首页 > 最新文献

Asian Journal of Social Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The relationship among cultural variables and weight issues for Asian American women 亚裔美国妇女的文化变量与体重问题之间的关系
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2024-01-07 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12599
Megan Tsutakawa, Glenn Gamst, Jerry L. Kernes, Aghop Der Karabetian

The present study investigated whether multicultural variables derived from the Multicultural Assessment-Intervention Process model (shifting, gender roles, acculturation, perceived discrimination) could predict levels of internalized weight bias (IWB) or disordered eating behaviours in 429 Asian American adult women. A facet of shifting, White beauty conformity and mainstream acculturation were the only multicultural variables that significantly predicted all five body- and eating-related outcomes (IWB, restraint, eating, shape and weight concerns). Gender roles and two aspects of perceived discrimination (social exclusion and stigmatization) also showed predictive ability for one or more of the outcome measures. Results highlight the relationship between multicultural and body- and eating-related variables for Asian American women and underscore the salience of body shape and weight, the importance placed on conforming to Western culture and beauty standards and the detrimental effects of doing so for this population. Clinical implications and future research are discussed.

本研究调查了从多元文化评估-干预过程模型中衍生出的多元文化变量(转变、性别角色、文化适应、感知到的歧视)能否预测 429 名亚裔美国成年女性的内化体重偏差(IWB)或饮食行为紊乱水平。在多元文化变量中,只有 "转变 "的一个方面、"符合白人审美 "和 "主流文化 "能显著预测与身体和饮食有关的所有五种结果(内化体重偏差、克制、饮食、体形和体重问题)。性别角色和感知到的歧视的两个方面(社会排斥和污名化)也显示了对一个或多个结果测量的预测能力。研究结果突显了亚裔美国妇女的多元文化与身体和饮食相关变量之间的关系,并强调了体形和体重的重要性、符合西方文化和审美标准的重要性以及这样做对亚裔美国妇女的不利影响。本文还讨论了临床影响和未来研究。
{"title":"The relationship among cultural variables and weight issues for Asian American women","authors":"Megan Tsutakawa,&nbsp;Glenn Gamst,&nbsp;Jerry L. Kernes,&nbsp;Aghop Der Karabetian","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12599","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12599","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The present study investigated whether multicultural variables derived from the Multicultural Assessment-Intervention Process model (shifting, gender roles, acculturation, perceived discrimination) could predict levels of internalized weight bias (IWB) or disordered eating behaviours in 429 Asian American adult women. A facet of shifting, White beauty conformity and mainstream acculturation were the only multicultural variables that significantly predicted all five body- and eating-related outcomes (IWB, restraint, eating, shape and weight concerns). Gender roles and two aspects of perceived discrimination (social exclusion and stigmatization) also showed predictive ability for one or more of the outcome measures. Results highlight the relationship between multicultural and body- and eating-related variables for Asian American women and underscore the salience of body shape and weight, the importance placed on conforming to Western culture and beauty standards and the detrimental effects of doing so for this population. Clinical implications and future research are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"318-334"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139397571","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
She wants the best: Maximizing tendency, work-to-family enrichment and female employee adaptivity 她想要最好的:最大化倾向、工作与家庭的丰富性和女性员工的适应性
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-12-31 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12598
Huiyuan Jia, Mushi Li

Stereotypes often link female employees with increased family–work conflicts and low work outcomes. Nevertheless, female employees may excel in both home and work environments. This study aimed to examine the effect of maximizing tendency on female employees' family-to-work enrichment and adaptivity. A three-wave survey involving 1074 female employees was conducted. Results indicated that maximizing tendency positively predicts adaptivity via family-to-work enrichment. Spousal relationship quality weakens the effect of maximizing on family-to-work enrichment and employee adaptivity. The findings highlight the benefits of maximizing tendency in female employees, suggesting a bright side to maximizing as it enhances adaptability in dynamic work situations. The study also identifies maximizing tendency as a novel personal characteristic that facilitates resource transfer from family to work domain. The findings offer a valuable perspective for promoting female employees' success in the workplace.

