首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior最新文献

英文 中文
The Association of Embracing with Daily Mood and General Life Satisfaction: An Ecological Momentary Assessment Study. 拥抱与日常情绪和一般生活满意度的关系:一项生态瞬时评估研究。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2022-08-04 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-022-00411-8
Julian Packheiser, Imke Marlene Malek, Jacqueline Sophia Reichart, Laura Katona, Maike Luhmann, Sebastian Ocklenburg

Embracing has several positive health effects, such as lowering blood pressure and decreasing infection risk. However, its association with general life satisfaction and daily mood has not been researched in detail. Here, we used a smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment (EMA) approach to monitor the daily number of embraces and daily mood in a sample of 94 adults over the course of seven days. We found that embracing frequency differed slightly over the week, with embracing occurring more frequently on weekends than on weekdays. We also found that higher daily embracing frequencies were associated with better daily mood using multilevel modeling. Only singles benefitted from increases in average embracing regarding their life satisfaction, whereas individuals in a relationship were unaffected by their embracing tendencies. Although our results are strictly correlational and do not indicate any direction or causality, embraces may be important for daily mood and general life satisfaction, but their efficacy seems to depend on relationship status.

Supplementary information: The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10919-022-00411-8.

拥抱对健康有几个积极的影响,比如降低血压和减少感染风险。然而,它与一般生活满意度和日常情绪的关系尚未得到详细研究。在这里,我们使用一种基于智能手机的生态瞬间评估(EMA)方法来监测94名成年人在7天内每天拥抱的次数和每天的情绪。我们发现,拥抱的频率在一周内略有不同,周末拥抱的频率高于工作日。我们还发现,使用多层模型,较高的日常拥抱频率与更好的日常情绪有关。在生活满意度方面,只有单身人士从平均拥抱次数的增加中受益,而处于恋爱关系中的个人则不受拥抱倾向的影响。虽然我们的结果是严格相关的,并没有指明任何方向或因果关系,拥抱可能对日常情绪和一般生活满意度很重要,但它们的功效似乎取决于关系状态。补充信息:在线版本包含补充资料,下载地址为10.1007/s10919-022-00411-8。
{"title":"The Association of Embracing with Daily Mood and General Life Satisfaction: An Ecological Momentary Assessment Study.","authors":"Julian Packheiser,&nbsp;Imke Marlene Malek,&nbsp;Jacqueline Sophia Reichart,&nbsp;Laura Katona,&nbsp;Maike Luhmann,&nbsp;Sebastian Ocklenburg","doi":"10.1007/s10919-022-00411-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00411-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Embracing has several positive health effects, such as lowering blood pressure and decreasing infection risk. However, its association with general life satisfaction and daily mood has not been researched in detail. Here, we used a smartphone-based ecological momentary assessment (EMA) approach to monitor the daily number of embraces and daily mood in a sample of 94 adults over the course of seven days. We found that embracing frequency differed slightly over the week, with embracing occurring more frequently on weekends than on weekdays. We also found that higher daily embracing frequencies were associated with better daily mood using multilevel modeling. Only singles benefitted from increases in average embracing regarding their life satisfaction, whereas individuals in a relationship were unaffected by their embracing tendencies. Although our results are strictly correlational and do not indicate any direction or causality, embraces may be important for daily mood and general life satisfaction, but their efficacy seems to depend on relationship status.</p><p><strong>Supplementary information: </strong>The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10919-022-00411-8.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 4","pages":"519-536"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9362016/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40699407","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Discriminative and Affective Processing of Touch: Associations with Severity of Skin-picking. 触觉的辨别性和情感性加工:与抠皮严重程度的关系。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2022-08-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-022-00415-4
Anne Schienle, Albert Wabnegger

