首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Linguistics最新文献

英文 中文
Daniel Altshuler and Robert Truswell, Coordination and the Syntax-Discourse Interface (Oxford Surveys in Syntax and Morphology 11). Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2022. Pp. xii + 315. Daniel Altshuler和Robert Truswell,协调与句法话语界面(牛津语法与形态学调查11)。牛津:牛津大学出版社,2022年。第xii+315页。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-02-16 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226723000038
R. Chaves
In the last half century, much research has focused on the nature of coordination, particularly on its extraction patterns, collectively dubbed as the Coordinate Structure Constraint (CSC) by Ross (1967). The first part of the CSC, known as the Conjunct Constraint, bans the extraction of conjuncts (*Who did you see Robin and _?), and the second part, known as the Element Constraint, blocks extraction from conjuncts (*Who did you see Robin and a picture of _?). The latter is circumvented when extraction is Across-the-Board (Who did you buy a picture of _and a book about _?) or when the the order of conjuncts has certain asymmetrical interpretations, such as narration (e.g. Here’s the whiskey which I went to the store and bought _) or violated expectation (e.g. How much can you drink_and still stay sober]?). Prior research on the CSC focuses on the syntax or semantic-pragmatic components, not on their interaction. The strength of this monograph is precisely the attention that both domains earn. The conclusion (276) is that neither syntax nor discourse is exclusively responsible for explaining the observable extraction patterns; rather, both are needed to fully understand how coordination interacts with the grammar of extraction. This book offers a deep examination of the role that discourse restrictions play in explaining how extraction works in coordination, covering an extraordinary amount of data. Apart from minor issues discussed below, it is well-argued. Chapter 2 surveys definitions of the object of study and coordination, and it concludes that they defy a cross-linguistic characterization, as there are no necessary and sufficient morphosyntactic properties in the languages of the world. Another challenge for a definition of coordination that the authors mention is ‘nonconstituent’ coordination phenomena, such as Right-Node Raising (RNR) andGapping, but the problemwith this claim is that these phenomena are not strictly related to coordination:
在过去的半个世纪里,许多研究都集中在协调的性质上,特别是其提取模式上,Ross(1967)将其统称为协调结构约束(CSC)。CSC的第一部分,被称为连词约束,禁止提取连词(*你看到了Robin和_?),而第二部分被称为元素约束,阻止从连词中提取(*你见过Robin和一张_的照片?)。当提取是全面的(你买了谁的照片和一本关于_的书。先前对CSC的研究主要集中在句法或语义语用成分上,而不是它们之间的相互作用。这本专著的优势正是这两个领域所获得的关注。结论(276)是,语法和语篇都不专门负责解释可观察到的提取模式;相反,两者都需要充分理解协调是如何与提取语法相互作用的。这本书深入探讨了话语限制在解释提取如何协同工作中所起的作用,涵盖了大量数据。除了下面讨论的一些小问题外,这是有充分理由的。第二章考察了研究对象和协调对象的定义,得出的结论是,它们违背了跨语言的特征,因为世界语言中没有必要和充分的形态句法特性。作者提到的对协调定义的另一个挑战是“非结构性”协调现象,如右节点抬高(RNR)和间隙,但这一说法的问题是,这些现象与协调并不严格相关:
{"title":"Daniel Altshuler and Robert Truswell, Coordination and the Syntax-Discourse Interface (Oxford Surveys in Syntax and Morphology 11). Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2022. Pp. xii + 315.","authors":"R. Chaves","doi":"10.1017/S0022226723000038","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226723000038","url":null,"abstract":"In the last half century, much research has focused on the nature of coordination, particularly on its extraction patterns, collectively dubbed as the Coordinate Structure Constraint (CSC) by Ross (1967). The first part of the CSC, known as the Conjunct Constraint, bans the extraction of conjuncts (*Who did you see Robin and _?), and the second part, known as the Element Constraint, blocks extraction from conjuncts (*Who did you see Robin and a picture of _?). The latter is circumvented when extraction is Across-the-Board (Who did you buy a picture of _and a book about _?) or when the the order of conjuncts has certain asymmetrical interpretations, such as narration (e.g. Here’s the whiskey which I went to the store and bought _) or violated expectation (e.g. How much can you drink_and still stay sober]?). Prior research on the CSC focuses on the syntax or semantic-pragmatic components, not on their interaction. The strength of this monograph is precisely the attention that both domains earn. The conclusion (276) is that neither syntax nor discourse is exclusively responsible for explaining the observable extraction patterns; rather, both are needed to fully understand how