首页 > 最新文献

Asian Journal of Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Backlash towards male versus female leaders' interpersonal emotion management strategy use: The role of followers' gender-based leadership stereotypes 男性与女性领导者在使用人际情绪管理策略时的反差:追随者基于性别的领导刻板印象的作用
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-19 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12608
Mahmut Bayazit, Gergely Czukor, Uzay Dural Şenoğuz, İlknur Özalp Türetgen

Research on the backlash effect predominantly investigated penalties men and women incurred when they violate gender norms in the domain of achievement-oriented aggressiveness. We investigated backlash reactions towards female versus male leaders' attempts to manage follower emotions using one of two gender-stereotypic interpersonal emotion management strategies, cognitive change or expression suppression, in a 2×2 vignette experiment in which undergraduate students as participants acted as followers (N = 206). We hypothesized that followers high in explicit or implicit prejudice towards female leadership would be motivated to show backlash in the form of negative attitudes and anger when female leaders use an expression suppression strategy and when male leaders use a cognitive change strategy, violating gender norms. We also explored the role of followers' gender as a boundary condition of backlash reactions towards leaders of the same versus opposite sex. Male participants with negative explicit attitudes towards women leaders in general expressed higher levels of anger towards a female leader who utilized a suppression strategy. Female participants holding implicit stereotypes reported negative attitudes for both female and male leaders who utilized a gender-incongruent emotion management strategy. We discuss the implications of our findings for theory and research on the backlash effect.

有关反冲效应的研究主要是调查男性和女性在违反以成就为导向的攻击性领域的性别规范时所受到的惩罚。我们在一个 2×2 的小实验中调查了女性与男性领导者在尝试使用两种性别陈规定型的人际情绪管理策略(认知改变或表达抑制)之一来管理追随者情绪时的反冲反应。我们假设,当女性领导者使用表达压制策略和男性领导者使用认知改变策略时,对女性领导者存在显性或隐性偏见的追随者会以负面态度和愤怒的形式表现出反弹,从而违反性别规范。我们还探讨了追随者的性别作为对同性和异性领导者反弹反应的边界条件的作用。对女性领导者普遍持负面明确态度的男性受试者对使用压制策略的女性领导者表达了更高程度的愤怒。持有内隐刻板印象的女性受试者对采用性别一致情绪管理策略的女性和男性领导者都持负面态度。我们将讨论我们的发现对反冲效应理论和研究的影响。
{"title":"Backlash towards male versus female leaders' interpersonal emotion management strategy use: The role of followers' gender-based leadership stereotypes","authors":"Mahmut Bayazit,&nbsp;Gergely Czukor,&nbsp;Uzay Dural Şenoğuz,&nbsp;İlknur Özalp Türetgen","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12608","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12608","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research on the backlash effect predominantly investigated penalties men and women incurred when they violate gender norms in the domain of achievement-oriented aggressiveness. We investigated backlash reactions towards female versus male leaders' attempts to manage follower emotions using one of two gender-stereotypic interpersonal emotion management strategies, cognitive change or expression suppression, in a 2×2 vignette experiment in which undergraduate students as participants acted as followers (<i>N</i> = 206). We hypothesized that followers high in explicit or implicit prejudice towards female leadership would be motivated to show backlash in the form of negative attitudes and anger when female leaders use an expression suppression strategy and when male leaders use a cognitive change strategy, violating gender norms. We also explored the role of followers' gender as a boundary condition of backlash reactions towards leaders of the same versus opposite sex. Male participants with negative explicit attitudes towards women leaders in general expressed higher levels of anger towards a female leader who utilized a suppression strategy. Female participants holding implicit stereotypes reported negative attitudes for both female and male leaders who utilized a gender-incongruent emotion management strategy. We discuss the implications of our findings for theory and research on the backlash effect.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"454-472"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140169991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How can previous intergroup contact predict willingness for intergroup interaction? The mediating role of specific intergroup emotions 以往的群体间接触如何预测群体间互动的意愿?特定群体间情绪的中介作用
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-03-13 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12607
Lingjie Tang, Chang'an Zhang, Zhifang Liu