Skin-picking is a common behavior in the general population that generally serves emotion regulation (e.g., reduction of tension). However, recent research suggests it may also be associated with changes in tactile processing sensitivity. Along these lines, the present study examined whether the severity of skin-picking (SOSP) is related to discriminative and affective touch processing. A total of 160 participants (59 males, 101 females, mean age = 31 years) completed two tactile discrimination tests (two-point discrimination, surface texture discrimination), as well as a well-validated affective touch paradigm (delivery of soft/slow touch, which is found to be generally pleasant). A hierarchical regression analysis was carried out to investigate the association between SOSP, age, sex, and indicators of tactile sensitivity. Replicating previous findings, females reported higher SOSP. While the performance in the discrimination tests did not predict SOSP, affective touch processing was associated with SOSP. Participants with high SOSP reported an urge to pick their skin after being softly touched. This seems paradoxical since previous findings have suggested skin-picking may be carried out to manage negative affective states. Our findings add to the literature describing altered sensitivity and responsivity to specific tactile stimuli in individuals with excessive skin-picking.

在一般人群中,抠皮肤是一种常见的行为,通常用于情绪调节(例如,减轻紧张)。然而,最近的研究表明,它也可能与触觉处理敏感性的变化有关。沿着这些思路,本研究考察了抠皮的严重程度(SOSP)是否与辨别和情感触摸处理有关。共有160名参与者(59名男性,101名女性,平均年龄= 31岁)完成了两项触觉辨别测试(两点辨别,表面纹理辨别),以及一个经过验证的情感触摸范式(传递柔软/缓慢的触摸,这通常是令人愉快的)。采用层次回归分析探讨SOSP、年龄、性别和触觉敏感指标之间的关系。与之前的研究结果重复,女性报告了更高的SOSP。虽然辨别测试的表现不能预测SOSP,但情感触摸处理与SOSP有关。高SOSP的参与者报告说,在被轻轻地触摸后,他们有抠皮肤的冲动。这似乎是自相矛盾的,因为之前的研究结果表明,抠皮肤可能是为了控制消极的情感状态。我们的发现补充了文献中描述的过度抠皮肤的个体对特定触觉刺激的敏感性和反应性的改变。
{"title":"Discriminative and Affective Processing of Touch: Associations with Severity of Skin-picking.","authors":"Anne Schienle,&nbsp;Albert Wabnegger","doi":"10.1007/s10919-022-00415-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00415-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Skin-picking is a common behavior in the general population that generally serves emotion regulation (e.g., reduction of tension). However, recent research suggests it may also be associated with changes in tactile processing sensitivity. Along these lines, the present study examined whether the severity of skin-picking (SOSP) is related to discriminative and affective touch processing. A total of 160 participants (59 males, 101 females, mean age = 31 years) completed two tactile discrimination tests (two-point discrimination, surface texture discrimination), as well as a well-validated affective touch paradigm (delivery of soft/slow touch, which is found to be generally pleasant). A hierarchical regression analysis was carried out to investigate the association between SOSP, age, sex, and indicators of tactile sensitivity. Replicating previous findings, females reported higher SOSP. While the performance in the discrimination tests did not predict SOSP, affective touch processing was associated with SOSP. Participants with high SOSP reported an urge to pick their skin after being softly touched. This seems paradoxical since previous findings have suggested skin-picking may be carried out to manage negative affective states. Our findings add to the literature describing altered sensitivity and responsivity to specific tactile stimuli in individuals with excessive skin-picking.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 4","pages":"537-545"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9596513/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40671506","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Just Seconds of Laughter Reveals Relationship Status: Laughter with Friends Sounds More Authentic and Less Vulnerable than Laughter with Romantic Partners. 仅几秒钟的笑声就能揭示关系状况:与朋友的笑声相比,与恋人的笑声听起来更真实、更不脆弱。
IF 1.2 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-01 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-022-00406-5
Sally D Farley, Deborah Carson, Susan M Hughes

The dual pathway model posits that spontaneous and volitional laughter are voiced using distinct production systems, and perceivers rely upon these system-related cues to make accurate judgments about relationship status. Yet, to our knowledge, no empirical work has examined whether raters can differentiate laughter directed at friends and romantic partners and the cues driving this accuracy. In Study 1, raters (N = 50), who listened to 52 segments of laughter, identified conversational partner (friend versus romantic partner) with greater than chance accuracy (M = 0.57) and rated laughs directed at friends to be more pleasant-sounding than laughs directed at romantic partners. Study 2, which involved 58 raters, revealed that prototypical friendship laughter sounded more spontaneous (e.g., natural) and less "vulnerable" (e.g., submissive) than prototypical romantic laughter. Study 3 replicated the findings of the first two studies using a large cross-cultural sample (N = 252). Implications for the importance of laughter as a subtle relational signal of affiliation are discussed.

