首页 > 最新文献

Music Perception最新文献

英文 中文
A Critical Cross-cultural Study of Sensorimotor and Groove Responses to Syncopation Among Ghanaian and American University Students and Staff 加纳和美国大学生和员工对切分音的感觉运动和沟槽反应的跨文化研究
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-03-11 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.278
Maria A. G. Witek, Jingyi Liu, John Kuubertzie, A. Yankyera, Senyo Adzei, P. Vuust
The pleasurable desire to move to a beat is known as groove and is partly explained by rhythmic syncopation. While many contemporary groove-directed genres originated in the African diaspora, groove music psychology has almost exclusively studied European or North American listeners. While cross-cultural approaches can help us understand how different populations respond to music, comparing African and Western musical behaviors has historically tended to rely on stereotypes. Here we report on two studies in which sensorimotor and groove responses to syncopation were measured in university students and staff from Cape Coast, Ghana and Williamstown, MA, United States. In our experimental designs and interpretations, we show sensitivity towards the ethical implications of doing cross-cultural research in an African context. The Ghanaian group showed greater synchronization precision than Americans during monophonic syncopated patterns, but this was not reflected in synchronization accuracy. There was no significant group difference in the pleasurable desire to move. Our results have implications for how we understand the relationship between exposure and synchronization, and how we define syncopation in cultural and musical contexts. We hope our critical approach to cross-cultural comparison contributes to developing music psychology into a more inclusive and culturally grounded field.
随着节拍移动的愉悦欲望被称为凹槽,部分原因是有节奏的切分音。虽然许多当代凹槽音乐流派起源于非洲侨民,但凹槽音乐心理学几乎只研究欧洲或北美听众。虽然跨文化方法可以帮助我们了解不同人群对音乐的反应,但比较非洲和西方的音乐行为在历史上往往依赖于刻板印象。在这里,我们报道了两项研究,在这两项研究中,来自加纳海岸角和美国马萨诸塞州威廉斯敦的大学生和教职员工对切分音的感觉运动和凹槽反应进行了测量。在我们的实验设计和解释中,我们对在非洲背景下进行跨文化研究的伦理含义表现出敏感性。在单音切分模式中,加纳组比美国人表现出更高的同步精度,但这并没有反映在同步精度上。在愉快的运动欲望方面,没有显著的群体差异。我们的研究结果对我们如何理解暴露和同步之间的关系,以及我们如何在文化和音乐背景下定义切分音具有启示意义。我们希望我们对跨文化比较的批判性方法有助于将音乐心理学发展成为一个更具包容性和文化基础的领域。
{"title":"A Critical Cross-cultural Study of Sensorimotor and Groove Responses to Syncopation Among Ghanaian and American University Students and Staff","authors":"Maria A. G. Witek, Jingyi Liu, John Kuubertzie, A. Yankyera, Senyo Adzei, P. Vuust","doi":"10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.278","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.278","url":null,"abstract":"The pleasurable desire to move to a beat is known as groove and is partly explained by rhythmic syncopation. While many contemporary groove-directed genres originated in the African diaspora, groove music psychology has almost exclusively studied European or North American listeners. While cross-cultural approaches can help us understand how different populations respond to music, comparing African and Western musical behaviors has historically tended to rely on stereotypes. Here we report on two studies in which sensorimotor and groove responses to syncopation were measured in university students and staff from Cape Coast, Ghana and Williamstown, MA, United States. In our experimental designs and interpretations, we show sensitivity towards the ethical implications of doing cross-cultural research in an African context. The Ghanaian group showed greater synchronization precision than Americans during monophonic syncopated patterns, but this was not reflected in synchronization accuracy. There was no significant group difference in the pleasurable desire to move. Our results have implications for how we understand the relationship between exposure and synchronization, and how we define syncopation in cultural and musical contexts. We hope our critical approach to cross-cultural comparison contributes to developing music psychology into a more inclusive and culturally grounded field.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2020-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47913631","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Perceived Tension, Movement, and Pleasantness in Harmonic Musical Intervals and Noises 在和声音程和噪音中感受到的张力、运动和愉悦
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-03-11 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.298
M. Costa, M. Nese
Perceived valence, tension, and movement of harmonic musical intervals (from the unison to the octave presented in a low- and high-register) and standard noises (brown, pink, white, blue, purple) were assessed in two studies that differed in the crossmodal procedure by which tension and movement were rated: proprioceptive device or visual analog scale. Valence was evaluated in both studies with the visual analog scale. In a preliminary study, the proprioceptive device was calibrated with a psychophysical procedure. Roughness of the stimuli was included as covariate. Tension was perceived higher in dissonant intervals and in intervals presented in the high register. The higher the high-pitch energy content in the standard noise, the higher the perceived tension. The visual analog scale resulted in higher tension ratings than the proprioceptive device. Perception of movement was higher in dissonant intervals, in intervals in the high register, and in standard noises than in musical intervals. High-pitch spectrum noises were associated with more sense of movement than low-pitch spectrum noises. Consonant intervals and low-register intervals were evaluated as more pleasant than dissonant and high-register intervals. High-pitch spectrum purple and blue noises were evaluated as more unpleasant than low-pitch spectrum noises.