Prior research recognizes intergroup emotions as crucial for intergroup attitudes and interactions, but the psychological mechanisms linking prior intergroup interactions to contact intentions remain incompletely understood. This study investigated the joint effects and interplay of Chinese students' positive and negative contact on behavioural intentions to interact with international students in the future. Six affective variables (fear, anger, anxiety, empathy, happiness, and trust) were tested as mediators. Results revealed that positive contact directly predicted stronger contact willingness and indirectly affected this variable through lower levels of fear, anger, and anxiety and higher levels of empathy, happiness, and trust. Negative contact, however, was negatively associated with positive emotions and contact willingness and predicted more negative emotions. All intergroup emotions played significant mediating roles in the relationship between intergroup contact and willingness for future contact, and anxiety, fear, and trust emerged as robust mediators. Notably, positive contact had larger effects as compared to negative contact. Moreover, no association was observed between negative contact and intergroup emotions when positive contact was frequent. However, when both positive and negative contact were at higher levels, positive contact emerged as a more influential predictor of all intergroup emotions. This research emphasizes examining both positive and negative contact and affective variables as contact mediators, shedding light on enhancing contact intentions.

先前的研究认为,群体间的情感对群体间的态度和互动至关重要,但对先前的群体间互动与接触意向之间的心理机制仍不甚了解。本研究调查了中国学生正面和负面接触对未来与留学生互动的行为意向的共同影响和相互作用。研究测试了六个情感变量(恐惧、愤怒、焦虑、同情、快乐和信任)作为中介变量。结果显示,积极接触直接预测了更强的接触意愿,并通过较低的恐惧、愤怒和焦虑水平以及较高的移情、快乐和信任水平间接影响了这一变量。然而,消极接触与积极情绪和接触意愿呈负相关,并预示着更多的消极情绪。在群体间接触与未来接触意愿之间的关系中,所有群体间情绪都起着重要的中介作用,而焦虑、恐惧和信任则是强有力的中介因素。值得注意的是,与消极接触相比,积极接触的影响更大。此外,当积极接触频繁时,消极接触与群体间情绪之间没有关联。然而,当积极接触和消极接触都处于较高水平时,积极接触对所有群际情绪的预测影响更大。这项研究强调将积极和消极接触以及情感变量作为接触中介进行研究,从而为增强接触意向提供启示。
{"title":"How can previous intergroup contact predict willingness for intergroup interaction? The mediating role of specific intergroup emotions","authors":"Lingjie Tang,&nbsp;Chang'an Zhang,&nbsp;Zhifang Liu","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12607","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12607","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Prior research recognizes intergroup emotions as crucial for intergroup attitudes and interactions, but the psychological mechanisms linking prior intergroup interactions to contact intentions remain incompletely understood. This study investigated the joint effects and interplay of Chinese students' positive and negative contact on behavioural intentions to interact with international students in the future. Six affective variables (fear, anger, anxiety, empathy, happiness, and trust) were tested as mediators. Results revealed that positive contact directly predicted stronger contact willingness and indirectly affected this variable through lower levels of fear, anger, and anxiety and higher levels of empathy, happiness, and trust. Negative contact, however, was negatively associated with positive emotions and contact willingness and predicted more negative emotions. All intergroup emotions played significant mediating roles in the relationship between intergroup contact and willingness for future contact, and anxiety, fear, and trust emerged as robust mediators. Notably, positive contact had larger effects as compared to negative contact. Moreover, no association was observed between negative contact and intergroup emotions when positive contact was frequent. However, when both positive and negative contact were at higher levels, positive contact emerged as a more influential predictor of all intergroup emotions. This research emphasizes examining both positive and negative contact and affective variables as contact mediators, shedding light on enhancing contact intentions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"435-453"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-03-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140247772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A dual-angle exploration towards understanding lapses in COVID-19 social responsibility 为理解 COVID-19 社会责任缺失而进行的双角度探索
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12605
Sean T. H. Lee, Jerome J. X. Mah, Angela K.-y. Leung

Breaking infection chains requires not just behaviours that allow individuals to stay healthy and uninfected (i.e. health protective behaviours) but also for those who are possibly infected to protect others from their harboured infection risk (i.e. socially responsible behaviours). However, socially responsible behaviours entail costs without clear, immediate benefits to the individual, such that public health-risking lapses occur from time to time. In this important yet understudied area, the current exploratory study sought to identify possible psychological factors that may affect people's likelihood of engaging in socially responsible behaviours. Assuming that self-perceived infection should provide an impetus to engage in socially responsible behaviours, we contend that lapses could occur in two scenarios: discounting of possible infection or prioritizing self-interest over collective good. Through a vignette portraying COVID-19 relevant symptoms presented to culturally diverse participants (Singapore and United States; N = 645), we found dispositional denialism (an ego defence mechanism) to exert a negative indirect effect on likelihood of engaging in socially responsible behaviours through its negative association with perceived infection status. Further, social value orientation and cultural orientation appeared to significantly moderate the positive association between perceived infection status and the likelihood of engaging in socially responsible behaviours, such that the positive association held only when individuals espouse both a prosocial value orientation and a collectivistic cultural orientation. Further analyses also point toward a possible attenuation of this positive association when individuals espouse a vertical cultural orientation. Future directions and implications for public health management are discussed.