双通道模型认为,自发笑声和自愿笑声是通过不同的生产系统发出的,感知者依靠这些与系统相关的线索对关系状态做出准确判断。然而,据我们所知,还没有任何一项实证研究对评分者是否能区分针对朋友和浪漫伴侣的笑声以及驱动这种准确性的线索进行过研究。在研究 1 中,听了 52 个笑声片段的评分者(N = 50)识别对话伙伴(朋友还是恋爱伙伴)的准确率超过了机会准确率(M = 0.57),并认为针对朋友的笑声比针对恋爱伙伴的笑声听起来更悦耳。研究 2 有 58 位评分者参与,结果显示,与原型浪漫笑声相比,原型友谊笑声听起来更自发(如自然)、更不 "脆弱"(如顺从)。研究 3 利用大量跨文化样本(N = 252)重复了前两项研究的结果。研究讨论了笑声作为一种微妙的关系信号的重要性。
{"title":"Just Seconds of Laughter Reveals Relationship Status: Laughter with Friends Sounds More Authentic and Less Vulnerable than Laughter with Romantic Partners.","authors":"Sally D Farley, Deborah Carson, Susan M Hughes","doi":"10.1007/s10919-022-00406-5","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10919-022-00406-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The dual pathway model posits that spontaneous and volitional laughter are voiced using distinct production systems, and perceivers rely upon these system-related cues to make accurate judgments about relationship status. Yet, to our knowledge, no empirical work has examined whether raters can differentiate laughter directed at friends and romantic partners and the cues driving this accuracy. In Study 1, raters (<i>N</i> = 50), who listened to 52 segments of laughter, identified conversational partner (friend versus romantic partner) with greater than chance accuracy (<i>M</i> = 0.57) and rated laughs directed at friends to be more pleasant-sounding than laughs directed at romantic partners. Study 2, which involved 58 raters, revealed that prototypical friendship laughter sounded more spontaneous (e.g., natural) and less \"vulnerable\" (e.g., submissive) than prototypical romantic laughter. Study 3 replicated the findings of the first two studies using a large cross-cultural sample (<i>N</i> = 252). Implications for the importance of laughter as a subtle relational signal of affiliation are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 4","pages":"421-448"},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9247916/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40573772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Perceived Epistemic Authority (Source Credibility) of a TV Interviewer Moderates the Media Bias Effect Caused by His Nonverbal Behavior. 电视采访者的认知权威性(来源可信度)可调节其非语言行为造成的媒体偏见效应。
IF 1.2 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2022-01-28 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-022-00397-3
Refael Tikochinski, Elisha Babad

The Media Bias Effect (MBE) represents the biasing influence of the nonverbal behavior of a TV interviewer on viewers' impressions of the interviewee. In the MBE experiment, participants view a 4-min made-up political interview in which they are exposed only to the nonverbal behavior of the actors. The interviewer is friendly toward the politician in one experimental condition and hostile in the other. The interviewee was a confederate filmed in the same studio, and his clips are identical in the two conditions. This experiment was used successfully in a series of studies in several countries (Babad and Peer in J Nonverbal Behav 34(1):57-78, 2010. 10.1007/s10919-009-0078-x) and was administered in the present research. The present investigation focused on the interviewer's source credibility and its persuasive influence. The viewers filled out questionnaires about their impressions of both the interviewer and the interviewee. A component of "interviewer's authority" was derived in PCA, with substantial variance in viewers' impressions of the interviewer. In our design, we preferred the conception of Epistemic Authority (Kruglanski et al. in Adv Exp Soc Psychol 37:345-392, 2005)-based on viewers' subjective perceptions for deriving authority status-to the conventional design of source credibility studies, where dimensions of authority are pre-determined as independent variables. The results demonstrated that viewers who perceived the interviewer as an effective leader demonstrated a clear MBE and were susceptible to his influencing bias, but no bias effect was found for viewers who did not perceive the interviewer as an effective leader. Thus, Epistemic Authority (source credibility) moderated the Media Bias Effect.