在两项研究中评估了和声音程(从和声到以低音和高音呈现的八度音阶)和标准噪音(棕色、粉色、白色、蓝色、紫色)的感知效价、张力和运动,这两项研究在张力和运动的跨模态评定程序上有所不同:本体感觉装置或视觉模拟量表。两项研究均采用视觉模拟量表对价进行了评估。在一项初步研究中,本体感觉装置是用心理物理学程序校准的。刺激的粗糙度被纳入协变量。不和谐音程和高音音程的紧张感更高。标准噪声中的高音能量含量越高,感知到的张力就越高。视觉模拟量表的张力等级高于本体感觉装置。在不和谐音程、高音音程和标准噪音中,对运动的感知高于音乐音程。高音谱噪声比低音谱噪声与更多的运动感有关。辅音音程和低音程被评价为比不和谐音程和高音程更令人愉快。高音谱紫色和蓝色噪声被评估为比低音谱噪声更令人不快。
{"title":"Perceived Tension, Movement, and Pleasantness in Harmonic Musical Intervals and Noises","authors":"M. Costa, M. Nese","doi":"10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.298","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.298","url":null,"abstract":"Perceived valence, tension, and movement of harmonic musical intervals (from the unison to the octave presented in a low- and high-register) and standard noises (brown, pink, white, blue, purple) were assessed in two studies that differed in the crossmodal procedure by which tension and movement were rated: proprioceptive device or visual analog scale. Valence was evaluated in both studies with the visual analog scale. In a preliminary study, the proprioceptive device was calibrated with a psychophysical procedure. Roughness of the stimuli was included as covariate. Tension was perceived higher in dissonant intervals and in intervals presented in the high register. The higher the high-pitch energy content in the standard noise, the higher the perceived tension. The visual analog scale resulted in higher tension ratings than the proprioceptive device. Perception of movement was higher in dissonant intervals, in intervals in the high register, and in standard noises than in musical intervals. High-pitch spectrum noises were associated with more sense of movement than low-pitch spectrum noises. Consonant intervals and low-register intervals were evaluated as more pleasant than dissonant and high-register intervals. High-pitch spectrum purple and blue noises were evaluated as more unpleasant than low-pitch spectrum noises.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2020-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43945519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
The Territory Between Speech and Song 语与歌之间的疆域
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-03-11 DOI: 10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.347
Fred Cummins
Speech and song have frequently been treated as contrasting categories. We here observe a variety of collective activities in which multiple participants utter the same thing at the same time, a behavior we call joint speech. This simple empirical definition serves to single out practices of ritual, protest, and the enactment of identity that span the range from speech to song and allows consideration of the manner in which such activities serve to ground collectives. We consider how the musical elements in joint speech such as rhythm, melody, and instrumentation are related to the context of occurrence and the purposes of the participants. While music and language have been greatly altered by developments in media technologies—from writing to recordings—joint speech has been, and continues to be, an integral part of practices, both formal and informal, from which communities derive their identity. The absence of joint speech from the scientific treatment of language has made language appear as an abstract intellectual and highly individualized activity. Joint speech may act as a corrective to draw our attention back to the voice in context, and the manner in which collective identities are enacted.