打破感染链不仅需要让个人保持健康、不受感染的行为(即健康保护行为),还需要那些可能受感染的人保护他人免受其隐藏的感染风险(即对社会负责的行为)。然而,对社会负责的行为需要付出代价,但对个人却没有明显的直接益处,因此公共卫生风险失误时有发生。在这一重要但研究不足的领域,本探索性研究试图找出可能影响人们参与社会责任行为可能性的心理因素。假设自我感觉受到感染应该是参与社会责任行为的动力,我们认为,失误可能发生在两种情况下:对可能的感染打折扣或将自身利益置于集体利益之上。通过向不同文化背景的参与者(新加坡和美国;N = 645)展示 COVID-19 相关症状的小故事,我们发现处置性否认主义(一种自我防御机制)通过其与感知感染状态的负相关,对参与社会责任行为的可能性产生了负面的间接影响。此外,社会价值取向和文化取向似乎在很大程度上缓和了感知感染状况与参与社会责任行为的可能性之间的正相关关系,只有当个体同时拥护亲社会价值取向和集体主义文化取向时,这种正相关关系才会成立。进一步的分析还表明,当个体信奉纵向文化取向时,这种正相关可能会减弱。本文讨论了公共卫生管理的未来方向和影响。
{"title":"A dual-angle exploration towards understanding lapses in COVID-19 social responsibility","authors":"Sean T. H. Lee,&nbsp;Jerome J. X. Mah,&nbsp;Angela K.-y. Leung","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12605","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12605","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Breaking infection chains requires not just behaviours that allow individuals to stay healthy and uninfected (i.e. health protective behaviours) but also for those who are possibly infected to protect others from their harboured infection risk (i.e. socially responsible behaviours). However, socially responsible behaviours entail costs without clear, immediate benefits to the individual, such that public health-risking lapses occur from time to time. In this important yet understudied area, the current exploratory study sought to identify possible psychological factors that may affect people's likelihood of engaging in socially responsible behaviours. Assuming that self-perceived infection should provide an impetus to engage in socially responsible behaviours, we contend that lapses could occur in two scenarios: discounting of possible infection or prioritizing self-interest over collective good. Through a vignette portraying COVID-19 relevant symptoms presented to culturally diverse participants (Singapore and United States; <i>N</i> = 645), we found dispositional denialism (an ego defence mechanism) to exert a negative indirect effect on likelihood of engaging in socially responsible behaviours through its negative association with perceived infection status. Further, social value orientation and cultural orientation appeared to significantly moderate the positive association between perceived infection status and the likelihood of engaging in socially responsible behaviours, such that the positive association held only when individuals espouse both a prosocial value orientation and a collectivistic cultural orientation. Further analyses also point toward a possible attenuation of this positive association when individuals espouse a vertical cultural orientation. Future directions and implications for public health management are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"422-434"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ajsp.12605","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139979104","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do good relationships contribute to innovative behaviour? A study of relationship conflict affecting team innovative behaviour 良好关系有助于创新行为吗?影响团队创新行为的关系冲突研究
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-02-11 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12606
Po-Chien Chang, Xun Xu, Xiaoxiao Gao, Amber Yun-Ping Lee

This study examines the relationship between relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour with transactive memory system (TMS) as a mediator and team-oriented leadership as a moderator. Adopting a two-wave data collection procedure, 584 team members and 113 team leaders from 20 organizations participated in this study. The PROCESS macro for SPSS was used to test the hypothesized relationships. The results indicate that the TMS mediates the relationship between relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour. Moreover, team-oriented leadership moderates the relationship between relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour as well as between relationship conflict and the transactive memory system. Additionally, it moderates the indirect effect of relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour via the TMS. In particular, all three relationships become weaker when team-oriented leadership is high. Based on social information processing theory, this study makes an additional contribution to the current conflict management literature by providing empirical evidence on the mediating role of the TMS and the moderating role of team-oriented leadership in how relationship conflict affects cross-functional team innovative behaviour.

