它们会选择通往隐藏目标位置的最优路径。然而,随后的研究发现了不同的结果,包括先前对人类的研究显示出非最佳反应策略。在这里,我们在一个与托尔曼所使用的相同的虚拟太阳爆发迷宫中测试人类参与者。在三个不同的实验中,我们发现参与者始终采用非最佳反应策略,包括遵循编码路线或使用灯光作为信标提示。添加远端边界线索(实验2)或增强固有线索(实验3)的总体结果相同。我们的研究结果表明,当参与者有机会自发地走捷径时,他们的搜索策略往往不是最优的。
{"title":"Evidence for flexible navigation strategies during spatial learning involving path choices","authors":"Stephanie Doner, Jingyi Zheng, Andrew S. McAvan, Michael J. Starrett, R. Campbell, Delaney Sanders, Arne D. Ekstrom","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2022.2158090","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2022.2158090","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT In a classic study in 1946, Tolman et al. found that rodents chose the optimal path to a hidden goal location when given the option to take a shortcut. Subsequent studies, though, found mixed results, including a previous study in humans showing non-optimal response strategies. Here, we tested human participants in a virtual sunburst maze identical to that employed by Tolman. Across three different experiments, we found that participants consistently employed non-optimal response strategies involving either following the encoded route or using the light as a beacon cue. Adding distal boundary cues (Experiment 2) or enhancing idiothetic cues (Experiment 3) led to the same overall results. Our findings suggest participants’ search strategies are often suboptimal when given the opportunity to spontaneously take a short cut.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"79 1","pages":"233 - 262"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83791361","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Landmark weights - an alternative to spatial distances in shortest route algorithms 地标权重——最短路径算法中空间距离的替代方案
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-10-05 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2022.2130330
Eva Nuhn, S. Timpf
ABSTRACT Although numerous studies have shown that landmarks are important for navigation, almost all navigation systems implement a shortest-route algorithm without considering landmarks. Which options do we have to integrate landmarks into a route calculation? How would the resulting routes differ from shortest routes? We propose a weighting method for Dijkstra’s shortest route algorithm to generate “landmark routes” and compare them to their corresponding shortest routes. We show that the extra distance and time needed to walk the landmark routes is acceptable in most of the routes. The main contribution is a thorough discussion of the differences between the two types of routes. Since the results are promising, we discuss variations in computing the weights as well as recommend human subject tests.
尽管大量研究表明地标对导航很重要,但几乎所有的导航系统都实现了不考虑地标的最短路径算法。我们有哪些选择将地标整合到路线计算中?由此产生的路线与最短的路线有何不同?我们提出了一种Dijkstra最短路径算法的加权方法来生成“地标路径”,并将它们与相应的最短路径进行比较。我们表明,在大多数路线中,步行标志性路线所需的额外距离和时间是可以接受的。主要的贡献是对两种路线之间的差异进行了深入的讨论。由于结果是有希望的,我们讨论了计算权重的变化以及推荐人体受试者测试。
{"title":"Landmark weights - an alternative to spatial distances in shortest route algorithms","authors":"Eva Nuhn, S. Timpf","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2022.2130330","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2022.2130330","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Although numerous studies have shown that landmarks are important for navigation, almost all navigation systems implement a shortest-route algorithm without considering landmarks. Which options do we have to integrate landmarks into a route calculation? How would the resulting routes differ from shortest routes? We propose a weighting method for Dijkstra’s shortest route algorithm to generate “landmark routes” and compare them to their corresponding shortest routes. We show that the extra distance and time needed to walk the landmark routes is acceptable in most of the routes. The main contribution is a thorough discussion of the differences between the two types of routes. Since the results are promising, we discuss variations in computing the weights as well as recommend human subject tests.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"150 1","pages":"206 - 232"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-10-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77401726","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Transparency study of architectural space based on a scalar field function 基于标量场函数的建筑空间透明度研究
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-08-31 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2022.2117045
X. Lou, K. Deng, Yidi Li, Changhai Peng
ABSTRACT This paper studies the sufficient, necessary, and optimal conditions of the phenomenal transparency of architectural space (PTAS) by the eigenvector and eigenvalue of the gradient function of Scalar Field Function (SFF). Then, the SFF’s method is used to analyze the PTAS of significant contemporary or canonical architectural works. The conclusions are: the eigenvalue of the SFF and its integral can be used to describe PTAS; the sufficient and necessary conditions of PTAS are the eigenvalue cannot be zero, and the area integral of the eigenvalue should be greater than a certain value; the optimal condition of PTAS is that the eigenvalue is the largest; the corresponding design methods include spatial stratification, graphic overlay, and grid rotation.
