首页 > 最新文献

Infant and Child Development最新文献

英文 中文
Registered Reports with secondary developmental data: Introduction to the special issue 使用二次开发数据的注册报告:特刊简介
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2506
Pamela E. Davis-Kean, Alexa Ellis, Moin Syed
<p>Events of the past decade have revealed substantial limitations in our standard approach to evaluating manuscripts for publication. Preference for ‘positive results’ and findings that are surprising or novel has led to a substantial publication bias that casts doubt on large portions of the existing literature (Davis-Kean & Ellis, <span>2019</span>; Scheel et al., <span>2021</span>). Registered Reports represent a major initiative to combat these problems, as they shift the focus of evaluation from the nature of the findings to the strength of the conceptualization, research design and analytic plan (Chambers, <span>2013</span>).</p><p>Contrary to the standard review process, with Registered Reports the process is split into two distinct stages. Authors initially submit a Stage 1 proposal consisting of the Introduction, Method and Planned Analysis sections prior to conducting the study. The Stage 1 proposal is sent for peer review, with an ultimate positive outcome of an ‘in principle acceptance,’ which is a guarantee that the journal will publish the full article, regardless of the results, providing the authors conduct the study as planned and do so competently. Following the in principle acceptance authors collect the data and/or conduct the analysis and then submit the Stage 2 report for final review. For more information, and answers to frequently asked questions about Registered Reports, see https://cos.io/rr/.</p><p>Registered Reports have been increasingly adopted in journals across the sciences in general, and psychology in particular. Although uptake had initially been slow amongst developmental journals, this has changed considerably in recent years (see Syed et al., <span>2023</span>, for Registered Reports specifically and Silverstein et al., <span>2024</span>, for open science and metascience more generally). Nevertheless, there remain many questions about how the format works for complex longitudinal designs and secondary data (van den Akker et al., <span>2021</span>), both of which are common in developmental research (see also Syed & Donnellan, <span>2020</span>).</p><p>The purpose of this Special Issue was to feature Registered Reports using secondary (pre-existing) data pertaining to developmental issues from the prenatal period through early adulthood. Secondary datasets refer to data collected by someone other than the primary user. Datasets used in the eight articles featured in the special issue covered wide ground. Two of the articles relied on the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, one to investigate how early caregiver interactions are related to educational attainment, income and employment (Duncan et al., <span>2024</span>), and the other to compare relations between parental sensitivity and two different methods for assessing attachment (Nivison et al., <span>2024</span>). Examining parenting from a different perspective, Wright and Jack