本研究以交互式记忆系统(TMS)为中介,以团队导向型领导力为调节,探讨了关系冲突与跨职能团队创新行为之间的关系。本研究采用两波数据收集程序,共有来自 20 家组织的 584 名团队成员和 113 名团队领导参与。研究使用 SPSS 的 PROCESS 宏来检验假设的关系。结果表明,TMS 在关系冲突与跨职能团队创新行为之间起到了中介作用。此外,团队导向型领导对关系冲突与跨职能团队创新行为之间的关系以及关系冲突与交互式记忆系统之间的关系起到了调节作用。此外,它还通过 TMS 调节了关系冲突与跨职能团队创新行为之间的间接影响。特别是,当团队导向型领导力较高时,这三种关系都会变弱。本研究以社会信息处理理论为基础,提供了有关 TMS 的中介作用和团队导向型领导力对关系冲突如何影响跨职能团队创新行为的调节作用的实证证据,为当前的冲突管理文献做出了新的贡献。
{"title":"Do good relationships contribute to innovative behaviour? A study of relationship conflict affecting team innovative behaviour","authors":"Po-Chien Chang,&nbsp;Xun Xu,&nbsp;Xiaoxiao Gao,&nbsp;Amber Yun-Ping Lee","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12606","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12606","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examines the relationship between relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour with transactive memory system (TMS) as a mediator and team-oriented leadership as a moderator. Adopting a two-wave data collection procedure, 584 team members and 113 team leaders from 20 organizations participated in this study. The PROCESS macro for SPSS was used to test the hypothesized relationships. The results indicate that the TMS mediates the relationship between relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour. Moreover, team-oriented leadership moderates the relationship between relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour as well as between relationship conflict and the transactive memory system. Additionally, it moderates the indirect effect of relationship conflict and cross-functional team innovative behaviour via the TMS. In particular, all three relationships become weaker when team-oriented leadership is high. Based on social information processing theory, this study makes an additional contribution to the current conflict management literature by providing empirical evidence on the mediating role of the TMS and the moderating role of team-oriented leadership in how relationship conflict affects cross-functional team innovative behaviour.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"408-421"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139766123","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not all hands get hot: Success rates and hot-hand predictions 并非所有的手都会变热:成功率和热手预测
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-29 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12603
João Niza Braga, Sofia Jacinto

When predicting someone's performance, people expect that short runs of consistent successful outcomes will continue—the hot-hand. This tendency has been shown in contexts where athletes show a local performance streak, but no other information about their performance is provided. In real-life settings, performance predictions often use global-performance records like success-rate probabilities, although judgements often neglect such statistical information. Aimed at understanding psychological momentums, in a classical sports domain the present work explores how global-performance information (success rates) about an athlete impacts intentionality judgements and moderate predictions of success after a streak. Four studies show that (1) although participants tend to predict the continuation of streaks of success, they are less likely to predict that successful streaks will continue when success rates are low (vs. high or unknown); (2) sensitiveness to local performance's consistency affects perceived ability for high-success rate athletes and perceived effort for low success-rate athletes; (3) the mediation model describing that intentionality attributions mediate the effect of global success-rate information on performance predictions fits the data. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.

在预测一个人的表现时,人们会预期短时间内持续成功的结果会持续下去--这就是 "热手"。在运动员表现出局部连胜,但没有提供有关其表现的其他信息时,这种倾向就会显现出来。在现实生活中,对成绩的预测通常会使用全局成绩记录,如成功率概率,尽管判断往往会忽略此类统计信息。为了理解心理动量,本研究在经典体育领域探讨了运动员的总体成绩信息(成功率)如何影响连胜后的意向性判断和对成功的适度预测。四项研究表明:(1) 虽然参与者倾向于预测连续成功的持续性,但当成功率较低时(相对于成功率较高或未知),他们不太可能预测成功的连续性会持续下去;(2) 对局部表现一致性的敏感性会影响高成功率运动员的感知能力和低成功率运动员的感知努力;(3) 描述意向性归因对全球成功率信息对表现预测的影响的中介模型符合数据。本文讨论了理论和实践意义。
{"title":"Not all hands get hot: Success rates and hot-hand predictions","authors":"João Niza Braga,&nbsp;Sofia Jacinto","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12603","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12603","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When predicting someone's performance, people expect that short runs of consistent successful outcomes will continue—the hot-hand. This tendency has been shown in contexts where athletes show a local performance streak, but no other information about their performance is provided. In real-life settings, performance predictions often use global-performance records like success-rate probabilities, although judgements often neglect such statistical information. Aimed at understanding psychological momentums, in a classical sports domain the present work explores how global-performance information (success rates) about an athlete impacts intentionality judgements and moderate predictions of success after a streak. Four studies show that (1) although participants tend to predict the continuation of streaks of success, they are less likely to predict that successful streaks will continue when success rates are low (vs. high or unknown); (2) sensitiveness to local performance's consistency affects perceived ability for high-success rate athletes and perceived effort for low success-rate athletes; (3) the mediation model describing that intentionality attributions mediate the effect of global success-rate information on performance predictions fits the data. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"391-407"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139585228","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The social evaluation of accents and perceived social influence in Singapore: A comparison of American and Singaporean English accents 新加坡对口音的社会评价和感知的社会影响:美国和新加坡英语口音的比较
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-21 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12600
Matthew H. S. Ng, Chi-Ying Cheng