陈规定型观念往往将女性员工与家庭与工作冲突加剧和工作成果低下联系在一起。然而,女性员工可能在家庭和工作环境中都表现出色。本研究旨在探讨最大化倾向对女性员工家庭工作丰富性和适应性的影响。研究共进行了三波调查,涉及 1074 名女性员工。结果表明,最大化倾向通过家庭对工作的丰富性正向预测适应性。配偶关系质量削弱了最大化对家庭工作丰富性和员工适应性的影响。研究结果凸显了最大化倾向对女性员工的益处,表明最大化倾向有其光明的一面,因为它能增强员工在动态工作环境中的适应能力。研究还发现,最大化倾向是一种新的个人特征,有利于资源从家庭转移到工作领域。研究结果为促进女性员工在工作场所取得成功提供了一个有价值的视角。
{"title":"She wants the best: Maximizing tendency, work-to-family enrichment and female employee adaptivity","authors":"Huiyuan Jia,&nbsp;Mushi Li","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12598","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12598","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Stereotypes often link female employees with increased family–work conflicts and low work outcomes. Nevertheless, female employees may excel in both home and work environments. This study aimed to examine the effect of maximizing tendency on female employees' family-to-work enrichment and adaptivity. A three-wave survey involving 1074 female employees was conducted. Results indicated that maximizing tendency positively predicts adaptivity via family-to-work enrichment. Spousal relationship quality weakens the effect of maximizing on family-to-work enrichment and employee adaptivity. The findings highlight the benefits of maximizing tendency in female employees, suggesting a bright side to maximizing as it enhances adaptability in dynamic work situations. The study also identifies maximizing tendency as a novel personal characteristic that facilitates resource transfer from family to work domain. The findings offer a valuable perspective for promoting female employees' success in the workplace.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"279-291"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139071035","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Time Perception Scale: Measurement invariance between the United States and Japan 时间感量表:美国和日本的测量不变性
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-12-31 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12594
Syamil Yakin, Yu Niiya

The Time Perception Scale (Niiya, 2019, Journal of Happiness Studies) measures people's perception of time as zero-sum (i.e., the perception that they are taking time away from others, that they are offering time to others, or that others are taking away time from them) and nonzero-sum (i.e., the perception that time spent on others is time spent on themselves). This scale was validated in Japan, but whether people from a non-Asian culture also perceive time as nonzero-sum remained unknown. We tested the measurement invariance of the Time Perception Scale with 189 Americans and 240 Japanese and examined its correlations with psychological well-being and cultural self-construals. We confirmed the configural, metric, and partial scalar invariance of the scale and also found that zero-sum and nonzero-sum time perceptions were differently associated with basic needs satisfaction, life satisfaction, and interdependent self-construal in both cultures. Nonzero-sum time perception may provide a new possibility where a concept derived from East Asian philosophy can contribute to our psychological well-being.

时间感知量表(Niiya,2019年,《幸福研究杂志》)测量人们对时间的零和(即认为自己从他人那里拿走了时间、自己向他人提供了时间或他人从自己那里拿走了时间)和非零和(即认为花在他人身上的时间就是花在自己身上的时间)感知。该量表已在日本得到验证,但来自非亚洲文化背景的人是否也认为时间是非零和的仍是未知数。我们用 189 名美国人和 240 名日本人测试了时间感知量表的测量不变性,并研究了它与心理健康和文化自我概念的相关性。我们证实了该量表在配置、度量和部分标度上的不变性,还发现在两种文化中,零和时间感知和非零和时间感知与基本需求满意度、生活满意度和相互依存的自我建构有着不同的关联。非零和时间观念可能提供了一种新的可能性,即源自东亚哲学的概念可以促进我们的心理健康。
{"title":"Time Perception Scale: Measurement invariance between the United States and Japan","authors":"Syamil Yakin,&nbsp;Yu Niiya","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12594","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12594","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The Time Perception Scale (Niiya, 2019, <i>Journal of Happiness Studies</i>) measures people's perception of time as zero-sum (i.e., the perception that they are taking time away from others, that they are offering time to others, or that others are taking away time from them) and nonzero-sum (i.e., the perception that time spent on others is time spent on themselves). This scale was validated in Japan, but whether people from a non-Asian culture also perceive time as nonzero-sum remained unknown. We tested the measurement invariance of the Time Perception Scale with 189 Americans and 240 Japanese and examined its correlations with psychological well-being and cultural self-construals. We confirmed the configural, metric, and partial scalar invariance of the scale and also found that zero-sum and nonzero-sum time perceptions were differently associated with basic needs satisfaction, life satisfaction, and interdependent self-construal in both cultures. Nonzero-sum time perception may provide a new possibility where a concept derived from East Asian philosophy can contribute to our psychological well-being.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"292-302"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ajsp.12594","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139071363","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Climate change perception scale: Adaptation and psychometric properties in the Indonesian context 气候变化感知量表:印度尼西亚背景下的适应性和心理测量特性
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-12-17 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12595
Bartolomeus Yofana Adiwena, Maria Bramanwidyantari