摘要本文利用标量场函数(Scalar Field function, SFF)的梯度函数的特征向量和特征值,研究了建筑空间(PTAS)现象透明的充要条件、必要条件和最优条件。然后,使用SFF的方法分析重要的当代或规范建筑作品的PTAS。结论是:SFF的特征值及其积分可以用来描述PTAS;PTAS的充要条件是特征值不能为零,且特征值的面积积分大于某一值;PTAS的最优条件是特征值最大;相应的设计方法包括空间分层、图形叠加、网格旋转等。
{"title":"Transparency study of architectural space based on a scalar field function","authors":"X. Lou, K. Deng, Yidi Li, Changhai Peng","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2022.2117045","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2022.2117045","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This paper studies the sufficient, necessary, and optimal conditions of the phenomenal transparency of architectural space (PTAS) by the eigenvector and eigenvalue of the gradient function of Scalar Field Function (SFF). Then, the SFF’s method is used to analyze the PTAS of significant contemporary or canonical architectural works. The conclusions are: the eigenvalue of the SFF and its integral can be used to describe PTAS; the sufficient and necessary conditions of PTAS are the eigenvalue cannot be zero, and the area integral of the eigenvalue should be greater than a certain value; the optimal condition of PTAS is that the eigenvalue is the largest; the corresponding design methods include spatial stratification, graphic overlay, and grid rotation.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"258 1","pages":"177 - 205"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-08-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"73347549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An empirical study of the semantic similarity of geospatial prepositions and their senses 地理空间介词及其意义语义相似性的实证研究
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-08-19 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2022.2111683
Niloofar Aflaki, Kristin Stock, C. B. Jones, H. Guesgen, J. Morley
ABSTRACT Spatial prepositions have been studied in some detail from multiple disciplinary perspectives. However, neither the semantic similarity of these prepositions, nor the relationships between the multiple senses of different spatial prepositions, are well understood. In an empirical study of 24 spatial prepositions, we identify the degree and nature of semantic similarity and extract senses for three semantically similar groups of prepositions using t-SNE, DBSCAN clustering, and Venn diagrams. We validate the work by manual annotation with another data set. We find nuances in meaning among proximity and adjacency prepositions, such as the use of close to instead of near for pairs of lines, and the importance of proximity over contact for the next to preposition, in contrast to other adjacency prepositions.
空间介词已经从多个学科角度进行了较为详细的研究。然而,这些介词的语义相似性和不同空间介词的多重意义之间的关系都没有得到很好的理解。在对24个空间介词的实证研究中,我们利用t-SNE、DBSCAN聚类和Venn图对3组语义相似的介词进行了语义相似性程度和性质的识别和语义提取。我们通过使用另一个数据集进行手动注释来验证工作。我们发现邻近介词和邻接介词在意义上的细微差别,例如对行使用close to而不是near,以及与其他邻接介词相比,邻近介词比接触更重要。
{"title":"An empirical study of the semantic similarity of geospatial prepositions and their senses","authors":"Niloofar Aflaki, Kristin Stock, C. B. Jones, H. Guesgen, J. Morley","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2022.2111683","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2022.2111683","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Spatial prepositions have been studied in some detail from multiple disciplinary perspectives. However, neither the semantic similarity of these prepositions, nor the relationships between the multiple senses of different spatial prepositions, are well understood. In an empirical study of 24 spatial prepositions, we identify the degree and nature of semantic similarity and extract senses for three semantically similar groups of prepositions using t-SNE, DBSCAN clustering, and Venn diagrams. We validate the work by manual annotation with another data set. We find nuances in meaning among proximity and adjacency prepositions, such as the use of close to instead of near for pairs of lines, and the importance of proximity over contact for the next to preposition, in contrast to other adjacency prepositions.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"12 1","pages":"132 - 176"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87038999","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Speaking of location: a review of spatial language research 说到位置:空间语言研究述评
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-07-19 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2022.2095275
Kristin Stock, C. B. Jones, T. Tenbrink
ABSTRACT Spatial language incorporates descriptions of locations, routes, and landscapes, and is used by humans daily. Research has addressed a wide range of aspects of spatial language, including its form; the ways in which it is selected and applied; and cognitive, geometric, and functional factors affecting its use. Furthermore, much work has been done on the automation of spatial language extraction, analysis, interpretation, and generation. To introduce the Special Issue on this broad topic, this paper reviews spatial language research framed by an extension to the well-known semantic triangle, the “spatial semantic pyramid,” which represents both human spatial language and relevant computational research. By introducing it, we hope to stimulate discussion about gaps and future directions in this important research field.