Accents are an important differentiator between groups which influence social perception and interaction, especially in a diverse country like Singapore. Social identity theory suggests that individuals would exhibit favoritism towards their own accents. However, the accent prestige theory demonstrates instances whereby foreign accents are perceived as more prestigious than one's own accent and are associated with more positive characteristics. This paper sought to explore which of these two theories is more prevalent in Singapore by comparing the perceptions of American English accents and local Singaporean English accents along the competence-warmth paradigm of the Stereotype Content Model. Further, the current research also sought to examine the mediating effect of competence/warmth on the relationship between accents and social influence. Study 1 showed that Singaporeans perceived American-accented speakers as significantly more competent (but not warmer) than local accented speakers. Furthermore, increased perceived competence significantly mediated the relationship between American accents and social influence. Study 2 further investigated perceived threat as a potential moderator in this mediating relationship. The findings replicated the accent-competence-social influence relations in Study 1 but found no effect of perceived threat. Taken together, the findings supported accent prestige towards American accents within Singapore. Theoretical and practical implications were discussed.

口音是区分群体的重要标志,影响着社会认知和互动,尤其是在新加坡这样一个多元化的国家。社会认同理论认为,个人会偏爱自己的口音。然而,口音声望理论则表明,外国口音被认为比自己的口音更有声望,并与更多积极特征联系在一起。本文试图通过比较美国英语口音和新加坡本地英语口音在刻板印象内容模型的能力-温暖范式下的看法,来探讨这两种理论中哪一种在新加坡更为普遍。此外,本研究还试图探讨能力/温暖对口音与社会影响力之间关系的中介效应。研究 1 显示,新加坡人认为说美式口音的人明显比说本地口音的人更有能力(但不温暖)。此外,感知能力的提高在很大程度上调节了美式口音与社会影响力之间的关系。研究 2 进一步调查了感知威胁作为这种中介关系的潜在调节因素的情况。研究结果重复了研究 1 中口音-能力-社会影响力之间的关系,但没有发现感知威胁的影响。综上所述,研究结果支持新加坡人对美国口音的声望。研究还讨论了理论和实践意义。
{"title":"The social evaluation of accents and perceived social influence in Singapore: A comparison of American and Singaporean English accents","authors":"Matthew H. S. Ng,&nbsp;Chi-Ying Cheng","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12600","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12600","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Accents are an important differentiator between groups which influence social perception and interaction, especially in a diverse country like Singapore. Social identity theory suggests that individuals would exhibit favoritism towards their own accents. However, the accent prestige theory demonstrates instances whereby foreign accents are perceived as more prestigious than one's own accent and are associated with more positive characteristics. This paper sought to explore which of these two theories is more prevalent in Singapore by comparing the perceptions of American English accents and local Singaporean English accents along the competence-warmth paradigm of the Stereotype Content Model. Further, the current research also sought to examine the mediating effect of competence/warmth on the relationship between accents and social influence. Study 1 showed that Singaporeans perceived American-accented speakers as significantly more competent (but not warmer) than local accented speakers. Furthermore, increased perceived competence significantly mediated the relationship between American accents and social influence. Study 2 further investigated perceived threat as a potential moderator in this mediating relationship. The findings replicated the accent-competence-social influence relations in Study 1 but found no effect of perceived threat. Taken together, the findings supported accent prestige towards American accents within Singapore. Theoretical and practical implications were discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"376-390"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139561821","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Non-negligible levels of implicit skin tone bias among Australian healthcare workers between 2007 and 2022: Analysis of subgroups and trends over time based on Project Implicit data 2007 年至 2022 年间澳大利亚医护人员中不可忽略的隐性肤色偏差水平:基于隐性项目数据的亚组分析和长期趋势分析
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-14 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12602
Anastasia Ejova, Natasha R. van Antwerpen, Carolyn Semmler, Christopher G. Bean, Deanne M. Green