言语和歌曲经常被视为对立的范畴。我们在这里观察到各种各样的集体活动,在这些活动中,多个参与者同时说出同样的话,这种行为我们称之为联合讲话。这个简单的经验定义有助于挑选出从演讲到歌曲的范围内的仪式,抗议和身份制定的实践,并允许考虑这些活动为基础集体服务的方式。我们考虑联合演讲中的音乐元素,如节奏、旋律和乐器如何与发生的背景和参与者的目的相关。虽然音乐和语言已经被媒体技术的发展极大地改变了——从写作到录音——但联合演讲一直是,并将继续是正式和非正式实践的一个组成部分,社区从中获得他们的身份。科学对待语言缺乏联合言语,使得语言表现为一种抽象的智力活动和高度个性化的活动。联合言语可以作为一种纠正,将我们的注意力拉回到语境中的声音,以及集体身份的制定方式。
{"title":"The Territory Between Speech and Song","authors":"Fred Cummins","doi":"10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.347","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.347","url":null,"abstract":"Speech and song have frequently been treated as contrasting categories. We here observe a variety of collective activities in which multiple participants utter the same thing at the same time, a behavior we call joint speech. This simple empirical definition serves to single out practices of ritual, protest, and the enactment of identity that span the range from speech to song and allows consideration of the manner in which such activities serve to ground collectives. We consider how the musical elements in joint speech such as rhythm, melody, and instrumentation are related to the context of occurrence and the purposes of the participants. While music and language have been greatly altered by developments in media technologies—from writing to recordings—joint speech has been, and continues to be, an integral part of practices, both formal and informal, from which communities derive their identity. The absence of joint speech from the scientific treatment of language has made language appear as an abstract intellectual and highly individualized activity. Joint speech may act as a corrective to draw our attention back to the voice in context, and the manner in which collective identities are enacted.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2020-03-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1525/MP.2020.37.4.347","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43559000","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
A Computational Cognitive Model for the Analysis and Generation of Voice Leadings 语音引导性分析与生成的计算认知模型
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-02-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.208
Peter M. C. Harrison,Marcus T. Pearce
Voice leading is a common task in Western music composition whose conventions are consistent with fundamental principles of auditory perception. Here we introduce a computational cognitive model of voice leading, intended both for analyzing voice-leading practices within encoded musical corpora and for generating new voice leadings for unseen chord sequences. This model is feature-based, quantifying the desirability of a given voice leading on the basis of different features derived from Huron’s (2001) perceptual account of voice leading. We use the model to analyze a corpus of 370 chorale harmonizations by J. S. Bach, and demonstrate the model’s application to the voicing of harmonic progressions in different musical genres. The model is implemented in a new R package, “voicer,” which we release alongside this paper.