coordination interacts with the grammar of extraction. This book offers a deep examination of the role that discourse restrictions play in explaining how extraction works in coordination, covering an extraordinary amount of data. Apart from minor issues discussed below, it is well-argued. Chapter 2 surveys definitions of the object of study and coordination, and it concludes that they defy a cross-linguistic characterization, as there are no necessary and sufficient morphosyntactic properties in the languages of the world. Another challenge for a definition of coordination that the authors mention is ‘nonconstituent’ coordination phenomena, such as Right-Node Raising (RNR) andGapping, but the problemwith this claim is that these phenomena are not strictly related to coordination:","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"691 - 695"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-02-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"45857045","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shigeru Miyagawa, Syntax in the treetops (Linguistic Inquiry Monographs). Cambridge, MA & London: MIT Press, 2022. Pp. xviii + 234. 宫川茂,《树梢上的句法》(语言学探究专著)。马萨诸塞州剑桥和伦敦:麻省理工学院出版社,2022年。第xvii+234页。
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-02-14 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226723000026
Nicholas Catasso
The syntax of the leftmost domain(s) of the clause and its interplay with discourse, the speaker’s coordinates, and those of the world in which a proposition is uttered or interpreted have been the focus of an immense amount of linguistic research in the last decades, in particular since Rizzi’s (1997) seminal paper on the fine structure of the CP. Some of the questions that have maintained the interest of the scientific community to date, and still have not been answered in a unitary way, include whether – and if so, towhat extent – themakeup of the left periphery can be assumed to be cross-linguistically stable; what formal mechanisms (e.g. Move and Merge) lead to linearizations in this domain and how the projections situated there, whether they are positioned intraor extrasententially, determine the licensing of the phenomena overtly realized in the clause; and how the interaction of syntax, pragmatics, and semantics can be effectively explained in a formal model. Shigeru Miyagawa’s monograph Syntax in the treetops is an excellent and much-needed attempt to shed light on some of these long-burning issues. Looking at data from Japanese and other languages, the author explores the very role of syntax in connecting the contextual features and the semantics of an utterance, proceeding from the assumption that this module ‘provides the basic framework that makes the performance of a speech act and the conveyance of meaning possible’ (x). In particular, Miyagawa assumes with Krifka (2014) that the realization of any speech act involves the presence and activation of given ‘actors’ (at least a speaker and a hearer) endowed with given linguistic attitudes and correlates (an intention, the capacity to use language in a persuasive way, etc.) and that these factors are represented in the syntax, specifically in the left periphery. The explicit objective of this work is to demonstrate (and provide empirical evidence for) the existence of dedicated projections for the encoding of these coordinates in the left domain of the clause and to show how they work and correlate with syntactic phenomena. Miyagawa’s monograph is organized in six chapters preceded by a foreword, preface, and list of abbreviations. The last chapter is followed by the section ‘Notes’, the references, a name index, and a subject index (234 pages in total). In Chapter 1 (‘Setting the stage’, 1–36), the author discusses the notion of ‘root (clause)’ and its implications for the representation of speaker and addressee in the
最左边的域(s)的语法与话语的条款及其相互作用,演讲者的坐标,和世界的命题是说出或解释大量的语言研究的重点在过去的几十年里,特别是组织者自Rizzi(1997)的论文的CP的精细结构。一些问题保持着科学界的兴趣到目前为止,还没有统一的方式回答,包括是否——如果是,在多大程度上——左脑外围的构成可以被认为是跨语言稳定的;何种形式机制(例如Move和Merge)会导致该域中的线性化,以及位于该域中的投影(无论它们是定位于内部还是外部)如何决定对该条款中公开实现的现象的许可;以及语法、语用和语义的相互作用如何在形式化模型中得到有效解释。Shigeru Miyagawa的专著《树梢上的句法》(Syntax in the tree - top)是一个极好的、急需的尝试,它阐明了这些长期燃烧的问题。通过查看日语和其他语言的数据,作者探索了语法在连接话语的语境特征和语义方面的作用,并从这个模块“提供了使言语行为的表现和意义的传递成为可能的基本框架”的假设出发(x)。Miyagawa和Krifka(2014)认为,任何言语行为的实现都涉及给定的“行动者”(至少是一个说话者和一个听者)的存在和激活,这些“行动者”具有给定的语言态度和相关关系(意图、以有说服力的方式使用语言的能力等),这些因素在句法中表现出来,特别是在左外围。这项工作的明确目标是证明(并提供经验证据)在子句的左域中这些坐标编码的专用投影的存在,并展示它们如何工作并与句法现象相关联。宫川的专著分为六个章节,前面有前言、序言和缩写列表。最后一章后面是“注释”部分、参考文献、名称索引和主题索引(总共234页)。在第一章(“设置舞台”,1 - 36)中,作者讨论了“根(子句)”的概念及其对英语中说话人和收件人的表征的影响