过去十年中发生的事件揭示了我们评估发表稿件的标准方法存在很大的局限性。对 "正面结果 "以及令人惊讶或新颖的发现的偏好导致了大量的出版偏见,使人们对大部分现有文献产生怀疑(Davis-Kean &amp; Ellis, 2019; Scheel et al.)注册报告是应对这些问题的一项重大举措,因为它们将评估重点从研究结果的性质转移到概念化、研究设计和分析计划的优势上(Chambers,2013)。在开展研究之前,作者首先要提交一份由引言、方法和计划分析部分组成的第 1 阶段建议书。第 1 阶段的建议书将送交同行评审,最终的肯定结果是 "原则上接受",这就保证了只要作者按计划进行研究,并能胜任,无论结果如何,期刊都会发表全文。原则上接受后,作者收集数据和/或进行分析,然后提交第二阶段报告供终审。有关注册报告的更多信息和常见问题的解答,请参阅 https://cos.io/rr/.Registered 报告已越来越多地被科学期刊采用,心理学期刊尤其如此。虽然发展类期刊最初采用报告的速度较慢,但近年来这种情况已大为改观(具体参见 Syed 等人,2023;Silverstein 等人,2024;开放科学和元科学)。然而,对于复杂的纵向设计和二手数据(van den Akker et al.,2021),这种格式如何发挥作用仍有许多问题,而这两种情况在发展研究中都很常见(另见 Syed &amp; Donnellan, 2020)。本特刊的目的是介绍使用与产前至成年早期发展问题有关的二手(已有)数据的注册报告。二级数据集是指由主要用户以外的其他人收集的数据。特刊中的八篇文章所使用的数据集覆盖面很广。其中两篇文章依赖于美国国家儿童健康与人类发展研究所(National Institute of Child Health and Human Development)的 "儿童早期保育与青少年发展研究"(Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development),一篇用于调查早期照料者之间的互动与教育程度、收入和就业之间的关系(Duncan et al.,2024),另一篇用于比较父母敏感性与两种不同的依恋评估方法之间的关系(Nivison et al.,2024)。Wright 和 Jackson(2024 年)从另一个角度研究了养育子女的问题,他们利用德国社会经济小组研究,测试了父母和子女的人格特质对各种青少年结果(如健康、教育、家庭和公民参与)的独立预测能力。有几项研究侧重于学校教育背景:Spiegler等人(2024年)利用 "欧洲四国移民子女纵向调查 "研究了同伴伤害与班级种族多样性和教师互动的关系;Kim和Sidney(2024年)利用 "国际数学和科学趋势研究 "研究了学生学业自我概念个体差异的影响来源;Zimmermann等人(2024年)利用 "2011年幼儿纵向研究幼儿园班级 "了解了学前班入学与进入和离开幼儿园期间社会发展指标的关系。最后,有两项研究考察了发展的生理方面:Chaku 和 Barry(2024 年)利用青少年大脑认知发展研究了解荷尔蒙特征与认知发展的关系,Woods 等人(2024 年)利用家庭未来和儿童福祉研究测试睡眠指标是否与认知和行为发展纵向相关。从本期特刊发表的文章中可以清楚地看出,对于那些使用现有数据集或二手数据集的人来说,注册报告是一个可行的选择,而且这些文章只是触及了可用数据集的表面,这些数据集可以很好地利用注册报告。二级数据集的其他一些例子包括但不限于:《监测未来》、《收入动态面板研究》、《国家纵向调查》、《英国队列研究》、《苏格兰的成长》等(有关评论,请参见 Davis-Kean &amp; Ellis, 2019,以及 Davis-Kean 等人,2015,有关如何访问这些数据集和其他数据集的更多详情)。 所有这些数据集以及更多数据集都有可能适用于注册报告,但注册报告格式却未得到充分利用。首先,审稿人有时会纠结于如何审阅那些不包含数据,只包含观点和方法的 "建议 "的论文。特别是对于二手数据,审稿人会因为已经收集了现有数据而无法就新的原始数据收集提出建议。正如 Davis-Kean 等人(2015 年)所指出的,使用二手数据回答什么问题往往受到限制。无法选择在现有数据的基础上增加数据或改变研究方法。审稿人往往会建议对这些论文进行这样的修改,但这是不可能的。因此,对今后的二手数据登记报告提出的一项建议是,应提供非常明确的审稿人信息,说明审稿人的作用,以及审稿人可向作者提出的要求的限制。基本上,研究的方法和数据结构一般都是已知的,审稿人的工作是确定问题是否与研究领域相关,以及所使用的方法和数据集是否适合回答所提出的问题。审稿人和作者之间的互动存在一个挑战,这在预注册和注册报告中都很明显,那就是经常需要进行稳健性检查,这些检查以前可能没有注册过,但对验证研究结果很重要。这些通常被视为探索性分析,但当评审人注意到数据集中有一个既可信又可检验的替代假说时,往往会合理地要求对研究结果的有效性进行检查。原始数据的收集可能会限制进行稳健性检验的变量的广度,而二手数据则不同,它通常拥有可用于检验的可用构造。我们建议作者和审稿人在第 1 阶段建议阶段就考虑增加可利用现有数据进行检验的替代规格或假设。不过,有时这种认识是在事后产生的,因此必须作为探索性分析添加到第二阶段稿件中。我们鼓励作者这样做,也鼓励编辑和审稿人为其提供便利,只要添加的内容得到透明的报告即可,因为我们总是希望确保我们的研究进行了最严格的测试。最后,在本特刊的最初送审稿中,有时提交的实际问题并不是该领域的主要问题,或者无法用提议的数据集进行充分测试。对二手数据的任何分析都可以进行,但重要的是我们都要考虑是否应该这样做。注册报告的主要目标之一是确保一项研究无论结果如何,都能为其研究问题提供信息。这一设计特点要求高质量的研究,尤其适合最复杂、最具争议性和最具变革性的研究,因为它对经常困扰发展研究的可疑研究实践和出版偏见提供了最明确的约束(Davis-Kean &amp; Ellis, 2019)。将这一格式提升到注册报告作者身份的格式,表明为获得研究结果而进行的极端谨慎和精心策划,这将有助于增强人们对婴幼儿发展研究的信心。
{"title":"Registered Reports with secondary developmental data: Introduction to the special issue","authors":"Pamela E. Davis-Kean,&nbsp;Alexa Ellis,&nbsp;Moin Syed","doi":"10.1002/icd.2506","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2506","url":null,"abstract":"&lt;p&gt;Events of the past decade have revealed substantial limitations in our standard approach to evaluating manuscripts for publication. Preference for ‘positive results’ and findings that are surprising or novel has led to a substantial publication bias that casts doubt on large portions of the existing literature (Davis-Kean &amp; Ellis, &lt;span&gt;2019&lt;/span&gt;; Scheel et al., &lt;span&gt;2021&lt;/span&gt;). Registered Reports represent a major initiative to combat these problems, as they shift the focus of evaluation from the nature of the findings to the strength of the conceptualization, research design and analytic plan (Chambers, &lt;span&gt;2013&lt;/span&gt;).&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Contrary to the standard review process, with Registered Reports the process is split into two distinct stages. Authors initially submit a Stage 1 proposal consisting of the Introduction, Method and Planned Analysis sections prior to conducting the study. The Stage 1 proposal is sent for peer review, with an ultimate positive outcome of an ‘in principle acceptance,’ which is a guarantee that the journal will publish the full article, regardless of the results, providing the authors conduct the study as planned and do so competently. Following the in principle acceptance authors collect the data and/or conduct the analysis and then submit the Stage 2 report for final review. For more information, and answers to frequently asked questions about Registered Reports, see https://cos.io/rr/.