One of the crucial factors in changing behaviour towards pro-environmental actions is climate change perception. The latest measurement tool for assessing this variable is the Climate Change Perception Scale (CCPS). However, CCPS has not gained widespread adoption and usage across different cultures, including Indonesia, and no analyses have been conducted to adapt and validate CCPS in the Indonesian context. Therefore, this study aimed to adapt, validate, and assess the measurement tool's consistency in Indonesia's cultural context. To investigate the instrument's psychometric properties, two studies (n = 358, 415) were conducted in Indonesia to provide evidence of the internal consistency, convergent validity, and predictive validity of CCPS. The results of a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) showed that the Indonesian version of CCPS, which consists of 15 items, with three for each dimension, accurately and reliably measured perception of climate change. CCPS also demonstrated good convergent validity, as indicated by the significant correlations with a nature relatedness (r = 0.425, p < 0.01) and biospheric value (r = 0.341, p < 0.01) construct. Furthermore, it exhibited predictive validity by predicting pro-environmental behaviour, β = 0.243, b = 0.176, t(413) = 5.10, p < 0.01. However, the Indonesian version of CCPS still needs to be tested and confirmed through various studies, considering the poor validity of the temporal dimension and the weak factor loading of the consequence dimension in Study 2.

气候变化感知是改变行为、采取环保行动的关键因素之一。评估这一变量的最新测量工具是气候变化感知量表(CCPS)。然而,CCPS 并未在包括印尼在内的不同文化背景中得到广泛采纳和使用,也没有针对印尼情况对 CCPS 进行调整和验证的分析。因此,本研究旨在根据印尼的文化背景对测量工具进行调整、验证和一致性评估。为了调查该工具的心理测量特性,在印尼进行了两项研究(n = 358、415),以证明 CCPS 的内部一致性、收敛效度和预测效度。确证因子分析(CFA)结果表明,印尼版 CCPS 由 15 个项目组成,每个维度有三个项目,能够准确可靠地测量气候变化感知。CCPS 与自然相关性(r = 0.425,p <0.01)和生物圈价值(r = 0.341,p <0.01)之间的显著相关性也表明,CCPS 具有良好的收敛效度。此外,它还通过预测亲环境行为表现出预测有效性,β = 0.243,b = 0.176,t(413) = 5.10,p < 0.01。然而,考虑到研究 2 中时间维度的有效性较差和后果维度的因子负荷较弱,印尼版 CCPS 仍需通过各种研究进行检验和确认。
{"title":"Climate change perception scale: Adaptation and psychometric properties in the Indonesian context","authors":"Bartolomeus Yofana Adiwena,&nbsp;Maria Bramanwidyantari","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12595","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12595","url":null,"abstract":"<p>One of the crucial factors in changing behaviour towards pro-environmental actions is climate change perception. The latest measurement tool for assessing this variable is the Climate Change Perception Scale (CCPS). However, CCPS has not gained widespread adoption and usage across different cultures, including Indonesia, and no analyses have been conducted to adapt and validate CCPS in the Indonesian context. Therefore, this study aimed to adapt, validate, and assess the measurement tool's consistency in Indonesia's cultural context. To investigate the instrument's psychometric properties, two studies (<i>n</i> = 358, 415) were conducted in Indonesia to provide evidence of the internal consistency, convergent validity, and predictive validity of CCPS. The results of a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) showed that the Indonesian version of CCPS, which consists of 15 items, with three for each dimension, accurately and reliably measured perception of climate change. CCPS also demonstrated good convergent validity, as indicated by the significant correlations with a nature relatedness (<i>r</i> = 0.425, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.01) and biospheric value (<i>r</i> = 0.341, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.01) construct. Furthermore, it exhibited predictive validity by predicting pro-environmental behaviour, <i>β</i> = 0.243, <i>b</i> = 0.176, <i>t</i>(413) = 5.10, <i>p</i> &lt; 0.01. However, the Indonesian version of CCPS still needs to be tested and confirmed through various studies, considering the poor validity of the temporal dimension and the weak factor loading of the consequence dimension in Study 2.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"267-278"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138743049","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Skin tone-based stereotyping with Asian targets: Exploring possible mechanisms 基于肤色的亚裔目标刻板印象:探索可能的机制
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-12-14 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12593
Chang Hyun Ha, Sang Hee Park