{"title":"Shigeru Miyagawa, Syntax in the treetops (Linguistic Inquiry Monographs). Cambridge, MA & London: MIT Press, 2022. Pp. xviii + 234.","authors":"Nicholas Catasso","doi":"10.1017/S0022226723000026","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226723000026","url":null,"abstract":"The syntax of the leftmost domain(s) of the clause and its interplay with discourse, the speaker’s coordinates, and those of the world in which a proposition is uttered or interpreted have been the focus of an immense amount of linguistic research in the last decades, in particular since Rizzi’s (1997) seminal paper on the fine structure of the CP. Some of the questions that have maintained the interest of the scientific community to date, and still have not been answered in a unitary way, include whether – and if so, towhat extent – themakeup of the left periphery can be assumed to be cross-linguistically stable; what formal mechanisms (e.g. Move and Merge) lead to linearizations in this domain and how the projections situated there, whether they are positioned intraor extrasententially, determine the licensing of the phenomena overtly realized in the clause; and how the interaction of syntax, pragmatics, and semantics can be effectively explained in a formal model. Shigeru Miyagawa’s monograph Syntax in the treetops is an excellent and much-needed attempt to shed light on some of these long-burning issues. Looking at data from Japanese and other languages, the author explores the very role of syntax in connecting the contextual features and the semantics of an utterance, proceeding from the assumption that this module ‘provides the basic framework that makes the performance of a speech act and the conveyance of meaning possible’ (x). In particular, Miyagawa assumes with Krifka (2014) that the realization of any speech act involves the presence and activation of given ‘actors’ (at least a speaker and a hearer) endowed with given linguistic attitudes and correlates (an intention, the capacity to use language in a persuasive way, etc.) and that these factors are represented in the syntax, specifically in the left periphery. The explicit objective of this work is to demonstrate (and provide empirical evidence for) the existence of dedicated projections for the encoding of these coordinates in the left domain of the clause and to show how they work and correlate with syntactic phenomena. Miyagawa’s monograph is organized in six chapters preceded by a foreword, preface, and list of abbreviations. The last chapter is followed by the section ‘Notes’, the references, a name index, and a subject index (234 pages in total). In Chapter 1 (‘Setting the stage’, 1–36), the author discusses the notion of ‘root (clause)’ and its implications for the representation of speaker and addressee in the","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"459 - 463"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-02-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"46361039","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Voice mismatch and contrast in French Right-Node Raising 法语右结提升中的语音错配与对比
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-02-13 DOI: 10.1017/S002222672200055X
Abeillé, Aoi Shiraishi, Barbara Hemforth
Right-Node Raising is generally considered to impose stricter identity conditions than other kinds of ellipsis, such as VP ellipsis, according to Hankamer & Sag 1976 and Hardt 1993. In this paper, we investigate voice mismatch in French Right-Node Raising (RNR) through a corpus study and two experiments. We show that RNR with voice mismatch can be found in a written corpus (frTenTen 2012) and that many examples involve coordination of a reflexive active and a short passive form. We suggest this is because semantic contrast (here, between self and external agent) plays a role according to Hartmann (2000) and Abeillé and Mouret (2010). We ran two acceptability judgement experiments to test voice mismatch and semantic contrast. We did not find any penalty for voice mismatch with VP ellipsis but an interaction with semantic contrast. We also found an effect of semantic contrast when coordinating an active and a passive VP without participle ellipsis. We conclude that voice mismatch is acceptable with RNR and propose a Head-driven Phrase-Structure Grammar (HPSG) analysis, following Chaves (2014) and Shiraïshi et al. (2019).