Implicit (i.e. unconscious) racial biases held by health professionals negatively affect patient–practitioner communication and health outcomes. Implicit biases are typically assessed through implicit association tests (IATs). We extracted cross-sectional IAT data originating in Australia from two large publicly available data sets hosted by Project Implicit. In total, IATs were available from 1956 healthcare workers (1249 of European ethnicity; 829 professionals/technicians as opposed to support workers). All had participated between 2006 and 2022. Levels of implicit skin tone bias were found to be moderate according to Project Implicit guidelines and substantially greater than zero (d = 0.87). Participants of European ethnicity and older age scored higher, but there were no differences across occupation types (professional vs. support worker) or over years. Consistent with findings around the world, the findings highlight a need for interventions that could reduce implicit bias and thereby improve the healthcare experiences of people from minority groups.

医护人员所持有的隐性(即无意识的)种族偏见会对患者与医护人员之间的沟通和健康结果产生负面影响。内隐偏见通常通过内隐关联测试(IATs)来评估。我们从 "内隐项目"(Project Implicit)托管的两个大型公开数据集中提取了来自澳大利亚的横截面 IAT 数据。共有 1956 名医护人员(1249 名欧洲裔;829 名专业人员/技术人员,而非辅助人员)提供了 IAT 数据。他们都是在 2006 年至 2022 年期间参与调查的。根据 "隐性项目 "指南,发现隐性肤色偏差的程度为中度,且大大高于零(d = 0.87)。欧洲裔和年龄较大的参与者得分较高,但不同职业类型(专业人员与辅助人员)或不同年份的参与者之间没有差异。这些研究结果与世界各地的研究结果一致,强调需要采取干预措施来减少隐性偏见,从而改善少数群体的医疗保健体验。
{"title":"Non-negligible levels of implicit skin tone bias among Australian healthcare workers between 2007 and 2022: Analysis of subgroups and trends over time based on Project Implicit data","authors":"Anastasia Ejova,&nbsp;Natasha R. van Antwerpen,&nbsp;Carolyn Semmler,&nbsp;Christopher G. Bean,&nbsp;Deanne M. Green","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12602","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12602","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Implicit (i.e. unconscious) racial biases held by health professionals negatively affect patient–practitioner communication and health outcomes. Implicit biases are typically assessed through implicit association tests (IATs). We extracted cross-sectional IAT data originating in Australia from two large publicly available data sets hosted by <i>Project Implicit</i>. In total, IATs were available from 1956 healthcare workers (1249 of European ethnicity; 829 professionals/technicians as opposed to support workers). All had participated between 2006 and 2022. Levels of implicit skin tone bias were found to be moderate according to <i>Project Implicit</i> guidelines and substantially greater than zero (<i>d</i> = 0.87). Participants of European ethnicity and older age scored higher, but there were no differences across occupation types (professional vs. support worker) or over years. Consistent with findings around the world, the findings highlight a need for interventions that could reduce implicit bias and thereby improve the healthcare experiences of people from minority groups.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"360-375"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ajsp.12602","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139470197","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
High relational mobility is associated with perceiving more economic inequality in everyday life 高关系流动性与在日常生活中感受到更多的经济不平等有关
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-13 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12597
Guillermo B. Willis, Yukiko Uchida, Juan Diego García-Castro, Kosuke Takemura

In this paper, we argue that one of the variables that may influence perceived inequality in everyday life is cultural differences in relational mobility. We examined this research question across two cross-cultural studies in Japan (Study 1, N = 196; Study 2, N = 214) and Spain (Study 1, N = 199; Study 2, 199), two countries that differ in their relational mobility levels. In Study 1, we contrasted two competing hypotheses: On the one hand, it could be predicted that individuals in countries with high relational mobility perceive greater inequality due to increased exposure to diverse social networks. On the other hand, it is also plausible that in contexts of high relational mobility, people perceive less inequality as they selectively choose to interact with individuals who share similar social backgrounds. Results supported the former hypotheses and showed that people in a country high in relational mobility (i.e. Spain) tend to perceive more economic inequality (vs. Japan, a country low in relational mobility). Moreover, we found an indirect effect of this cross-cultural difference through the cultural affordances for meeting new people (i.e. one of the dimensions of relational mobility). In Study 2, we preregistered and replicated these results.