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Registered Reports have been increasingly adopted in journals across the sciences in general, and psychology in particular. Although uptake had initially been slow amongst developmental journals, this has changed considerably in recent years (see Syed et al., &lt;span&gt;2023&lt;/span&gt;, for Registered Reports specifically and Silverstein et al., &lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;, for open science and metascience more generally). Nevertheless, there remain many questions about how the format works for complex longitudinal designs and secondary data (van den Akker et al., &lt;span&gt;2021&lt;/span&gt;), both of which are common in developmental research (see also Syed &amp; Donnellan, &lt;span&gt;2020&lt;/span&gt;).&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;The purpose of this Special Issue was to feature Registered Reports using secondary (pre-existing) data pertaining to developmental issues from the prenatal period through early adulthood. Secondary datasets refer to data collected by someone other than the primary user. Datasets used in the eight articles featured in the special issue covered wide ground. Two of the articles relied on the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, one to investigate how early caregiver interactions are related to educational attainment, income and employment (Duncan et al., &lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;), and the other to compare relations between parental sensitivity and two different methods for assessing attachment (Nivison et al., &lt;span&gt;2024&lt;/span&gt;). Examining parenting from a different perspective, Wright and Jack","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 2","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2506","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140556308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The affective, cognitive, and physiological effects of implementing antecedent‐focused emotion regulation strategies in childhood 在儿童期实施以前因后果为重点的情绪调节策略对情感、认知和生理的影响
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-12 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2509
Elizabeth L. Davis, Shannon M. Brady, Kasey Pankratz, Zariah Tolman, Parisa Parsafar, Emily W. Shih
Different components of emotional responding may be affected by using specific emotion regulation strategies that enable children's volitional self‐regulation. This study examined the affective, cognitive, and physiological effects of experimentally instructing children to deploy distraction or reappraisal to regulate negative emotion during an evocative film clip. One‐hundred eighty‐four 4‐ to 11‐year‐old children [M = 7.66 years; SD = 2.33 years; 94 girls; mixed race (36%), Latino/Latina (30%), European American (19%), African American (11%), Asian American (2%), or other (2%)] participated. Neither strategy affected observed distress or self‐reported negative emotion. Relative to a control condition, children instructed to use reappraisal reported attenuated rumination. Distraction also predicted attenuated rumination, as well as a pattern of parasympathetic reactivity indicative of disengagement that correlated with parents' reported use of minimizing and punitive emotion socialization practices. Findings underscore the utility of multi‐method approaches that examine parasympathetic activity in conjunction with volitional measures of self‐regulation.