Research on skin tone bias (i.e., bias against members of the same racial group with different skin tones) has been conducted mostly with Black/White targets. We tested skin tone bias with East Asian (Korean) targets and investigated its possible mechanisms. In Study 1, comparisons of impressions between targets with different skin tones (darker-, medium-, and lighter-skinned) showed statistically significant differences in warmth, competence, morality, cleanliness, dangerousness, strongness, and social status. Study 2 revealed that lighter-skinned Koreans were perceived to be more similar to Whites (vs. Blacks or South Asians), but this perceived similarity did not explain the effect of skin tone on impression ratings. In Study 3, participants showed associations of darker (vs. lighter) skin with rural (vs. urban) areas and with blue-collar (vs. white-collar) jobs. However, these associations explained only the skin tone effects on dangerousness. This research demonstrated skin tone bias with East Asian participants and targets.

有关肤色偏差(即针对同一种族群体中不同肤色成员的偏差)的研究主要是针对黑人/白人目标群体进行的。我们用东亚(韩国)目标测试了肤色偏差,并研究了其可能的机制。在研究 1 中,对不同肤色(深肤色、中等肤色和浅肤色)目标之间的印象进行比较后发现,在热情、能力、道德感、清洁度、危险性、强势和社会地位等方面存在统计学意义上的显著差异。研究 2 显示,肤色较浅的韩国人被认为更像白人(相对于黑人或南亚人),但这种相似性并不能解释肤色对印象评分的影响。在 "研究 3 "中,受试者表现出肤色深(与肤色浅)与农村(与城市)地区和蓝领(与白领)工作的关联。然而,这些关联只能解释肤色对危险性的影响。这项研究显示了东亚参与者和目标的肤色偏差。
{"title":"Skin tone-based stereotyping with Asian targets: Exploring possible mechanisms","authors":"Chang Hyun Ha,&nbsp;Sang Hee Park","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12593","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12593","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research on skin tone bias (i.e., bias against members of the same racial group with different skin tones) has been conducted mostly with Black/White targets. We tested skin tone bias with East Asian (Korean) targets and investigated its possible mechanisms. In Study 1, comparisons of impressions between targets with different skin tones (darker-, medium-, and lighter-skinned) showed statistically significant differences in warmth, competence, morality, cleanliness, dangerousness, strongness, and social status. Study 2 revealed that lighter-skinned Koreans were perceived to be more similar to Whites (vs. Blacks or South Asians), but this perceived similarity did not explain the effect of skin tone on impression ratings. In Study 3, participants showed associations of darker (vs. lighter) skin with rural (vs. urban) areas and with blue-collar (vs. white-collar) jobs. However, these associations explained only the skin tone effects on dangerousness. This research demonstrated skin tone bias with East Asian participants and targets.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 3","pages":"251-266"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2023-12-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138686844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effects of cost and praise-seeking need on motive inference for those who engage in helping behaviours 成本和寻求赞美的需求对参与助人行为者动机推断的影响
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-12-08 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12591
Keisuke Yamamoto, Tomoko Ikegami, Hiroshi Yama