在本文中,我们认为可能影响日常生活中感知到的不平等的变量之一是关系流动中的文化差异。我们在日本(研究 1,人数 = 196;研究 2,人数 = 214)和西班牙(研究 1,人数 = 199;研究 2,人数 = 199)进行了两项跨文化研究,探讨了这一研究问题。在研究 1 中,我们对比了两个相互竞争的假设:一方面,我们可以预测,关系流动性高的国家中的个人由于接触到更多不同的社会网络,因而会感受到更大的不平等。另一方面,在关系流动性高的情况下,人们会选择与社会背景相似的人交往,从而减少不平等感。结果支持了前一种假设,并表明在关系流动性较高的国家(如西班牙),人们倾向于感知到更多的经济不平等(相对于关系流动性较低的国家日本)。此外,我们还发现,这种跨文化差异会通过结识新朋友的文化承受能力(即关系流动性的一个维度)产生间接影响。在研究 2 中,我们预先注册并复制了这些结果。
{"title":"High relational mobility is associated with perceiving more economic inequality in everyday life","authors":"Guillermo B. Willis,&nbsp;Yukiko Uchida,&nbsp;Juan Diego García-Castro,&nbsp;Kosuke Takemura","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12597","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12597","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In this paper, we argue that one of the variables that may influence perceived inequality in everyday life is cultural differences in relational mobility. We examined this research question across two cross-cultural studies in Japan (Study 1, <i>N</i> = 196; Study 2, <i>N</i> = 214) and Spain (Study 1, <i>N</i> = 199; Study 2, 199), two countries that differ in their relational mobility levels. In Study 1, we contrasted two competing hypotheses: On the one hand, it could be predicted that individuals in countries with high relational mobility perceive greater inequality due to increased exposure to diverse social networks. On the other hand, it is also plausible that in contexts of high relational mobility, people perceive less inequality as they selectively choose to interact with individuals who share similar social backgrounds. Results supported the former hypotheses and showed that people in a country high in relational mobility (i.e. Spain) tend to perceive more economic inequality (vs. Japan, a country low in relational mobility). Moreover, we found an indirect effect of this cross-cultural difference through the cultural affordances for meeting new people (i.e. one of the dimensions of relational mobility). In Study 2, we preregistered and replicated these results.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"348-359"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ajsp.12597","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139461620","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The violent turn in non-violent collective action: What happens? 非暴力集体行动中的暴力转向:会发生什么?
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-07 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12596
Sik Hung Ng, Michael J. Platow

Collective actions (e.g., protest marches, social movements) that begin peacefully sometimes turn into violent clashes between demonstrators and police, with alarming consequences. Research on the violent turn, informed by intergroup social psychology and micro-sociology, has made significant but separate advances. We review them jointly to form an integrated, fuller understanding of the violent turn and promote interdisciplinary dialogue by discussing their respective strengths and identifying areas where they complement each other. For these purposes we first extract from the psychology of interpersonal aggression literature a three-phase temporal pattern of aggression escalation to provide a common reference for reviewing representative studies and theories in intergroup social psychology (e.g., elaborated social identity model of crowd rioting, intergroup emotions theory) and micro-sociology (e.g., micro-sociological theory of violence, emotional dynamics and behavioural dynamics). The results of our analysis show a sequence of events beginning from instigators and their impellance in the early phase of the violent turn, psychological transformations forming a proclivity to aggress in the middle phase, and collective empowerment that disinhibits violence in the late phase. Group power and, to a less extent, group emotions permeate all phases. The sequence of events reveals transitional points where intervention may be applied to avert or abort violence.