通过使用特定的情绪调节策略来实现儿童的意志自我调节,可能会对情绪反应的不同组成部分产生影响。本研究考察了实验指导儿童在观看唤起性电影片段时,通过转移注意力或重新评价来调节负面情绪所产生的情感、认知和生理影响。184名4至11岁的儿童[中位数=7.66岁;标准差=2.33岁;94名女孩;混血(36%)、拉丁/拉美裔(30%)、欧美裔(19%)、非裔(11%)、亚裔(2%)或其他(2%)]参加了这项研究。这两种策略都不会影响观察到的痛苦或自我报告的负面情绪。与对照组相比,接受重新评估指导的儿童的反刍有所减少。分散注意力也预示着反刍的减弱,以及副交感神经反应的模式,这种模式表明脱离,与父母报告的使用最小化和惩罚性情绪社会化做法相关。研究结果凸显了结合自我调节的意志测量来检查副交感神经活动的多种方法的实用性。
{"title":"The affective, cognitive, and physiological effects of implementing antecedent‐focused emotion regulation strategies in childhood","authors":"Elizabeth L. Davis, Shannon M. Brady, Kasey Pankratz, Zariah Tolman, Parisa Parsafar, Emily W. Shih","doi":"10.1002/icd.2509","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2509","url":null,"abstract":"Different components of emotional responding may be affected by using specific emotion regulation strategies that enable children's volitional self‐regulation. This study examined the affective, cognitive, and physiological effects of experimentally instructing children to deploy distraction or reappraisal to regulate negative emotion during an evocative film clip. One‐hundred eighty‐four 4‐ to 11‐year‐old children [<jats:italic>M</jats:italic> = 7.66 years; <jats:italic>SD</jats:italic> = 2.33 years; 94 girls; mixed race (36%), Latino/Latina (30%), European American (19%), African American (11%), Asian American (2%), or other (2%)] participated. Neither strategy affected observed distress or self‐reported negative emotion. Relative to a control condition, children instructed to use reappraisal reported attenuated rumination. Distraction also predicted attenuated rumination, as well as a pattern of parasympathetic reactivity indicative of disengagement that correlated with parents' reported use of minimizing and punitive emotion socialization practices. Findings underscore the utility of multi‐method approaches that examine parasympathetic activity in conjunction with volitional measures of self‐regulation.","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"294 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140551947","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Piloting a battery to evaluate parasympathetic reactivity and externalizing behaviours during early childhood in autism spectrum disorder 试用电池评估自闭症谱系障碍儿童早期的副交感神经反应性和外化行为
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-12 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2504
Yael Braverman, Madison Surmacz, Gina Schnur, Nasim Sheikhi, Susan Faja
Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia Reactivity (RSA‐R) correlates both positively and negatively with externalizing behaviour in autistic individuals. These inconsistencies may result from task‐based differences. This pilot study measured RSA‐R in 4‐to 6‐year‐olds, across two timepoints, using four validated tasks with matched baseline and challenge periods. Social, cognitive, sensory and emotional tasks were employed to evaluate the use of a domain‐specific approach in measuring RSA‐R in young autistic children. RSA and parent‐reported externalizing behaviour were collected from 16 children (Mage = 5.60 years; 13 male; 12 White/Caucasian; 15 non‐Hispanic/Latine). RSA‐R was measured by the difference score of the challenge task minus its corresponding comparison task. Correlations were computed to evaluate associations between RSA‐R and behaviour. RSA was reliably measured for 3/4 tasks (0.694 ≤ intraclass correlation coefficients [ICCs] ≤ 0.896). Only RSA‐R during a social task correlated with externalizing behaviour. These results support using a battery that measures a range of challenges, differing in social demands, to characterize how arousal contributes to emotion regulation demands among young autistic children.
呼吸窦性心律失常反应性(RSA-R)与自闭症患者的外化行为既有正相关也有负相关。这些不一致可能是基于任务的差异造成的。这项试验性研究使用四种经过验证的任务,在两个时间点测量了 4-6 岁儿童的 RSA-R,并匹配了基线期和挑战期。研究采用了社交、认知、感官和情绪任务,以评估在测量自闭症幼儿的 RSA-R 时使用特定领域方法的效果。从 16 名儿童(年龄=5.60 岁;13 名男性;12 名白人/高加索人;15 名非西班牙裔/拉丁裔)中收集了 RSA 和家长报告的外化行为。RSA-R 以挑战任务得分减去相应对比任务得分的差值来衡量。计算相关性以评估 RSA-R 与行为之间的关联。3/4项任务的RSA测量结果可靠(0.694≤类内相关系数[ICC]≤0.896)。只有社交任务中的 RSA-R 与外化行为相关。这些结果支持使用测量一系列社会需求不同的挑战的电池来描述唤醒如何促进自闭症幼儿的情绪调节需求。
{"title":"Piloting a battery to evaluate parasympathetic reactivity and externalizing behaviours during early childhood in autism spectrum disorder","authors":"Yael Braverman, Madison Surmacz, Gina Schnur, Nasim Sheikhi, Susan Faja","doi":"10.1002/icd.2504","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2504","url":null,"abstract":"Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia Reactivity (RSA‐R) correlates both positively and negatively with externalizing behaviour in autistic individuals. These inconsistencies may result from task‐based differences. This pilot study measured RSA‐R in 4‐to 6‐year‐olds, across two timepoints, using four validated tasks with matched baseline and challenge periods. Social, cognitive, sensory and emotional tasks were employed to evaluate the use of a domain‐specific approach in measuring RSA‐R in young autistic children. RSA and parent‐reported externalizing behaviour were collected