This study revealed determinants that influence motive inference toward helpers who engage in lifesaving. The costly signalling theory contends that higher costs associated with helping behaviour will promote one's inference of altruistic motives toward a helper. We examined how costs affect third parties' motive inference when controlling for benefits to recipients. In addition, in terms of projection theory, we predicted that the stronger the third parties' praise-seeking need, the more likely it is they will infer self-presentational motives toward a helper. We also examined if the praise-seeking need would moderate the effect of cost on motive inference. Two vignette studies using hypothetical scenarios were conducted. In Study 1 (N = 140) and Study 2 (N = 100), the main effect of costs was found to be significant, indicating that the larger the cost, the more likely it is that a third party inferred altruistic motives toward a helper. Moreover, the main effect (but not the moderation effect) of the praise-seeking need was also significant, as predicted above. This study provides stronger support for the costly signalling theory and reveals an individual-difference factor that determines motive inference toward a helper.

本研究揭示了影响对参与救生的求助者的动机推断的决定因素。成本信号理论认为,与助人行为相关的成本越高,人们对助人者的利他动机推断就越高。我们研究了在控制受助者利益的情况下,成本如何影响第三方的动机推断。此外,根据投射理论,我们预测第三方寻求赞美的需求越强烈,他们就越有可能推断出求助者的自我表现动机。我们还研究了寻求赞美的需求是否会缓和成本对动机推断的影响。我们使用假设情景进行了两项小故事研究。在研究 1(N = 140)和研究 2(N = 100)中,我们发现成本的主效应是显著的,这表明成本越大,第三方越有可能推断出求助者的利他动机。此外,如上所述,寻求赞美需求的主效应(而非调节效应)也很显著。本研究为代价信号理论提供了更有力的支持,并揭示了决定对求助者动机推断的个体差异因素。
{"title":"Effects of cost and praise-seeking need on motive inference for those who engage in helping behaviours","authors":"Keisuke Yamamoto,&nbsp;Tomoko Ikegami,&nbsp;Hiroshi Yama","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12591","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12591","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study revealed determinants that influence motive inference toward helpers who engage in lifesaving. The costly signalling theory contends that higher costs associated with helping behaviour will promote one's inference of altruistic motives toward a helper. We examined how costs affect third parties' motive inference when controlling for benefits to recipients. In addition, in terms of projection theory, we predicted that the stronger the third parties' praise-seeking need, the more likely it is they will infer self-presentational motives toward a helper. We also examined if the praise-seeking need would moderate the effect of cost on motive inference. Two vignette studies using hypothetical scenarios were conducted. In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 140) and Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 100), the main effect of costs was found to be significant, indicating that the larger the cost, the more likely it is that a third party inferred altruistic motives toward a helper. Moreover, the main effect (but not the moderation effect) of the praise-seeking need was also significant, as predicted above. This study provides stronger support for the costly signalling theory and reveals an individual-difference factor that determines motive inference toward a helper.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 2","pages":"219-230"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138575902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Collectivistic orientation moderates the effect of personal control on evaluations of societal disease-control measures: During and beyond the COVID-19 crisis in China 集体主义取向调节了个人控制对社会疾病控制措施评价的影响:中国 COVID-19 危机期间和危机之后
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12589
Nan Zhu, Yang Li, Lei Chang

Two pre-registered studies examined people's psychological collectivism, personal control, and attitudes toward disease-control measures in the context of the COVID-19 crisis and more generalized contexts. Study 1 surveyed 819 residents in Shanghai in late May 2022 when they were undergoing a stringent city-wide lockdown caused by an outbreak of the Omicron variant. We found that participants' psychological collectivism attenuated the negative association between experiences of COVID-19 restrictions and personal control and enhanced the positive association between personal control and support for COVID-19 restrictions. Study 2 (N = 403) recruited an online sample with diverse backgrounds and demographic characteristics and sought to extend the findings of Study 1 beyond the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Participants were exposed to a series of hypothetical scenarios depicting a fictitious virus with varying fatality and transmissibility. As in Study 1, participants higher in psychological collectivism exhibited a stronger positive association between personal control and endorsement of stricter societal disease-control measures, but only in low-fatality, high-transmissibility situations. The implications of these findings for facilitating public support for disease-control efforts are discussed.