根据Hankamer & Sag 1976和Hardt 1993,右节点提升通常被认为比其他类型的省略号(如VP省略号)施加更严格的身份条件。本文通过语料库研究和两个实验研究了法语右节点提升(RNR)中的语音失配问题。我们表明,语音不匹配的RNR可以在书面语料库中找到(frTenTen 2012),并且许多例子涉及自反主动和简短被动形式的协调。根据Hartmann(2000)和abeill和Mouret(2010)的研究,我们认为这是因为语义对比(这里是自我和外部主体之间的对比)发挥了作用。我们进行了两个可接受性判断实验来测试语音不匹配和语义对比。我们没有发现语音不匹配与VP省略的任何惩罚,但与语义对比的相互作用。我们还发现在不省略分词的情况下,主动副语态和被动副语态的语义对比效果。我们得出结论,语音不匹配在RNR中是可以接受的,并提出了头部驱动的短语结构语法(HPSG)分析,继Chaves(2014)和Shiraïshi等人(2019)之后。
{"title":"Voice mismatch and contrast in French Right-Node Raising","authors":"Abeillé, Aoi Shiraishi, Barbara Hemforth","doi":"10.1017/S002222672200055X","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S002222672200055X","url":null,"abstract":"Right-Node Raising is generally considered to impose stricter identity conditions than other kinds of ellipsis, such as VP ellipsis, according to Hankamer & Sag 1976 and Hardt 1993. In this paper, we investigate voice mismatch in French Right-Node Raising (RNR) through a corpus study and two experiments. We show that RNR with voice mismatch can be found in a written corpus (frTenTen 2012) and that many examples involve coordination of a reflexive active and a short passive form. We suggest this is because semantic contrast (here, between self and external agent) plays a role according to Hartmann (2000) and Abeillé and Mouret (2010). We ran two acceptability judgement experiments to test voice mismatch and semantic contrast. We did not find any penalty for voice mismatch with VP ellipsis but an interaction with semantic contrast. We also found an effect of semantic contrast when coordinating an active and a passive VP without participle ellipsis. We conclude that voice mismatch is acceptable with RNR and propose a Head-driven Phrase-Structure Grammar (HPSG) analysis, following Chaves (2014) and Shiraïshi et al. (2019).","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"361 - 388"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47727372","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Information structural effects in processing contrastive ellipsis: Eye-tracking evidence from a flexible word order language 加工对比省略的信息结构效应:来自一种灵活词序语言的眼球追踪证据
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-02-03 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000597
Marju Kaps
Previous experimental work on the processing of clausal ellipsis with contrastive remnants shows a Locality preference – DP remnants are preferentially paired with the most recently encountered DP correlate in the antecedent clause, even in the presence of contrastive prosody or semantic bias favouring a non-local correlate. The Locality effect has been argued to arise from the language processor consulting (default) information-structural representations when pairing remnants and correlates, yet direct evidence for the information structure hypothesis for Locality has been difficult to obtain. Estonian is a flexible word order language that optionally marks Contrastive Topics (CTs) syntactically, while allowing for the linear distance between a CT subject correlate and remnant to be held constant, in order to rule out a Recency explanation for the Locality effect. In an eye-tracking during reading experiment with case-disambiguated subject and object remnants in Estonian, we see asymmetries in the Locality preference (i.e. object advantage) following canonical Verb-second antecedent clauses and subject CT-marking Verb-third clauses. This provides novel evidence for fine-grained information-structural representations guiding the processing of contrastive ellipsis.