媒体偏差效应(MBE)是指电视采访者的非语言行为对观众对被采访者印象的偏差影响。在 MBE 实验中,参与者观看 4 分钟的编造政治访谈,在访谈中他们只接触到演员的非语言行为。在一种实验条件下,采访者对政治家友好,而在另一种实验条件下,采访者对政治家充满敌意。被采访者是在同一摄影棚拍摄的一名助手,他的片段在两种实验条件下完全相同。该实验已在多个国家的一系列研究中成功应用(Babad 和 Peer,发表于《非言语行为》(J Nonverbal Behav)34(1):57-78,2010 年。10.1007/s10919-009-0078-x),并在本研究中使用。本次调查的重点是采访者的来源可信度及其说服力。观众填写了关于他们对采访者和被采访者印象的问卷。根据 PCA 得出的 "采访者权威 "成分,观众对采访者的印象存在很大差异。在我们的设计中,我们倾向于采用 "认识论权威 "的概念(Kruglanski 等人,Adv Exp Soc Psychol 37:345-392, 2005)--基于观众的主观感知来推导权威地位,而不是采用传统的来源可信度研究设计,即预先确定权威维度作为自变量。研究结果表明,认为采访者是有效领导者的观众表现出明显的 MBE,容易受到采访者影响偏差的影响,而不认为采访者是有效领导者的观众则没有发现偏差效应。因此,认识权威(消息来源可信度)对媒体偏见效应起着调节作用。
{"title":"Perceived Epistemic Authority (Source Credibility) of a TV Interviewer Moderates the Media Bias Effect Caused by His Nonverbal Behavior.","authors":"Refael Tikochinski, Elisha Babad","doi":"10.1007/s10919-022-00397-3","DOIUrl":"10.1007/s10919-022-00397-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Media Bias Effect (MBE) represents the biasing influence of the nonverbal behavior of a TV interviewer on viewers' impressions of the interviewee. In the MBE experiment, participants view a 4-min made-up political interview in which they are exposed only to the nonverbal behavior of the actors. The interviewer is friendly toward the politician in one experimental condition and hostile in the other. The interviewee was a confederate filmed in the same studio, and his clips are identical in the two conditions. This experiment was used successfully in a series of studies in several countries (Babad and Peer in J Nonverbal Behav 34(1):57-78, 2010. 10.1007/s10919-009-0078-x) and was administered in the present research. The present investigation focused on the interviewer's source credibility and its persuasive influence. The viewers filled out questionnaires about their impressions of both the interviewer and the interviewee. A component of \"interviewer's authority\" was derived in PCA, with substantial variance in viewers' impressions of the interviewer. In our design, we preferred the conception of Epistemic Authority (Kruglanski et al. in Adv Exp Soc Psychol 37:345-392, 2005)-based on viewers' subjective perceptions for deriving authority status-to the conventional design of source credibility studies, where dimensions of authority are pre-determined as independent variables. The results demonstrated that viewers who perceived the interviewer as an effective leader demonstrated a clear MBE and were susceptible to his influencing bias, but no bias effect was found for viewers who did not perceive the interviewer as an effective leader. Thus, Epistemic Authority (source credibility) moderated the Media Bias Effect.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 2","pages":"215-229"},"PeriodicalIF":1.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8795730/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39879567","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Identifying Signatures of Perceived Interpersonal Synchrony. 识别感知人际同步性特征。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2022-01-01 Epub Date: 2022-07-30 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-022-00410-9
Eric Novotny, Gary Bente