from 16 children (<jats:italic>M</jats:italic><jats:sub>age</jats:sub> = 5.60 years; 13 male; 12 White/Caucasian; 15 non‐Hispanic/Latine). RSA‐R was measured by the difference score of the challenge task minus its corresponding comparison task. Correlations were computed to evaluate associations between RSA‐R and behaviour. RSA was reliably measured for 3/4 tasks (0.694 ≤ intraclass correlation coefficients [ICCs] ≤ 0.896). Only RSA‐R during a social task correlated with externalizing behaviour. These results support using a battery that measures a range of challenges, differing in social demands, to characterize how arousal contributes to emotion regulation demands among young autistic children.","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"3 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140551970","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschool children's high-frequency heart rate variability during low and high emotional challenge in relation to their self-regulation 学龄前儿童在低情绪挑战和高情绪挑战时的高频心率变异性与自我调节的关系
IF 2.2 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-11 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2507
Caron A. C. Clark, Patricia Cardellini de Almeida, Keyoor Joshi
High-frequency heart rate variability (hf-HRV) theoretically provides a biomarker for self-regulation, although studies with young children offer mixed findings regarding the relevance of emotional demands in this link. We aimed to describe variation in children's hf-HRV during tasks with relatively high and low emotional load and to determine the relation of hf-HRV during these tasks to different behavioural measures of children's self-regulation. Electrocardiograms were recorded in 80 3–5-year-olds (M = 57 months; 54% male, 47% female; 87% White, 8% mixed/other race, 2% African American/Black, 1% were Asian and 1% Hispanic/Latinx) whilst they completed a Go/No-go task with low emotional load and an emotionally challenging Delay Frustration task. Mean hf-HRV was similar across these tasks, although it increased during a between-task rest interval. Accounting for age, gender and caregiver education, hf-HRV during the Go/No-go task correlated with task performance, whereas hf-HRV during the emotional task correlated with caregiver-reported regulation (Psuedo R2 = 03–0.05). Greater hf-HRV withdrawal during the tasks correlated with weaker Go/No performance and increased behavioural frustration (Psuedo R2 = 0.08–0.13). Children's maintenance of hf-HRV during emotional and cognitive challenges may support their effective self-regulation.
理论上,高频心率变异性(hf-HRV)可作为自我调节的生物标志物,但对幼儿进行的研究对情绪要求在这一环节中的相关性给出了不同的结论。我们的目的是描述儿童在完成情绪负荷相对较高和较低的任务时 hf-HRV 的变化,并确定这些任务中 hf-HRV 与儿童自我调节的不同行为测量之间的关系。研究记录了 80 名 3-5 岁儿童(男=57 个月;54% 为男性,47% 为女性;87% 为白人,8% 为混血/其他种族,2% 为非裔美国人/黑人,1% 为亚裔,1% 为西班牙裔/拉丁裔)在完成低情绪负荷的 "走/不走 "任务和具有情绪挑战性的 "延迟挫败 "任务时的心电图。在这些任务中,平均 hf-HRV 值相似,但在任务间歇休息期间有所增加。考虑到年龄、性别和照顾者的教育程度,在 "走/停 "任务中,hf-HRV 与任务表现相关,而在情绪任务中,hf-HRV 与照顾者报告的调节相关(Psuedo R2 = 03-0.05)。在任务过程中,hf-HRV 的退缩程度越大,则 Go/No 的表现越弱,行为挫折感越强(Psuedo R2 = 0.08-0.13)。儿童在情绪和认知挑战中保持 hf-HRV 可能有助于他们进行有效的自我调节。
{"title":"Preschool children's high-frequency heart rate variability during low and high emotional challenge in relation to their self-regulation","authors":"Caron A. C. Clark, Patricia Cardellini de Almeida, Keyoor Joshi","doi":"10.1002/icd.2507","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2507","url":null,"abstract":"High-frequency heart rate variability (hf-HRV) theoretically provides a biomarker for self-regulation, although studies with young children offer mixed findings regarding the relevance of emotional demands in this link. We aimed to describe variation in children's hf-HRV during tasks with relatively high and low emotional load and to determine the relation of hf-HRV during these tasks to different behavioural measures of children's self-regulation. Electrocardiograms were recorded in 80 3–5-year-olds (<i>M</i> = 57 months; 54% male, 47% female; 87% White, 8% mixed/other race, 2% African American/Black, 1% were Asian and 1% Hispanic/Latinx) whilst they completed a Go/No-go task with low emotional load and an emotionally challenging Delay Frustration task. Mean hf-HRV was similar across these tasks, although it increased during a between-task rest interval. Accounting for age, gender and caregiver education, hf-HRV during the Go/No-go task correlated with task performance, whereas hf-HRV during the emotional task correlated with caregiver-reported regulation (<i>Psuedo R</i><sup>2</sup> = 03–0.05). Greater hf-HRV withdrawal during the tasks correlated with weaker Go/No performance and increased behavioural frustration (<i>Psuedo R</i><sup>2</sup> = 0.08–0.13). Children's maintenance of hf-HRV during emotional and cognitive challenges may support their effective self-regulation.","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"21 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-04-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140545484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infant sleep and anxiety disorders in early childhood: Findings from an Australian pregnancy cohort study 婴儿睡眠与幼儿期焦虑症:澳大利亚妊娠队列研究的结果