导声是西方音乐创作中常见的一项任务,其惯例与听觉感知的基本原则是一致的。在这里,我们介绍了一个语音引导的计算认知模型,旨在分析编码音乐语料库中的语音引导实践,并为未见过的和弦序列生成新的语音引导。该模型是基于特征的,根据Huron(2001)对语音引导的感知描述得出的不同特征,量化给定语音引导的可取性。我们使用该模型分析了巴赫的370首赞美诗和声语料库,并展示了该模型在不同音乐流派的和声进行发声中的应用。该模型是在一个新的R包“voicer”中实现的,我们将与本文一起发布。
{"title":"A Computational Cognitive Model for the Analysis and Generation of Voice Leadings","authors":"Peter M. C. Harrison,Marcus T. Pearce","doi":"10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.208","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.208","url":null,"abstract":"Voice leading is a common task in Western music composition whose conventions are consistent with fundamental principles of auditory perception. Here we introduce a computational cognitive model of voice leading, intended both for analyzing voice-leading practices within encoded musical corpora and for generating new voice leadings for unseen chord sequences. This model is feature-based, quantifying the desirability of a given voice leading on the basis of different features derived from Huron’s (2001) perceptual account of voice leading. We use the model to analyze a corpus of 370 chorale harmonizations by J. S. Bach, and demonstrate the model’s application to the voicing of harmonic progressions in different musical genres. The model is implemented in a new R package, “voicer,” which we release alongside this paper.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2020-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138495026","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Choosing the Right Tune 选择正确的调子
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-02-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.240
L. Warrenburg
When designing a new study regarding how music can portray and elicit emotion, one of the most crucial design decisions involves choosing the best stimuli. Every researcher must find musical samples that are able to capture an emotional state, are appropriate lengths, and have minimal potential for biasing participants. Researchers have often utilized musical excerpts that have previously been used by other scholars, but the appropriate musical choices depend on the specific goals of the study in question and will likely change among various research designs. The intention of this paper is to examine how musical stimuli have been selected in a sample of 306 research articles dating from 1928 through 2018. Analyses are presented regarding the designated emotions, how the stimuli were selected, the durations of the stimuli, whether the stimuli are excerpts from a longer work, and whether the passages have been used in studies about perceived or induced emotion. The results suggest that the literature relies on nine emotional terms, focuses more on perceived emotion than on induced emotion, and contains mostly short musical stimuli. I suggest that some of the inconclusive results from previous reviews may be due to the inconsistent use of emotion terms throughout the music community.
在设计一项关于音乐如何描绘和引发情感的新研究时,最关键的设计决策之一涉及选择最佳刺激。每个研究人员都必须找到能够捕捉情绪状态的音乐样本,适当的长度,并尽可能减少对参与者的偏见。研究人员经常使用其他学者以前使用过的音乐节选,但适当的音乐选择取决于所讨论的研究的具体目标,并且可能会因不同的研究设计而改变。本文的目的是研究从1928年到2018年306篇研究文章的样本中音乐刺激是如何被选择的。分析了指定的情绪,如何选择刺激,刺激的持续时间,刺激是否摘录自较长的作品,以及这些段落是否被用于研究感知或诱导的情绪。结果表明,文献依赖于九个情绪术语,更多地关注感知情绪而不是诱导情绪,并且主要包含简短的音乐刺激。我认为,之前的评论中一些不确定的结果可能是由于整个音乐界对情感术语的使用不一致。
{"title":"Choosing the Right Tune","authors":"L. Warrenburg","doi":"10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.240","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.240","url":null,"abstract":"When designing a new study regarding how music can portray and elicit emotion, one of the most crucial design decisions involves choosing the best stimuli. Every researcher must find musical samples that are able to capture an emotional state, are appropriate lengths, and have minimal potential for biasing participants. Researchers have often utilized musical excerpts that have previously been used by other scholars, but the appropriate musical choices depend on the specific goals of the study in question and will likely change among various research designs. The intention of this paper is to examine how musical stimuli have been selected in a sample of 306 research articles dating from 1928 through 2018. Analyses are presented regarding the designated emotions, how the stimuli were selected, the durations of the stimuli, whether the stimuli are excerpts from a longer work, and whether the passages have been used in studies about perceived or induced emotion. The results suggest that the literature relies on nine emotional terms, focuses more on perceived emotion than on induced emotion, and contains mostly short musical stimuli. I suggest that some of the inconclusive results from previous reviews may be due to the inconsistent use of emotion terms throughout the music community.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2020-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.240","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44363278","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 14
Cross-Cultural Work in Music Cognition: Challenges, Insights, and Recommendations. 音乐认知的跨文化工作:挑战、见解和建议。
IF 1.3 2区 心理学 0 MUSIC Pub Date : 2020-02-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.185
Nori Jacoby, Elizabeth Hellmuth Margulis, Martin Clayton, Erin Hannon, Henkjan Honing, John Iversen, Tobias Robert Klein, Samuel A Mehr, Lara Pearson, Isabelle Peretz, Marc Perlman, Rainer Polak, Andrea Ravignani, Patrick E Savage, Gavin Steingo, Catherine J Stevens, Laurel Trainor, Sandra Trehub, Michael Veal, Melanie Wald-Fuhrmann

Many foundational questions in the psychology of music require cross-cultural approaches, yet the vast majority of work in the field to date has been conducted with Western participants and Western music. For cross-cultural research to thrive, it will require collaboration between people from different disciplinary backgrounds, as well as strategies for overcoming differences in assumptions, methods, and terminology. This position paper surveys the current state of the field and offers a number of concrete recommendations focused on issues involving ethics, empirical methods, and definitions of "music" and "culture."