先前关于小句省略与对比残余处理的实验工作显示了一种局部性偏好——即使在存在有利于非局部性相关的对比韵律或语义偏差的情况下,DP残余也优先与前句中最近遇到的DP相关配对。局部性效应被认为是由于语言处理器在配对残余和相关物时咨询(默认)信息结构表示而产生的,但是局部性的信息结构假设的直接证据很难获得。爱沙尼亚语是一种灵活的词序语言,可以选择在语法上标记对比主题(CT),同时允许CT主题相关和残余之间的线性距离保持不变,以排除局域效应的近因解释。在一项用爱沙尼亚语消歧主语和宾语残句进行的阅读过程眼动追踪实验中,我们发现在规范动词-第二先行句和主语ct标记动词-第三先行句之后,区域偏好(即宾语优势)存在不对称性。这为细粒度信息结构表征指导对比省略的处理提供了新的证据。
{"title":"Information structural effects in processing contrastive ellipsis: Eye-tracking evidence from a flexible word order language","authors":"Marju Kaps","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000597","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000597","url":null,"abstract":"Previous experimental work on the processing of clausal ellipsis with contrastive remnants shows a Locality preference – DP remnants are preferentially paired with the most recently encountered DP correlate in the antecedent clause, even in the presence of contrastive prosody or semantic bias favouring a non-local correlate. The Locality effect has been argued to arise from the language processor consulting (default) information-structural representations when pairing remnants and correlates, yet direct evidence for the information structure hypothesis for Locality has been difficult to obtain. Estonian is a flexible word order language that optionally marks Contrastive Topics (CTs) syntactically, while allowing for the linear distance between a CT subject correlate and remnant to be held constant, in order to rule out a Recency explanation for the Locality effect. In an eye-tracking during reading experiment with case-disambiguated subject and object remnants in Estonian, we see asymmetries in the Locality preference (i.e. object advantage) following canonical Verb-second antecedent clauses and subject CT-marking Verb-third clauses. This provides novel evidence for fine-grained information-structural representations guiding the processing of contrastive ellipsis.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"427 - 457"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-02-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42638082","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
LIN volume 59 issue 1 Cover and Front matter LIN第59卷第1期封面和封面
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1017/s0022226722000561
{"title":"LIN volume 59 issue 1 Cover and Front matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/s0022226722000561","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226722000561","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":"f1 - f2"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42063081","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
LIN volume 59 issue 1 Cover and Back matter LIN卷59期1封面和封底
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-02-01 DOI: 10.1017/s0022226722000573
{"title":"LIN volume 59 issue 1 Cover and Back matter","authors":"","doi":"10.1017/s0022226722000573","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226722000573","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"38 50","pages":"b1 - b2"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41244418","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intonation correlates of canonical and non-canonical wh-in-situ questions in Spanish 西班牙语规范和非规范wh原位疑问句的语调相关性
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-01-16 DOI: 10.1017/s0022226722000548
Carolina González, Lara Reglero
This project investigates the intonation of canonical (information-seeking) and non-canonical wh-in-situ echo questions conveying repetition and surprise in Northern Peninsular Spanish. Data from 14 female participants were collected via a contextualised elicitation task. The following correlates were examined: (i) the melodic curve of the wh-in-situ question, (ii) the nuclear peak (in Hz), (iii) the wh-tonal range (i.e. the difference between the lowest nuclear Low and the highest boundary High), and (iv) the nuclear contour. Results show that all wh-in-situ questions investigated display similar melodic curves and nuclear contours, but canonical questions have significantly lower nuclear peaks and wh-tonal ranges than non-canonical questions. Echo-repetition and echo-surprise questions also differ in nuclear peak and wh-tonal range. We propose a tentative analysis, whereby canonical in-situ questions have a final H% boundary tone, in contrast to non-canonical questions, which have an extra-High (upstepped) final boundary tone (¡H%).