Interpersonal synchrony serves as a subtle, yet powerful bonding mechanism in social interactions. Problematically, the term 'synchrony' has been used to label a variety of distinct aspects of interpersonal coordination, such as postural similarities or movement activity entrainment. Accordingly, different algorithms have been suggested to quantify interpersonal synchrony. Yet, it remains unknown whether the different measures of synchrony represent correlated features of the same perceivable core phenomenon. The current study addresses this by comparing the suitability of a set of algorithms with respect to their association with observers' judgments of dyadic synchrony and leader-followership. One-hundred fifteen observers viewed computer animations of characters portraying the movements of real dyads who performed a repetitive motor task with instruction to move in unison. Animations were based on full-body motion capture data synchronously collected for both partners during the joint exercise. Results showed most synchrony measures significantly correlated with (a) perceived synchrony and (b) the perceived level of balance of leading/following by each dyad member. Phase synchrony and Pearson correlations were associated most strongly with the observer ratings. This might be typical for intentional, structured forms synchrony such as ritualized group activities. It remains open if these findings also apply to spontaneous forms of synchrony as, for instance, occurring in free-running conversations.

在社会互动中,人际同步是一种微妙而强大的联系机制。有问题的是,“同步性”一词已被用于标记人际协调的各种不同方面,如姿势相似性或运动活动夹带。因此,人们提出了不同的算法来量化人际同步。然而,不同的同步性测量是否代表相同的可感知核心现象的相关特征仍然未知。当前的研究通过比较一组算法的适用性来解决这个问题,这些算法与观察者对二元同步性和领导-追随者关系的判断有关。一百一十五名观察者观看了电脑动画人物的动作,这些人物描绘了真实的二人组的动作,这些人在指令下完成了一项重复的运动任务。动画是基于在联合练习中同步收集的双方全身动作捕捉数据。结果表明,大多数同步性测量与(a)感知同步性和(b)每个双人组成员感知领导/跟随平衡水平显著相关。相位同步性和Pearson相关性与观察者评分的关系最为密切。这可能是典型的有意的、结构化的表单同步,比如仪式化的组活动。如果这些发现也适用于自发形式的同步,例如,发生在自由运行的对话中,它仍然是开放的。
{"title":"Identifying Signatures of Perceived Interpersonal Synchrony.","authors":"Eric Novotny,&nbsp;Gary Bente","doi":"10.1007/s10919-022-00410-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-022-00410-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Interpersonal synchrony serves as a subtle, yet powerful bonding mechanism in social interactions. Problematically, the term 'synchrony' has been used to label a variety of distinct aspects of interpersonal coordination, such as postural similarities or movement activity entrainment. Accordingly, different algorithms have been suggested to quantify interpersonal synchrony. Yet, it remains unknown whether the different measures of synchrony represent correlated features of the same perceivable core phenomenon. The current study addresses this by comparing the suitability of a set of algorithms with respect to their association with observers' judgments of dyadic synchrony and leader-followership. One-hundred fifteen observers viewed computer animations of characters portraying the movements of real dyads who performed a repetitive motor task with instruction to move in unison. Animations were based on full-body motion capture data synchronously collected for both partners during the joint exercise. Results showed most synchrony measures significantly correlated with (a) perceived synchrony and (b) the perceived level of balance of leading/following by each dyad member. Phase synchrony and Pearson correlations were associated most strongly with the observer ratings. This might be typical for intentional, structured forms synchrony such as ritualized group activities. It remains open if these findings also apply to spontaneous forms of synchrony as, for instance, occurring in free-running conversations.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 4","pages":"485-517"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9361934/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40699405","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Ad-Hoc Reviewers from 2021 2021年的特设评审员
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-11-03 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-021-00391-1