音乐心理学中的许多基础性问题都需要跨文化的研究方法,但迄今为止,该领域的绝大多数研究工作都是以西方参与者和西方音乐为对象开展的。跨文化研究要想蓬勃发展,就需要不同学科背景的人员通力合作,并制定克服假设、方法和术语差异的策略。本立场文件概述了该领域的现状,并针对涉及伦理、实证方法以及 "音乐 "和 "文化 "的定义等问题提出了一些具体建议。
{"title":"Cross-Cultural Work in Music Cognition: Challenges, Insights, and Recommendations.","authors":"Nori Jacoby, Elizabeth Hellmuth Margulis, Martin Clayton, Erin Hannon, Henkjan Honing, John Iversen, Tobias Robert Klein, Samuel A Mehr, Lara Pearson, Isabelle Peretz, Marc Perlman, Rainer Polak, Andrea Ravignani, Patrick E Savage, Gavin Steingo, Catherine J Stevens, Laurel Trainor, Sandra Trehub, Michael Veal, Melanie Wald-Fuhrmann","doi":"10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.185","DOIUrl":"10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.185","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b>Many foundational questions in the</b> psychology of music require cross-cultural approaches, yet the vast majority of work in the field to date has been conducted with Western participants and Western music. For cross-cultural research to thrive, it will require collaboration between people from different disciplinary backgrounds, as well as strategies for overcoming differences in assumptions, methods, and terminology. This position paper surveys the current state of the field and offers a number of concrete recommendations focused on issues involving ethics, empirical methods, and definitions of \"music\" and \"culture.\"</p>","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.3,"publicationDate":"2020-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10019032/pdf/nihms-1711599.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9152427","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Development of Sensitivity to Tonality Structure of Music 音乐音调结构敏感性的发展
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2020-02-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.225
R. Matsunaga, P. Hartono, K. Yokosawa, J. Abe
Tonal schemata are shaped by culture-specific music exposure. The acquisition process of tonal schemata has been delineated in Western mono-musical children, but cross-cultural variations have not been explored. We examined how Japanese children acquire tonal schemata in a bi-musical culture characterized by the simultaneous, and unbalanced, appearances of Western (dominant) music along with traditional Japanese (non-dominant) music. Progress of this acquisition was indexed by gauging children’s sensitivities to musical scale membership (differentiating scale-tones from non-scale-tones) and differences in tonal stability among scale tones (differentiating the tonic from another scale tone). Children (7-, 9-, 11-, 13-, and 14-year-olds) and adults judged how well two types of target tones (scale tone vs. non-scale tone; tonic vs. non-tonic) fit a preceding Western or traditional Japanese tonal context. Results showed that even 7-year-olds showed sensitivity to Western scale membership while sensitivity to Japanese scale membership did not appear until age nine. Also, sensitivity to the tonic emerged at age 13 for both types of melodies. These results suggest that even though they are exposed to both types of music simultaneously from birth, Japanese children begin by acquiring the tonal schema of the dominant Western music and this age of acquisition is not delayed relative to Western mono-musical peers.