本项目研究了规范(信息寻求)和非规范wh原位回声问题在北半岛西班牙语中传达重复和惊喜的语调。14名女性参与者的数据是通过情境启发任务收集的。检查了以下相关性:(i)wh原位问题的旋律曲线,(ii)核峰值(Hz),(iii)wh音调范围(即最低核Low和最高边界High之间的差异),以及(iv)核轮廓。结果表明,所有被调查的wh原位疑问句都显示出相似的旋律曲线和核等值线,但规范疑问句的核峰和wh音调范围明显低于非规范疑问句。回声重复和回声惊喜问题在核峰值和wh音调范围上也有所不同。我们提出了一个初步分析,即规范原位问题有一个最终的H%边界音,而非规范原位问题则有一个超高(上阶)的最终边界音(-H%)。
{"title":"Intonation correlates of canonical and non-canonical wh-in-situ questions in Spanish","authors":"Carolina González, Lara Reglero","doi":"10.1017/s0022226722000548","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226722000548","url":null,"abstract":"This project investigates the intonation of canonical (information-seeking) and non-canonical wh-in-situ echo questions conveying repetition and surprise in Northern Peninsular Spanish. Data from 14 female participants were collected via a contextualised elicitation task. The following correlates were examined: (i) the melodic curve of the wh-in-situ question, (ii) the nuclear peak (in Hz), (iii) the wh-tonal range (i.e. the difference between the lowest nuclear Low and the highest boundary High), and (iv) the nuclear contour. Results show that all wh-in-situ questions investigated display similar melodic curves and nuclear contours, but canonical questions have significantly lower nuclear peaks and wh-tonal ranges than non-canonical questions. Echo-repetition and echo-surprise questions also differ in nuclear peak and wh-tonal range. We propose a tentative analysis, whereby canonical in-situ questions have a final H% boundary tone, in contrast to non-canonical questions, which have an extra-High (upstepped) final boundary tone (¡H%).","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47632108","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Introduction to the special issue Experimental and Corpus-based Approaches to Ellipsis 专题导论:基于实验和语料库的省略号研究方法
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-01-13 DOI: 10.1017/S0022226722000585
Gabriela Bîlbîie, J. Nykiel
Ellipsis has been, and continues to be, of both theoretical and empirical interest. It affects the syntax of phrases or clauses by stranding their various constituents but keeps the semantics of the stranded constituents identical to that of their non-elliptical counterparts. The theoretical value of ellipsis lies, therefore, in the relationship between meaning and form that it encodes, such that a complete propositional meaning is paired with what appears to be a syntactically incomplete form. This property of ellipsis has inspired researchers to probe, in particular, the syntax of ellipsis and the role the surrounding context plays in helping resolve ellipsis, as stranded constituents depend on the surrounding context for their interpretation. Among the constructions that have attracted considerable attention over the years are clausal ellipsis (e.g. sluicing (Example 1), sprouting (Example 2), stripping (Example 3), and fragments (Example 4)), pseudogapping (Example 5), gapping (Example 6), and Right Node Raising (RNR) (Example 7), all of which are discussed in the contributions to this special issue.