两项预先登记的研究考察了在 COVID-19 危机和更普遍的背景下人们的心理集体主义、个人控制以及对疾病控制措施的态度。研究1于2022年5月下旬对上海的819名居民进行了调查,当时上海正因奥米克变异体的爆发而进行严格的全城封锁。我们发现,参与者的心理集体主义削弱了COVID-19限制与个人控制之间的负相关,增强了个人控制与支持COVID-19限制之间的正相关。研究 2(N = 403)招募了一个具有不同背景和人口特征的在线样本,并试图将研究 1 的发现扩展到 COVID-19 大流行的背景之外。研究人员向参与者展示了一系列假设情景,这些情景描述了一种致命性和传播性各不相同的虚构病毒。与研究 1 一样,心理集体主义程度较高的参与者在个人控制与认可更严格的社会疾病控制措施之间表现出更强的正相关,但仅限于低致死率、高传播性的情况。本研究讨论了这些发现对促进公众支持疾病控制工作的影响。
{"title":"Collectivistic orientation moderates the effect of personal control on evaluations of societal disease-control measures: During and beyond the COVID-19 crisis in China","authors":"Nan Zhu,&nbsp;Yang Li,&nbsp;Lei Chang","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12589","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12589","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Two pre-registered studies examined people's psychological collectivism, personal control, and attitudes toward disease-control measures in the context of the COVID-19 crisis and more generalized contexts. Study 1 surveyed 819 residents in Shanghai in late May 2022 when they were undergoing a stringent city-wide lockdown caused by an outbreak of the Omicron variant. We found that participants' psychological collectivism attenuated the negative association between experiences of COVID-19 restrictions and personal control and enhanced the positive association between personal control and support for COVID-19 restrictions. Study 2 (<i>N</i> = 403) recruited an online sample with diverse backgrounds and demographic characteristics and sought to extend the findings of Study 1 beyond the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Participants were exposed to a series of hypothetical scenarios depicting a fictitious virus with varying fatality and transmissibility. As in Study 1, participants higher in psychological collectivism exhibited a stronger positive association between personal control and endorsement of stricter societal disease-control measures, but only in low-fatality, high-transmissibility situations. The implications of these findings for facilitating public support for disease-control efforts are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 2","pages":"187-202"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139205180","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Formal versus informal supervisor socio-emotional support behaviours and employee trust: The role of cultural power distance 正式与非正式主管社会情感支持行为与员工信任:文化权力距离的作用
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-11-30 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12590
Jaee Cho, S. Arzu Wasti, Krishna Savani, Hwee Hoon Tan, Michael W. Morris

This research investigates how formal versus informal supervisor support behaviours shape employees' affect- and cognition-based trust across cultures of varying power distance. Using data from in-depth interviews, Study 1 found that trust-enhancing supervisor behaviours were more formal, status conscious and imposing in India (a high power distance culture) than in the Netherlands (a low power distance culture); unlike in India, supervisors acted more like friends or equals with their subordinates in the Netherlands. Using vignettes, Study 2 found that, compared to informal support behaviours, formal support behaviours increased both affect- and cognition-based trust among Indian participants, but among US participants, formal support behaviours only increased cognition-based trust. Study 3 conceptually replicated those findings by manipulating power distance in an organization. Together, the findings from these three studies suggest that supervisors' formal socio-emotional support behaviours are particularly effective in increasing affect-based trust in societal and organizational cultures that are high power distance.