音调图式是由特定文化的音乐暴露形成的。西方单一音乐儿童的调性图式习得过程已被描述,但尚未探讨其跨文化差异。我们研究了日本儿童是如何在双重音乐文化中习得调性图式的,这种文化的特点是西方(主导)音乐与传统日本(非主导)音乐同时出现,而且不平衡。通过测量儿童对音乐音阶成员的敏感性(区分音阶音调和非音阶音调)和音阶音调之间的音调稳定性差异(区分主音和另一个音阶音调)来索引这种习得的进展。儿童(7岁、9岁、11岁、13岁和14岁)和成年人对两种目标音调(有音阶的音调与无音阶的音调;主音与非主音)符合之前的西方或传统日本的调性背景。结果表明,即使是7岁的儿童也表现出对西方量表成员的敏感性,而对日本量表成员的敏感性直到9岁才出现。此外,在13岁时,两种类型的旋律对主音的敏感性都出现了。这些结果表明,尽管日本儿童从出生起就同时接触两种类型的音乐,但他们一开始就掌握了占主导地位的西方音乐的调性图式,而且与西方单一音乐的同龄人相比,这一习得年龄并不延迟。
{"title":"The Development of Sensitivity to Tonality Structure of Music","authors":"R. Matsunaga, P. Hartono, K. Yokosawa, J. Abe","doi":"10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.225","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.225","url":null,"abstract":"Tonal schemata are shaped by culture-specific music exposure. The acquisition process of tonal schemata has been delineated in Western mono-musical children, but cross-cultural variations have not been explored. We examined how Japanese children acquire tonal schemata in a bi-musical culture characterized by the simultaneous, and unbalanced, appearances of Western (dominant) music along with traditional Japanese (non-dominant) music. Progress of this acquisition was indexed by gauging children’s sensitivities to musical scale membership (differentiating scale-tones from non-scale-tones) and differences in tonal stability among scale tones (differentiating the tonic from another scale tone). Children (7-, 9-, 11-, 13-, and 14-year-olds) and adults judged how well two types of target tones (scale tone vs. non-scale tone; tonic vs. non-tonic) fit a preceding Western or traditional Japanese tonal context. Results showed that even 7-year-olds showed sensitivity to Western scale membership while sensitivity to Japanese scale membership did not appear until age nine. Also, sensitivity to the tonic emerged at age 13 for both types of melodies. These results suggest that even though they are exposed to both types of music simultaneously from birth, Japanese children begin by acquiring the tonal schema of the dominant Western music and this age of acquisition is not delayed relative to Western mono-musical peers.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2020-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1525/mp.2020.37.3.225","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43772182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The Effects of Timbre on Neural Responses to Musical Emotion 音色对音乐情绪神经反应的影响
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-12-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.134
Weixia Zhang, Fang Liu, Linshu Zhou, Wan-Chen Wang, Hanyuan Jiang, Cunmei Jiang
Timbre is an important factor that affects the perception of emotion in music. To date, little is known about the effects of timbre on neural responses to musical emotion. To address this issue, we used ERPs to investigate whether there are different neural responses to musical emotion when the same melodies are presented in different timbres. With a cross-modal affective priming paradigm, target faces were primed by affectively congruent or incongruent melodies without lyrics presented in the violin, flute, and voice. Results showed a larger P3 and a larger left anterior distributed LPC in response to affectively incongruent versus congruent trials in the voice version. For the flute version, however, only the LPC effect was found, which was distributed over centro-parietal electrodes. Unlike the voice and flute versions, an N400 effect was observed in the violin version. These findings revealed different patterns of neural responses to musical emotion when the same melodies were presented in different timbres, and provide evidence for the hypothesis that there are specialized neural responses to the human voice.