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-12 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2501
Kelli K. MacMillan, Declan Bourke, Stuart J. Watson, Andrew J. Lewis, Douglas M. Teti, Helen L. Ball, Megan Galbally

Emphasis on continuous infant sleep overnight may be driven by parental concern of risk to child mental health outcomes. The Mercy Pregnancy and Emotional Wellbeing Study (MPEWS) examined whether infant sleep at 6 and 12 months postpartum predicts anxiety disorders at 2–4 years, and whether this is moderated by maternal depression, active physical comforting (APC) or maternal cognitions about infant sleep. Data included 349 women and infants. Infant sleep was measured using the Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire and child anxiety disorders by the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment. The risk of developing generalised anxiety or social phobia disorders at 3–4 years was reduced by 42% (p = 0.001) and 31% (p = 0.001), respectively, for a one standard deviation increase in total sleep at 12 months. No other infant sleep outcomes were associated. Maternal depression, APC and cognitions about infant sleep did not significantly moderate these relationships. Focus may need to be on total infant sleep, rather than when sleep is achieved.

Highlights

  • To assess whether infant sleep outcomes (i.e., frequency of nocturnal wakes; nocturnal wakefulness and total sleep per day) at 6 and 12 months predict early childhood anxiety disorders at 3–4 years of age.
  • Maternally reported infant sleep outcomes were not associated with the risk of developing early childhood anxiety disorders at 3–4 years.
  • It may be total infant sleep, irrespective of when sleep occurs or night waking and, independently, active physical comforting that requires further investigation.
强调婴儿整夜连续睡眠可能是由于父母担心儿童心理健康会受到影响。美慈妊娠与情感幸福研究(MPEWS)考察了产后6个月和12个月的婴儿睡眠是否会预测2-4岁时的焦虑症,以及这是否会受到产妇抑郁、积极的身体安慰(APC)或产妇对婴儿睡眠的认知的影响。数据包括 349 名妇女和婴儿。婴儿睡眠采用婴儿睡眠简明问卷进行测量,儿童焦虑症则采用学龄前精神病学评估进行测量。12个月时总睡眠时间每增加一个标准差,3-4岁时患广泛性焦虑症或社交恐惧症的风险就会分别降低42%(p = 0.001)和31%(p = 0.001)。其他婴儿睡眠结果均无关联。母亲抑郁、APC 和对婴儿睡眠的认知并没有明显缓和这些关系。重点 评估婴儿 6 个月和 12 个月时的睡眠结果(即夜醒频率、夜醒次数和每天的总睡眠时间)是否能预测 3-4 岁儿童早期焦虑症。母亲报告的婴儿睡眠结果与3-4岁时罹患幼儿焦虑症的风险无关。需要进一步研究的可能是婴儿的总睡眠时间,而与睡眠时间或夜间唤醒无关,也与积极的身体安慰有关。
{"title":"Infant sleep and anxiety disorders in early childhood: Findings from an Australian pregnancy cohort study","authors":"Kelli K. MacMillan,&nbsp;Declan Bourke,&nbsp;Stuart J. Watson,&nbsp;Andrew J. Lewis,&nbsp;Douglas M. Teti,&nbsp;Helen L. Ball,&nbsp;Megan Galbally","doi":"10.1002/icd.2501","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2501","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Emphasis on continuous infant sleep overnight may be driven by parental concern of risk to child mental health outcomes. The Mercy Pregnancy and Emotional Wellbeing Study (MPEWS) examined whether infant sleep at 6 and 12 months postpartum predicts anxiety disorders at 2–4 years, and whether this is moderated by maternal depression, active physical comforting (APC) or maternal cognitions about infant sleep. Data included 349 women and infants. Infant sleep was measured using the Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire and child anxiety disorders by the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment. The risk of developing generalised anxiety or social phobia disorders at 3–4 years was reduced by 42% (<i>p</i> = 0.001) and 31% (<i>p</i> = 0.001), respectively, for a one standard deviation increase in total sleep at 12 months. No other infant sleep outcomes were associated. Maternal depression, APC and cognitions about infant sleep did not significantly moderate these relationships. Focus may need to be on total infant sleep, rather than when sleep is achieved.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>To assess whether infant sleep outcomes (i.e., frequency of nocturnal wakes; nocturnal wakefulness and total sleep per day) at 6 and 12 months predict early childhood anxiety disorders at 3–4 years of age.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Maternally reported infant sleep outcomes were not associated with the risk of developing early childhood anxiety disorders at 3–4 years.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>It may be total infant sleep, irrespective of when sleep occurs or night waking and, independently, active physical comforting that requires further investigation.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2501","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140118017","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential cognitive correlates in processing symbolic and situational mathematics 符号数学和情境数学处理过程中的认知相关性差异
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2500
Jiaxin Cui, Fan Yang, Yuanyi Peng, Saisai Wang, Xinlin Zhou