以和平方式开始的集体行动(如抗议游行、社会运动)有时会演变成示威者与警察之间的暴力冲突,后果令人担忧。受群体间社会心理学和微观社会学的启发,有关暴力转向的研究取得了重大但独立的进展。我们对它们进行联合评述,通过讨论它们各自的优势并确定它们相互补充的领域,形成对暴力转向的综合、更全面的理解,并促进跨学科对话。为此,我们首先从人际攻击心理学文献中提取了攻击升级的三阶段时间模式,为回顾群体间社会心理学(如人群骚乱的社会认同模型、群体间情绪理论)和微观社会学(如暴力的微观社会学理论、情绪动力学和行为动力学)中的代表性研究和理论提供共同参考。我们的分析结果表明,从暴力转向早期阶段的煽动者及其推动力开始,到中期阶段形成攻击倾向的心理转变,再到晚期阶段抑制暴力的集体赋权,一系列事件相继发生。在所有阶段中都渗透着群体力量,其次是群体情感。事件的发展顺序揭示了可采取干预措施以避免或中止暴力的过渡点。
{"title":"The violent turn in non-violent collective action: What happens?","authors":"Sik Hung Ng,&nbsp;Michael J. Platow","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12596","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12596","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Collective actions (e.g., protest marches, social movements) that begin peacefully sometimes turn into violent clashes between demonstrators and police, with alarming consequences. Research on the violent turn, informed by intergroup social psychology and micro-sociology, has made significant but separate advances. We review them jointly to form an integrated, fuller understanding of the violent turn and promote interdisciplinary dialogue by discussing their respective strengths and identifying areas where they complement each other. For these purposes we first extract from the psychology of interpersonal aggression literature a three-phase temporal pattern of aggression escalation to provide a common reference for reviewing representative studies and theories in intergroup social psychology (e.g., elaborated social identity model of crowd rioting, intergroup emotions theory) and micro-sociology (e.g., micro-sociological theory of violence, emotional dynamics and behavioural dynamics). The results of our analysis show a sequence of events beginning from instigators and their impellance in the early phase of the violent turn, psychological transformations forming a proclivity to aggress in the middle phase, and collective empowerment that disinhibits violence in the late phase. Group power and, to a less extent, group emotions permeate all phases. The sequence of events reveals transitional points where intervention may be applied to avert or abort violence.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"303-317"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ajsp.12596","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139397567","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Money or health? The effect of pathogen avoidance motives and life history strategies on health-economic trade-offs during the COVID-19 pandemic 金钱还是健康?在 COVID-19 大流行期间,病原体回避动机和生活史策略对健康经济权衡的影响
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-07 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12601
Qingyi Ma, Tingting Ji, Yongyu Guo

The COVID-19 pandemic has caused governments and individuals to face important but difficult trade-offs between health and the economy. How do individuals choose between health and economic outcomes during the pandemic? Based on the behavioural immune system (BIS) theory and the life history (LH) theory, the present study examined the effects of individual differences in pathogen disgust sensitivity and life history strategy on people's health-economic trade-offs during the COVID-19 pandemic. Results of an online study (N = 300) showed that people with higher pathogen disgust sensitivity felt less sense of control during the pandemic, and therefore chose health-related options over economic-related options. In addition, the association between pathogen disgust sensitivity and health outcome preference only existed in people with relatively faster life history strategies. Further, people's health-economic trade-offs were not influenced by their current economic status. Findings have important implications for policymakers and the public to understand people's health-economic choices during the pandemic.

COVID-19 大流行使政府和个人在健康和经济之间面临重要而艰难的权衡。在大流行期间,个人如何在健康和经济结果之间做出选择?本研究以行为免疫系统(BIS)理论和生活史(LH)理论为基础,探讨了病原体厌恶敏感性和生活史策略的个体差异对 COVID-19 大流行期间人们健康与经济权衡的影响。一项在线研究(N = 300)的结果显示,病原体厌恶敏感度较高的人在大流行期间的控制感较低,因此选择了与健康相关的选项,而不是与经济相关的选项。此外,病原体厌恶敏感度与健康结果偏好之间的关联只存在于生活史策略相对较快的人群中。此外,人们在健康与经济之间的权衡并不受当前经济状况的影响。研究结果对政策制定者和公众了解大流行病期间人们的健康-经济选择具有重要意义。
{"title":"Money or health? The effect of pathogen avoidance motives and life history strategies on health-economic trade-offs during the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"Qingyi Ma,&nbsp;Tingting Ji,&nbsp;Yongyu Guo","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12601","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12601","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The COVID-19 pandemic has caused governments and individuals to face important but difficult trade-offs between health and the economy. How do individuals choose between health and economic outcomes during the pandemic? Based on the behavioural immune system (BIS) theory and the life history (LH) theory, the present study examined the effects of individual differences in pathogen disgust sensitivity and life history strategy on people's health-economic trade-offs during the COVID-19 pandemic. Results of an online study (<i>N</i> = 300) showed that people with higher pathogen disgust sensitivity felt less sense of control during the pandemic, and therefore chose health-related options over economic-related options. In addition, the association between pathogen disgust sensitivity and health outcome preference only existed in people with relatively faster life history strategies. Further, people's health-economic trade-offs were not influenced by their current economic status. Findings have important implications for policymakers and the public to understand people's health-economic choices during the pandemic.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"335-347"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139397814","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Asian Journal of Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1