本研究探讨了在不同权力距离的文化中,正式与非正式的主管支持行为如何塑造员工基于情感和认知的信任。利用深度访谈的数据,研究1发现,在印度(高权力距离文化),与荷兰(低权力距离文化)相比,增强信任的主管行为更正式、地位意识更强、更强势;与印度不同,荷兰的主管对待下属更像是朋友或平等。通过小插曲,研究2发现,与非正式支持行为相比,正式支持行为增加了印度参与者基于情感和认知的信任,但在美国参与者中,正式支持行为只增加了基于认知的信任。研究3通过操纵组织中的权力距离,从概念上复制了这些发现。总之,这三项研究的结果表明,在高权力距离的社会和组织文化中,主管的正式社会情感支持行为在增加基于情感的信任方面特别有效。
{"title":"Formal versus informal supervisor socio-emotional support behaviours and employee trust: The role of cultural power distance","authors":"Jaee Cho,&nbsp;S. Arzu Wasti,&nbsp;Krishna Savani,&nbsp;Hwee Hoon Tan,&nbsp;Michael W. Morris","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12590","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12590","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This research investigates how formal versus informal supervisor support behaviours shape employees' affect- and cognition-based trust across cultures of varying power distance. Using data from in-depth interviews, Study 1 found that trust-enhancing supervisor behaviours were more formal, status conscious and imposing in India (a high power distance culture) than in the Netherlands (a low power distance culture); unlike in India, supervisors acted more like friends or equals with their subordinates in the Netherlands. Using vignettes, Study 2 found that, compared to informal support behaviours, formal support behaviours increased both affect- and cognition-based trust among Indian participants, but among US participants, formal support behaviours only increased cognition-based trust. Study 3 conceptually replicated those findings by manipulating power distance in an organization. Together, the findings from these three studies suggest that supervisors' formal socio-emotional support behaviours are particularly effective in increasing affect-based trust in societal and organizational cultures that are high power distance.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 2","pages":"203-218"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138542637","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing self-esteem discrepancies in Pakistan and Canada 比较巴基斯坦和加拿大的自尊差异
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-11-29 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12592
Thomas I. Vaughan-Johnston, Faizan Imtiaz, Li-Jun Ji, Rubina Hanif, Devin I. Fowlie, Jill A. Jacobson

The cross-cultural universality of people's pursuit of positive self-esteem is frequently disputed. Most research in this area has contrasted cultures of dignity (Western) and face cultures (East Asian), but less attention has been given to other cultures' views of self-esteem. In the present work, we examined Pakistan as uniquely influenced by honour culture and South Asian argumentation culture principles, and we contrasted it with Canada (a Western culture of dignity). Across two studies, Pakistanis had less positive self-esteem discrepancies (i.e., Pakistanis had minimal or no desire for higher self-esteem) compared to Canadians (who desired much higher self-esteem than they actually had). Pakistanis also believed less in the agentic benefits of high self-esteem but more in the communal benefits of high self-esteem than did Canadians. Differences in each cultures' beliefs about self-esteem's causal powers partially accounted for the differences in self-esteem discrepancies. These findings suggest unique conceptualizations of the value of self-esteem in distinct cultures.

人们追求积极自尊的跨文化普遍性经常受到争议。这一领域的大多数研究都对比了尊严文化(西方)和面子文化(东亚),但很少关注其他文化对自尊的看法。在目前的工作中,我们考察了巴基斯坦受荣誉文化和南亚辩论文化原则的独特影响,并将其与加拿大(西方的尊严文化)进行了对比。在两项研究中,与加拿大人相比,巴基斯坦人的积极自尊差异较小(即,巴基斯坦人对更高自尊的渴望很少或根本没有)(加拿大人对自尊的渴望比他们实际拥有的要高得多)。与加拿大人相比,巴基斯坦人更不相信高自尊带来的实实在在的好处,而更相信高自尊带来的集体好处。不同文化对自尊的因果能力的不同看法部分解释了自尊差异的差异。这些发现表明,在不同的文化中,自尊的价值有着独特的概念。
{"title":"Comparing self-esteem discrepancies in Pakistan and Canada","authors":"Thomas I. Vaughan-Johnston,&nbsp;Faizan Imtiaz,&nbsp;Li-Jun Ji,&nbsp;Rubina Hanif,&nbsp;Devin I. Fowlie,&nbsp;Jill A. Jacobson","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12592","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12592","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The cross-cultural universality of people's pursuit of positive self-esteem is frequently disputed. Most research in this area has contrasted cultures of dignity (Western) and face cultures (East Asian), but less attention has been given to other cultures' views of self-esteem. In the present work, we examined Pakistan as uniquely influenced by honour culture and South Asian argumentation culture principles, and we contrasted it with Canada (a Western culture of dignity). Across two studies, Pakistanis had less positive self-esteem discrepancies (i.e., Pakistanis had minimal or no desire for higher self-esteem) compared to Canadians (who desired much higher self-esteem than they actually had). Pakistanis also believed less in the agentic benefits of high self-esteem but more in the communal benefits of high self-esteem than did Canadians. Differences in each cultures' beliefs about self-esteem's causal powers partially accounted for the differences in self-esteem discrepancies. These findings suggest unique conceptualizations of the value of self-esteem in distinct cultures.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 2","pages":"231-247"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2023-11-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ajsp.12592","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138516216","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Under what conditions do gender differences exist in power and achievement values? The moderating role of gender ideology 权力和成就价值观在什么情况下存在性别差异?性别意识形态的调节作用
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2023-10-29 DOI: 10.1111/ajsp.12588
Gabriele Prati, Serena Stefani