音色是影响音乐情感感知的一个重要因素。到目前为止,人们对音色对音乐情绪的神经反应的影响知之甚少。为了解决这个问题,我们使用ERP来研究当相同的旋律以不同的音色呈现时,是否对音乐情绪有不同的神经反应。在跨模态情感启动范式中,目标面孔被情感一致或不一致的旋律所启动,而没有小提琴、长笛和人声中的歌词。结果显示,在语音版本中,较大的P3和较大的左前分布LPC响应于情感不一致与一致的试验。然而,对于长笛版本,只有LPC效应被发现,它分布在中央-顶叶电极上。与人声和长笛版本不同,小提琴版本中观察到了N400的效果。这些发现揭示了当相同的旋律以不同的音色呈现时,神经对音乐情绪的不同反应模式,并为人类声音存在专门的神经反应的假设提供了证据。
{"title":"The Effects of Timbre on Neural Responses to Musical Emotion","authors":"Weixia Zhang, Fang Liu, Linshu Zhou, Wan-Chen Wang, Hanyuan Jiang, Cunmei Jiang","doi":"10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.134","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.134","url":null,"abstract":"Timbre is an important factor that affects the perception of emotion in music. To date, little is known about the effects of timbre on neural responses to musical emotion. To address this issue, we used ERPs to investigate whether there are different neural responses to musical emotion when the same melodies are presented in different timbres. With a cross-modal affective priming paradigm, target faces were primed by affectively congruent or incongruent melodies without lyrics presented in the violin, flute, and voice. Results showed a larger P3 and a larger left anterior distributed LPC in response to affectively incongruent versus congruent trials in the voice version. For the flute version, however, only the LPC effect was found, which was distributed over centro-parietal electrodes. Unlike the voice and flute versions, an N400 effect was observed in the violin version. These findings revealed different patterns of neural responses to musical emotion when the same melodies were presented in different timbres, and provide evidence for the hypothesis that there are specialized neural responses to the human voice.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2019-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.134","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43908239","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Information-theoretic Modeling of Perceived Musical Complexity 感知音乐复杂性的信息理论建模
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-12-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.165
Sarah A. Sauvé, M. Pearce
What makes a piece of music appear complex to a listener? This research extends previous work by Eerola (2016), examining information content generated by a computational model of auditory expectation (IDyOM) based on statistical learning and probabilistic prediction as an empirical definition of perceived musical complexity. We systematically manipulated the melody, rhythm, and harmony of short polyphonic musical excerpts using the model to ensure that these manipulations systematically varied information content in the intended direction. Complexity ratings collected from 28 participants were found to positively correlate most strongly with melodic and harmonic information content, which corresponded to descriptive musical features such as the proportion of out-of-key notes and tonal ambiguity. When individual differences were considered, these explained more variance than the manipulated predictors. Musical background was not a significant predictor of complexity ratings. The results support information content, as implemented by IDyOM, as an information-theoretic measure of complexity as well as extending IDyOM9s range of applications to perceived complexity.
是什么让一段音乐对听者来说显得复杂?本研究扩展了Eerola(2016)之前的工作,研究了基于统计学习和概率预测的听觉期望计算模型(IDyOM)生成的信息内容,作为感知音乐复杂性的经验定义。我们使用该模型系统地操纵了短复调音乐节选的旋律、节奏和和声,以确保这些操作在预期的方向上系统地改变了信息内容。研究人员发现,从28名参与者那里收集的复杂性评级与旋律和和声信息内容呈正相关,而旋律和和声信息内容与描述性音乐特征(如走调音符的比例和音调模糊)相对应。当考虑到个体差异时,这些因素比被操纵的预测因素更能解释方差。音乐背景对复杂性评分没有显著的预测作用。结果支持IDyOM实现的信息内容作为复杂性的信息理论度量,并将idyom9的应用范围扩展到感知复杂性。
{"title":"Information-theoretic Modeling of Perceived Musical Complexity","authors":"Sarah A. Sauvé, M. Pearce","doi":"10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.165","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.165","url":null,"abstract":"What makes a piece of music appear complex to a listener? This research extends previous work by Eerola (2016), examining information content generated by a computational model of auditory expectation (IDyOM) based on statistical learning and probabilistic prediction as an empirical definition of perceived musical complexity. We systematically manipulated the melody, rhythm, and harmony of short polyphonic musical excerpts using the model to ensure that these manipulations systematically varied information content in the intended direction. Complexity ratings collected from 28 participants were found to positively correlate most strongly with melodic and harmonic information content, which corresponded to descriptive musical features such as the proportion of out-of-key notes and tonal ambiguity. When individual differences were considered, these explained more variance than the manipulated predictors. Musical background was not a significant predictor of complexity ratings. The results support information content, as implemented by IDyOM, as an information-theoretic measure of complexity as well as extending IDyOM9s range of applications to perceived complexity.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2019-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.165","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"47886071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Microtiming and Mental Effort 微计时和脑力劳动