Symbolic and situational mathematics are the two major representations of mathematical knowledge. Although previous literature has studied the relationship between the two from the perspective of teaching practice, learning effectiveness and behavioural performance, there is still a lack of empirical psychological research on cognitive mechanisms to explore the psychological processes of the two. The current study investigated the relationship between symbolic and situational mathematics by determining the differences in cognitive correlates between the two in fourth-grade children. Their symbolic and situational mathematics abilities were assessed using symbolic and situational enumeration tests under the same conditions. Several types of general cognitive abilities, language processing and academic achievements were also examined. Results showed that both situational and symbolic mathematics are crucial for mathematical achievement. Arithmetic computation is closely correlated with symbolic mathematics, whereas spatial processing and inductive reasoning ability are uniquely correlated with situational mathematics. The results suggest that situational and symbolic mathematics have separate cognitive correlates, which means the two are distinct in terms of psychological processing.

符号数学和情境数学是数学知识的两大表征。虽然以往的文献从教学实践、学习效果和行为表现等角度对二者的关系进行了研究,但仍缺乏对认知机制的实证心理学研究来探讨二者的心理过程。本研究通过确定四年级儿童符号数学和情境数学在认知相关性方面的差异,来研究两者之间的关系。他们的符号数学和情境数学能力是在相同条件下通过符号枚举测试和情境枚举测试进行评估的。此外,还考察了几种一般认知能力、语言处理能力和学业成绩。结果表明,情境数学和符号数学对数学成绩至关重要。算术计算与符号数学密切相关,而空间处理和归纳推理能力则与情境数学独特相关。研究结果表明,情境数学和符号数学具有不同的认知相关性,这意味着二者在心理处理方面是截然不同的。
{"title":"Differential cognitive correlates in processing symbolic and situational mathematics","authors":"Jiaxin Cui,&nbsp;Fan Yang,&nbsp;Yuanyi Peng,&nbsp;Saisai Wang,&nbsp;Xinlin Zhou","doi":"10.1002/icd.2500","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2500","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Symbolic and situational mathematics are the two major representations of mathematical knowledge. Although previous literature has studied the relationship between the two from the perspective of teaching practice, learning effectiveness and behavioural performance, there is still a lack of empirical psychological research on cognitive mechanisms to explore the psychological processes of the two. The current study investigated the relationship between symbolic and situational mathematics by determining the differences in cognitive correlates between the two in fourth-grade children. Their symbolic and situational mathematics abilities were assessed using symbolic and situational enumeration tests under the same conditions. Several types of general cognitive abilities, language processing and academic achievements were also examined. Results showed that both situational and symbolic mathematics are crucial for mathematical achievement. Arithmetic computation is closely correlated with symbolic mathematics, whereas spatial processing and inductive reasoning ability are uniquely correlated with situational mathematics. The results suggest that situational and symbolic mathematics have separate cognitive correlates, which means the two are distinct in terms of psychological processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140069832","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
COVID-19 pandemic impacts on kindergarteners' mental health: A qualitative study of perspectives of U.S. mothers with low income COVID-19 大流行对幼儿园儿童心理健康的影响:对美国低收入母亲观点的定性研究
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-03 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2503
Khara L. P. Turnbull, Brianna Jaworski, Deiby Mayaris Cubides Mateus, Frances L. Coolman, Jennifer LoCasale-Crouch, Rachel Y. Moon, Fern R. Hauck, Ann Kellams, Eve R. Colson

Because the COVID-19 pandemic has been implicated in increased mental health concerns for families of low income, we aimed to describe maternal perspectives about the pandemic's impact on their kindergartener's mental health during the 2020–2021 school year. We conducted 22 in-depth, semi-structured interviews with U.S. mothers with low income who had kindergarten-age children (50% male and 50% female). All participants were female, ranging in age from 24 to 44 years, and reported the following ethnic/racial identities: non-Hispanic Black or African American (41%), Hispanic of any race (36%) and non-Hispanic, White (23%). With a team comprising multiple researchers from varied disciplines (e.g., medicine, education and public health) our analytic process used an iterative approach for developing and revising codes and themes until we reached thematic saturation. Most mothers described negative impacts on social, behavioural and emotional aspects of mental health. Some described positive social impacts, including strengthened family relationships. Mothers described no positive changes to behavioural or emotional aspects of mental health. Maternal perspectives suggest the importance of prioritising access to screening and treating mental health needs to support children's kindergarten transition, mitigate pandemic impacts and plan for future disruptions.