Ellipsis一直并将继续受到理论和实证的关注。它通过使短语或从句的各种成分搁浅来影响其语法,但使搁浅成分的语义与非省略成分的语义保持一致。因此,省略的理论价值在于它所编码的意义和形式之间的关系,即一个完整的命题意义与一个似乎在句法上不完整的形式配对。省略的这一特性激发了研究人员的探索,特别是省略的句法以及周围语境在帮助解决省略中所起的作用,因为搁浅的成分取决于周围的环境来进行解释。在这些年来引起相当大关注的结构中,有从句省略(例如冲洗(实施例1)、发芽(实施例2)、剥离(实施例3)和片段(实施例4))、假间隙(实施例5)、间隙(实施方案6)和右节点抬高(RNR)(实施例7),所有这些都在本特刊的稿件中进行了讨论。
{"title":"Introduction to the special issue Experimental and Corpus-based Approaches to Ellipsis","authors":"Gabriela Bîlbîie, J. Nykiel","doi":"10.1017/S0022226722000585","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226722000585","url":null,"abstract":"Ellipsis has been, and continues to be, of both theoretical and empirical interest. It affects the syntax of phrases or clauses by stranding their various constituents but keeps the semantics of the stranded constituents identical to that of their non-elliptical counterparts. The theoretical value of ellipsis lies, therefore, in the relationship between meaning and form that it encodes, such that a complete propositional meaning is paired with what appears to be a syntactically incomplete form. This property of ellipsis has inspired researchers to probe, in particular, the syntax of ellipsis and the role the surrounding context plays in helping resolve ellipsis, as stranded constituents depend on the surrounding context for their interpretation. Among the constructions that have attracted considerable attention over the years are clausal ellipsis (e.g. sluicing (Example 1), sprouting (Example 2), stripping (Example 3), and fragments (Example 4)), pseudogapping (Example 5), gapping (Example 6), and Right Node Raising (RNR) (Example 7), all of which are discussed in the contributions to this special issue.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":"59 1","pages":"229 - 234"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48212783","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Oblique complements in Estonian: A corpus perspective 爱沙尼亚语中的斜补语:语料库视角
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2023-01-12 DOI: 10.1017/s0022226722000536
Mari Aigro, Virve Vihman
This study focuses on Estonian verb-complement structures, which include oblique (non-canonically marked) complements marked in spatial cases. Not all approaches agree on whether canonical arguments and oblique complements have argument status of the same type, but they do mostly agree that the two types of complement markings are used by different types of verbs. First, oblique case is viewed as always indexing the original semantics of the case (direct semantics), that is osutama ‘point at’ selecting an allative (‘onto’) complement. Second, oblique case usage is seen as referring to a restricted set of syntactic relations (indirect semantics), that is Estonian allative and adessive being used for marking Experiencers. In any case, oblique complement verbs are viewed as more semantically restricted than canonical object verbs. This study tests these two hypotheses in a quantitative corpus approach. In a non-semantically extracted sample of verbs (n = 232), it compares the lexical-semantic transitivity of oblique and canonical complement verbs in order to investigate the degree to which indirect semantic effects differentiate between the two types of verbs. In addition, it outlines direct semantic effects between oblique case frames in terms of semantic roles. Finally, it investigates the way these patterns are related to the cases’ individual grammaticalisation degrees.
本研究的重点是爱沙尼亚语动词补语结构,包括在空间格中标记的斜补语(非规范标记)。并非所有的方法都同意规范变元和斜补语是否具有相同类型的变元状态,但它们大多同意这两种类型的补语标记由不同类型的动词使用。首先,斜格被视为总是索引格的原始语义(直接语义),即osutama“在”选择全动(“nto”)补语的点。第二,斜格用法被视为指一组受限制的句法关系(间接语义),即爱沙尼亚语中用于标记经验者的所有格和主格。在任何情况下,斜补语动词都被认为比规范宾语动词在语义上更受限制。本研究采用定量语料库的方法检验了这两个假设。在一个非语义提取的动词样本(n=232)中,它比较了斜补动词和规范补动词的词汇语义及物性,以研究间接语义效应在多大程度上区分了这两类动词。此外,它还从语义角色的角度概述了斜格框架之间的直接语义效应。最后,研究了这些模式与案例的个体语法化程度之间的关系。