Caspar Addyman, Cameron P. Anderson, Pablo Arias, Toe Aung, Stephen Benning, R. Bjornsdottir, M. Bowdring, Daniel Bowling, Mitchell Brown, Morgan M. Bruck, Britta Brugman, J. Burgoon, Qing Cai, Stephanie M Carpenter, Daniel Conroy-Beam, William Cox, Belinda M. Craig, I. Croy, Hélio Clemente José Cuve, Tove I. Dahl, A. Dawel, Vincent Denault, J. Driskell, Harry Farmer, D. Fitton, Kory Floyd, J. Fox, R. Franklin, Justin Friesen, Jennifer Fugate, P. Furley, J. Girard, A. Gomila, Sam Gosling, Melissa J. Grey, S. Gunnery, D. Gurney, J. Guyer, Tua Hakanpaa, Nicole Hedgecoth, N. Heffer, Erik G. Helzer, M. Jackson, B. Jakubiak, Xiaoming Jiang, Matthieu Jost, Roza G. Komiğlu, Nobuhiro Kamiya, Mary H. Kayyal, Carrie Keating, Iring Koch, S. Korb, H. Kreysa, A. Kruglanski, J. Lakin, K. Lander, Sandra J. E. Langeslag, Aaron W. Lukaszewski, K. Manokara, V. Manusov, L. Marsh, Jared D. Martin, A. Mastergeorge, D. McFarland, Daniel McIntosh, G. McKeown, M. Mehu, L. Miller, Marta Miragall, Anshuman Mishra, Laura M. Morett,
{"title":"Ad-Hoc Reviewers from 2021","authors":"Caspar Addyman, Cameron P. Anderson, Pablo Arias, Toe Aung, Stephen Benning, R. Bjornsdottir, M. Bowdring, Daniel Bowling, Mitchell Brown, Morgan M. Bruck, Britta Brugman, J. Burgoon, Qing Cai, Stephanie M Carpenter, Daniel Conroy-Beam, William Cox, Belinda M. Craig, I. Croy, Hélio Clemente José Cuve, Tove I. Dahl, A. Dawel, Vincent Denault, J. Driskell, Harry Farmer, D. Fitton, Kory Floyd, J. Fox, R. Franklin, Justin Friesen, Jennifer Fugate, P. Furley, J. Girard, A. Gomila, Sam Gosling, Melissa J. Grey, S. Gunnery, D. Gurney, J. Guyer, Tua Hakanpaa, Nicole Hedgecoth, N. Heffer, Erik G. Helzer, M. Jackson, B. Jakubiak, Xiaoming Jiang, Matthieu Jost, Roza G. Komiğlu, Nobuhiro Kamiya, Mary H. Kayyal, Carrie Keating, Iring Koch, S. Korb, H. Kreysa, A. Kruglanski, J. Lakin, K. Lander, Sandra J. E. Langeslag, Aaron W. Lukaszewski, K. Manokara, V. Manusov, L. Marsh, Jared D. Martin, A. Mastergeorge, D. McFarland, Daniel McIntosh, G. McKeown, M. Mehu, L. Miller, Marta Miragall, Anshuman Mishra, Laura M. Morett,","doi":"10.1007/s10919-021-00391-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-021-00391-1","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 1","pages":"115 - 117"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2021-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41787913","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How Responsive are Anesthesiologists to Patient Pain? Residents’ Verbal and Nonverbal Responses to Standardized Patient Pain Cues 麻醉师对病人疼痛的反应如何?住院医师对标准化病人疼痛提示的言语和非言语反应
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-11-02 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-021-00390-2
Mollie A. Ruben, D. Blanch-Hartigan, Jill Laquidara, E. Meyer, Judith A. Hall, D. Waisel, Richard H Blum
{"title":"How Responsive are Anesthesiologists to Patient Pain? Residents’ Verbal and Nonverbal Responses to Standardized Patient Pain Cues","authors":"Mollie A. Ruben, D. Blanch-Hartigan, Jill Laquidara, E. Meyer, Judith A. Hall, D. Waisel, Richard H Blum","doi":"10.1007/s10919-021-00390-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-021-00390-2","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 1","pages":"137 - 154"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2021-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44500122","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Nonverbal Auditory Cues Allow Relationship Quality to be Inferred During Conversations 非语言的听觉线索可以在对话中推断出关系的质量
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-10-22 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-021-00386-y
R. Dunbar, J. Robledo, Ignacio Tamarit, I. Cross, Emma Smith
{"title":"Nonverbal Auditory Cues Allow Relationship Quality to be Inferred During Conversations","authors":"R. Dunbar, J. Robledo, Ignacio Tamarit, I. Cross, Emma Smith","doi":"10.1007/s10919-021-00386-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-021-00386-y","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"46 1","pages":"1 - 18"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2021-10-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44550170","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Beauty Goes Down to the Core: Attractiveness Biases Moral Character Attributions 美到核心:吸引力偏见道德品质归因
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-10-20 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-021-00388-w
Klebl, Christoph, Rhee, Joshua J., Greenaway, Katharine H., Luo, Yin, Bastian, Brock