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-12-01 DOI: 10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.111
Jo Fougner Skaansar, B. Laeng, A. Danielsen
The present study tested two assumptions concerning the auditory processing of microtiming in musical grooves (i.e., repeating, movement-inducing rhythmic patterns): 1) Microtiming challenges the listener's internal framework of timing regularities, or meter, and demands cognitive effort. 2) Microtiming promotes a “groove” experience—a pleasant sense of wanting to move along with the music. Using professional jazz musicians and nonmusicians as participants, we hypothesized that microtiming asynchronies between bass and drums (varying from −80 to 80 ms) were related to a) an increase in “mental effort” (as indexed by pupillometry), and b) a decrease in the quality of sensorimotor synchronization (as indexed by reduced finger tapping stability). We found bass/drums-microtiming asynchronies to be positively related to pupil dilation and negatively related to tapping stability. In contrast, we found that steady timekeeping (presence of eighth note hi-hat in the grooves) decreased pupil size and increased tapping performance, though there were no conclusive differences in pupil response between musicians and nonmusicians. However, jazz musicians consistently tapped with higher stability than nonmusicians, reflecting an effect of rhythmic expertise. Except for the condition most closely resembling real music, participants preferred the on-the-grid grooves to displacements in microtiming and bass-succeeding-drums-conditions were preferred over the reverse.
本研究验证了两种关于音乐凹槽(即重复的、运动诱导的节奏模式)中微节拍的听觉加工假设:1)微节拍挑战了听者的内在节拍规则框架,需要认知努力。2)微计时促进了一种“节奏”体验——一种想要随着音乐移动的愉悦感。使用专业爵士音乐家和非音乐家作为参与者,我们假设低音和鼓之间的微计时异步(从- 80到80毫秒不等)与a)“精神努力”的增加(通过瞳孔测量指标)和b)感觉运动同步质量的下降(通过手指敲击稳定性降低指标)有关。我们发现低音/鼓-微定时异步与瞳孔扩张呈正相关,与敲击稳定性负相关。相比之下,我们发现稳定的计时(在凹槽中出现八分音符)减少了瞳孔的大小,增加了敲击的表现,尽管音乐家和非音乐家之间的瞳孔反应没有决定性的差异。然而,爵士乐手的打拍子比非乐手更稳定,这反映了节奏专业知识的影响。除了最接近真实音乐的情况外,参与者更喜欢网格上的凹槽而不是微定时的位移,低音接替鼓的情况比相反的情况更受欢迎。
{"title":"Microtiming and Mental Effort","authors":"Jo Fougner Skaansar, B. Laeng, A. Danielsen","doi":"10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.111","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.111","url":null,"abstract":"The present study tested two assumptions concerning the auditory processing of microtiming in musical grooves (i.e., repeating, movement-inducing rhythmic patterns): 1) Microtiming challenges the listener's internal framework of timing regularities, or meter, and demands cognitive effort. 2) Microtiming promotes a “groove” experience—a pleasant sense of wanting to move along with the music. Using professional jazz musicians and nonmusicians as participants, we hypothesized that microtiming asynchronies between bass and drums (varying from −80 to 80 ms) were related to a) an increase in “mental effort” (as indexed by pupillometry), and b) a decrease in the quality of sensorimotor synchronization (as indexed by reduced finger tapping stability). We found bass/drums-microtiming asynchronies to be positively related to pupil dilation and negatively related to tapping stability. In contrast, we found that steady timekeeping (presence of eighth note hi-hat in the grooves) decreased pupil size and increased tapping performance, though there were no conclusive differences in pupil response between musicians and nonmusicians. However, jazz musicians consistently tapped with higher stability than nonmusicians, reflecting an effect of rhythmic expertise. Except for the condition most closely resembling real music, participants preferred the on-the-grid grooves to displacements in microtiming and bass-succeeding-drums-conditions were preferred over the reverse.","PeriodicalId":47786,"journal":{"name":"Music Perception","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2019-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1525/mp.2019.37.2.111","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"42977943","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 10
期刊
Music Perception
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1