{"title":"Oblique complements in Estonian: A corpus perspective","authors":"Mari Aigro, Virve Vihman","doi":"10.1017/s0022226722000536","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226722000536","url":null,"abstract":"This study focuses on Estonian verb-complement structures, which include oblique (non-canonically marked) complements marked in spatial cases. Not all approaches agree on whether canonical arguments and oblique complements have argument status of the same type, but they do mostly agree that the two types of complement markings are used by different types of verbs. First, oblique case is viewed as always indexing the original semantics of the case (direct semantics), that is osutama ‘point at’ selecting an allative (‘onto’) complement. Second, oblique case usage is seen as referring to a restricted set of syntactic relations (indirect semantics), that is Estonian allative and adessive being used for marking Experiencers. In any case, oblique complement verbs are viewed as more semantically restricted than canonical object verbs. This study tests these two hypotheses in a quantitative corpus approach. In a non-semantically extracted sample of verbs (n = 232), it compares the lexical-semantic transitivity of oblique and canonical complement verbs in order to investigate the degree to which indirect semantic effects differentiate between the two types of verbs. In addition, it outlines direct semantic effects between oblique case frames in terms of semantic roles. Finally, it investigates the way these patterns are related to the cases’ individual grammaticalisation degrees.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-01-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48493728","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The intricate inflectional relationships underpinning morphological analogy 支撑形态类比的复杂屈折关系
IF 1.1 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-12-27 DOI: 10.1017/s0022226722000494
Louise Esher
In Gévaudan varieties of Occitan (Gallo-Romance), exceptionless syncretism between preterite and imperfect subjunctive forms arises in the first and second person plural (e.g. faguessiám [faɡeˈsjɔn] ‘do.pret/ipf.sbjv.1pl’, faguessiatz [faɡeˈsjat] ‘do.pret/ipf.sbjv.2pl’). Reconstructing the historical emergence of this syncretism pattern reveals that it is crucially dependent on multiple and diverse implicational relationships of form, inferred and productively exploited by speakers: in particular, inherited identity between preterite and imperfect subjunctive stems, and identity between imperfect indicative forms of èstre [ɛsˈtʀe] ‘be’ and preterite or imperfect subjunctive desinences. The observed developments support a view of inflectional analogies as informed by intricate paradigmatic and implicational structure of the type proposed within ‘abstractive’, word-based theories of inflection.
在欧西坦的Gévaudan变种(加洛罗曼史)中,前体和不完全虚拟语气形式之间的无例外融合出现在第一人称和第二人称复数形式中(例如,faguessiám[faõesjõn]'do.prept/ipf.sbjv.1pl',fagueassiatz[faãesjat]'do.Prept/ipf.sbjv.2pl')。重建这种融合模式的历史出现表明,它在很大程度上取决于形式,被说话者推断并有效利用:特别是,前体和不完全虚拟词干之间的继承性同一性,以及èstre[s tʀe]的不完全指示形式与前体或不完全虚拟语气之间的同一性。观察到的发展支持了一种屈折类比的观点,这种观点是由“抽象”的、基于单词的屈折理论中提出的复杂的例证和暗示结构所提供的。
{"title":"The intricate inflectional relationships underpinning morphological analogy","authors":"Louise Esher","doi":"10.1017/s0022226722000494","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226722000494","url":null,"abstract":"In Gévaudan varieties of Occitan (Gallo-Romance), exceptionless syncretism between preterite and imperfect subjunctive forms arises in the first and second person plural (e.g. faguessiám [faɡeˈsjɔn] ‘do.pret/ipf.sbjv.1pl’, faguessiatz [faɡeˈsjat] ‘do.pret/ipf.sbjv.2pl’). Reconstructing the historical emergence of this syncretism pattern reveals that it is crucially dependent on multiple and diverse implicational relationships of form, inferred and productively exploited by speakers: in particular, inherited identity between preterite and imperfect subjunctive stems, and identity between imperfect indicative forms of èstre [ɛsˈtʀe] ‘be’ and preterite or imperfect subjunctive desinences. The observed developments support a view of inflectional analogies as informed by intricate paradigmatic and implicational structure of the type proposed within ‘abstractive’, word-based theories of inflection.","PeriodicalId":47027,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Linguistics","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2022-12-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47208648","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Linguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1