空间语言包含了对地点、路线和景观的描述,是人类日常使用的语言。研究涉及空间语言的广泛方面,包括其形式;选择和应用的方式;以及影响其使用的认知、几何和功能因素。此外,在空间语言提取、分析、解释和生成的自动化方面已经做了大量的工作。为了介绍这一广泛主题的特刊,本文回顾了空间语言的研究框架,即众所周知的“空间语义三角”的扩展,即“空间语义金字塔”,它代表了人类空间语言和相关的计算研究。通过介绍它,我们希望激发对这一重要研究领域的差距和未来方向的讨论。
{"title":"Speaking of location: a review of spatial language research","authors":"Kristin Stock, C. B. Jones, T. Tenbrink","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2022.2095275","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2022.2095275","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Spatial language incorporates descriptions of locations, routes, and landscapes, and is used by humans daily. Research has addressed a wide range of aspects of spatial language, including its form; the ways in which it is selected and applied; and cognitive, geometric, and functional factors affecting its use. Furthermore, much work has been done on the automation of spatial language extraction, analysis, interpretation, and generation. To introduce the Special Issue on this broad topic, this paper reviews spatial language research framed by an extension to the well-known semantic triangle, the “spatial semantic pyramid,” which represents both human spatial language and relevant computational research. By introducing it, we hope to stimulate discussion about gaps and future directions in this important research field.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"47 1","pages":"185 - 224"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86417505","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Age-related changes in how 3.5- to 5.5-year-olds observe and imagine rotational object motion 3.5- 5.5岁儿童观察和想象旋转物体运动的年龄相关变化
IF 1.9 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2022-07-10 DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2022.2095276
Salome Pedrett, Alain Chavaillaz, Andrea Frick
ABSTRACT Mental representations of rotation were investigated in 3.5- to 5.5-year-olds (N = 74) using a multi-method approach. In a novel mental-rotation task, children were asked to choose one of two rotated shapes that would fit onto a counterpart. The developmental trajectory of mental rotation was compared to eye-tracking results on how the same children observed and anticipated circular object motion. On the mental-rotation task, children below age 4 performed above chance up to angles of 150°, and performance improved with age. Eye-tracking results indicated that mental representations of circular motion were largely developed by the age of 3.5 years. In contrast, perception of rotational motion and mental rotation of asymmetrical shapes continued to develop between 3.5 and 5.5 years of age.
采用多方法研究了3.5 ~ 5.5岁儿童(N = 74)的旋转心理表征。在一项新颖的心理旋转任务中,孩子们被要求从两个旋转的形状中选择一个适合另一个的形状。研究人员将心理旋转的发展轨迹与眼球追踪的结果进行了比较,以了解这些儿童如何观察和预测圆形物体的运动。在心理旋转任务中,4岁以下儿童在150°角度上的表现高于正常水平,并且随着年龄的增长而提高。眼球追踪结果表明,圆形运动的心理表征在3.5岁时基本形成。相比之下,旋转运动的感知和不对称形状的心理旋转在3.5至5.5岁之间继续发展。
{"title":"Age-related changes in how 3.5- to 5.5-year-olds observe and imagine rotational object motion","authors":"Salome Pedrett, Alain Chavaillaz, Andrea Frick","doi":"10.1080/13875868.2022.2095276","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2022.2095276","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Mental representations of rotation were investigated in 3.5- to 5.5-year-olds (N = 74) using a multi-method approach. In a novel mental-rotation task, children were asked to choose one of two rotated shapes that would fit onto a counterpart. The developmental trajectory of mental rotation was compared to eye-tracking results on how the same children observed and anticipated circular object motion. On the mental-rotation task, children below age 4 performed above chance up to angles of 150°, and performance improved with age. Eye-tracking results indicated that mental representations of circular motion were largely developed by the age of 3.5 years. In contrast, perception of rotational motion and mental rotation of asymmetrical shapes continued to develop between 3.5 and 5.5 years of age.","PeriodicalId":46199,"journal":{"name":"Spatial Cognition and Computation","volume":"23 1","pages":"83 - 111"},"PeriodicalIF":1.9,"publicationDate":"2022-07-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88037234","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
期刊
Spatial Cognition and Computation
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1