Physical attractiveness is a heuristic that is often used as an indicator of desirable traits. In two studies (N = 1254), we tested whether facial attractiveness leads to a selective bias in attributing moral character—which is paramount in person perception—over non-moral traits. We argue that because people are motivated to assess socially important traits quickly, these may be the traits that are most strongly biased by physical attractiveness. In Study 1, we found that people attributed more moral traits to attractive than unattractive people, an effect that was stronger than the tendency to attribute positive non-moral traits to attractive (vs. unattractive) people. In Study 2, we conceptually replicated the findings while matching traits on perceived warmth. The findings suggest that the Beauty-is-Good stereotype particularly skews in favor of the attribution of moral traits. As such, physical attractiveness biases the perceptions of others even more fundamentally than previously understood.

外表吸引力是一种启发式,经常被用作理想特征的指标。在两项研究中(N = 1254),我们测试了面部吸引力是否会导致人们对道德品质的选择性偏见——道德品质在个人感知中是最重要的——而不是非道德品质。我们认为,因为人们有动力快速评估重要的社会特征,这些特征可能是身体吸引力最强烈的偏见。在研究1中,我们发现人们认为漂亮的人比不漂亮的人有更多的道德品质,这种影响比把积极的非道德品质归因于漂亮的人(相对于不漂亮的人)的倾向更强烈。在研究2中,我们在概念上重复了研究结果,同时匹配了感知温暖的特征。研究结果表明,“漂亮就是好”的刻板印象尤其倾向于道德品质的归因。因此,外表吸引力对他人看法的影响甚至比之前所理解的更为根本。
{"title":"Beauty Goes Down to the Core: Attractiveness Biases Moral Character Attributions","authors":"Klebl, Christoph, Rhee, Joshua J., Greenaway, Katharine H., Luo, Yin, Bastian, Brock","doi":"10.1007/s10919-021-00388-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-021-00388-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Physical attractiveness is a heuristic that is often used as an indicator of desirable traits. In two studies (<i>N</i> = 1254), we tested whether facial attractiveness leads to a selective bias in attributing moral character—which is paramount in person perception—over non-moral traits. We argue that because people are motivated to assess socially important traits quickly, these may be the traits that are most strongly biased by physical attractiveness. In Study 1, we found that people attributed more moral traits to attractive than unattractive people, an effect that was stronger than the tendency to attribute positive non-moral traits to attractive (vs. unattractive) people. In Study 2, we conceptually replicated the findings while matching traits on perceived warmth. The findings suggest that the Beauty-is-Good stereotype particularly skews in favor of the attribution of moral traits. As such, physical attractiveness biases the perceptions of others even more fundamentally than previously understood.</p>","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"14 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2021-10-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138527397","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Correction to: Emotion Expression in Context: Full Body Postures of Christian Prayer Orientations Compared to Specific Emotions 更正:语境中的情绪表达:基督教祷告取向的全身姿势与特定情绪的比较
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, SOCIAL Pub Date : 2021-10-15 DOI: 10.1007/s10919-021-00389-9
Patty Van Cappellen, Megan Edwards
{"title":"Correction to: Emotion Expression in Context: Full Body Postures of Christian Prayer Orientations Compared to Specific Emotions","authors":"Patty Van Cappellen, Megan Edwards","doi":"10.1007/s10919-021-00389-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-021-00389-9","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47747,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Nonverbal Behavior","volume":"45 1","pages":"633"},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2021-10-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47792965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1