Previous studies revealed differences between men and women in value priorities. It has been asserted that men do inherently attribute more importance than women to power and achievement values. Our study brings a sociocultural lens into account employing gender ideology as a moderator of the gender differences in these values. Specifically, we hypothesized that internalization and endorsement of gender role ideology shape gender differences in power and achievement values. We focused on two components of gender ideology as moderators of the gender differences in values: primacy of the breadwinner role, and acceptance of male privilege. We used data from the World Values Survey wave six (89,565 participants from 60 countries). Results indicated that gender ideology moderated the relationship between gender and the endorsement of achievement and power values. Specifically, the relationship between gender and achievement was significantly moderated by acceptance of male privilege, but not of primacy of the breadwinner role. In addition, the association between gender and power was significantly moderated by primacy of the breadwinner role, but not by acceptance of male privilege. In other words, when participants endorse egalitarian gender ideologies, gender differences in power and achievement disappear or even reverse. We contrast previous perspectives assuming inherent gender differences in human values and argue that such differences are related to beliefs in gendered separate spheres.

以往的研究表明,男性和女性在价值优先顺序方面存在差异。有人断言,男性在本质上比女性更重视权力和成就价值。我们的研究从社会文化的角度出发,将性别意识形态作为这些价值观性别差异的调节因素。具体来说,我们假设性别角色意识形态的内化和认可会形成权力和成就价值观的性别差异。我们重点研究了性别意识形态中作为价值观性别差异调节因素的两个组成部分:养家糊口者角色的首要地位和对男性特权的认可。我们使用了世界价值观调查第六波的数据(来自 60 个国家的 89,565 名参与者)。结果表明,性别意识形态调节了性别与成就价值观和权力价值观之间的关系。具体来说,性别与成就之间的关系在很大程度上受男性特权的认可而非养家糊口的首要地位的认可的调节。此外,性别与权力之间的关系在很大程度上受养家糊口的首要地位的影响,但不受对男性特权的接受程度的影响。换句话说,当参与者认可平等主义的性别意识形态时,在权力和成就方面的性别差异就会消失甚至逆转。我们对比了以往假定人类价值观存在固有性别差异的观点,认为这种差异与性别独立领域的信念有关。
{"title":"Under what conditions do gender differences exist in power and achievement values? The moderating role of gender ideology","authors":"Gabriele Prati,&nbsp;Serena Stefani","doi":"10.1111/ajsp.12588","DOIUrl":"10.1111/ajsp.12588","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Previous studies revealed differences between men and women in value priorities. It has been asserted that men do inherently attribute more importance than women to power and achievement values. Our study brings a sociocultural lens into account employing gender ideology as a moderator of the gender differences in these values. Specifically, we hypothesized that internalization and endorsement of gender role ideology shape gender differences in power and achievement values. We focused on two components of gender ideology as moderators of the gender differences in values: primacy of the breadwinner role, and acceptance of male privilege. We used data from the World Values Survey wave six (89,565 participants from 60 countries). Results indicated that gender ideology moderated the relationship between gender and the endorsement of achievement and power values. Specifically, the relationship between gender and achievement was significantly moderated by acceptance of male privilege, but not of primacy of the breadwinner role. In addition, the association between gender and power was significantly moderated by primacy of the breadwinner role, but not by acceptance of male privilege. In other words, when participants endorse egalitarian gender ideologies, gender differences in power and achievement disappear or even reverse. We contrast previous perspectives assuming inherent gender differences in human values and argue that such differences are related to beliefs in gendered separate spheres.</p>","PeriodicalId":47394,"journal":{"name":"Asian Journal of Social Psychology","volume":"27 2","pages":"176-186"},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2023-10-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ajsp.12588","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136157779","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Asian Journal of Social Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1