由于 COVID-19 大流行导致低收入家庭对心理健康的关注增加,我们旨在描述 2020-2021 学年大流行对其幼儿园孩子心理健康影响的母亲观点。我们对有幼儿园学龄儿童的美国低收入母亲(50% 为男性,50% 为女性)进行了 22 次深入的半结构式访谈。所有参与者均为女性,年龄从 24 岁到 44 岁不等,报告的民族/种族身份如下:非西班牙裔黑人或非裔美国人(41%)、任何种族的西班牙裔(36%)和非西班牙裔白人(23%)。我们的分析团队由来自不同学科(如医学、教育和公共卫生)的多位研究人员组成,在分析过程中,我们采用了迭代的方法来制定和修改代码和主题,直至达到主题饱和。大多数母亲描述了对心理健康的社会、行为和情感方面的负面影响。一些母亲描述了积极的社会影响,包括家庭关系的加强。母亲们没有描述心理健康在行为或情感方面的积极变化。母亲们的观点表明,必须优先考虑筛查和治疗心理健康需求,以支持儿童的幼儿园过渡,减轻大流行病的影响,并为未来的干扰做好计划。
{"title":"COVID-19 pandemic impacts on kindergarteners' mental health: A qualitative study of perspectives of U.S. mothers with low income","authors":"Khara L. P. Turnbull,&nbsp;Brianna Jaworski,&nbsp;Deiby Mayaris Cubides Mateus,&nbsp;Frances L. Coolman,&nbsp;Jennifer LoCasale-Crouch,&nbsp;Rachel Y. Moon,&nbsp;Fern R. Hauck,&nbsp;Ann Kellams,&nbsp;Eve R. Colson","doi":"10.1002/icd.2503","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2503","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Because the COVID-19 pandemic has been implicated in increased mental health concerns for families of low income, we aimed to describe maternal perspectives about the pandemic's impact on their kindergartener's mental health during the 2020–2021 school year. We conducted 22 in-depth, semi-structured interviews with U.S. mothers with low income who had kindergarten-age children (50% male and 50% female). All participants were female, ranging in age from 24 to 44 years, and reported the following ethnic/racial identities: non-Hispanic Black or African American (41%), Hispanic of any race (36%) and non-Hispanic, White (23%). With a team comprising multiple researchers from varied disciplines (e.g., medicine, education and public health) our analytic process used an iterative approach for developing and revising codes and themes until we reached thematic saturation. Most mothers described negative impacts on social, behavioural and emotional aspects of mental health. Some described positive social impacts, including strengthened family relationships. Mothers described no positive changes to behavioural or emotional aspects of mental health. Maternal perspectives suggest the importance of prioritising access to screening and treating mental health needs to support children's kindergarten transition, mitigate pandemic impacts and plan for future disruptions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/icd.2503","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140026532","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How still? Parent–infant interaction during the still-face and later infant attachment 如何静止?静止时父母与婴儿的互动与婴儿日后的依恋关系
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-03 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2492
Yeojin A. Ahn, Katherine Martin, Emily B. Prince, Sy-Miin Chow, Jeffrey F. Cohn, Jue Wang, Elizabeth A. Simpson, Daniel S. Messinger

In the still-face episode of the Face-to-Face/Still-Face (FFSF), parents are asked to become unresponsive. However, infant–parent interaction may be irrepressible, and there is some evidence that interaction during the still-face is associated with attachment outcome. To explore these questions, we independently coded the continuous affective valence (negative to positive) of seventy-three 6-month-old infants (45 males; 36 Hispanic/Latinx; 38 White, 3 Black/African-American, 2 Asian, 30 multiracial) and their parents (Mage = 36 years; 5 males; 30 Hispanic/Latinx; 65 White, 3 Black/African-American, 2 Asian, 2 unknown) during the FFSF and assessed attachment at 15 months with the Strange Situation Procedure (n = 66). There was a mean positive correlation between moment-to-moment parent and infant affective valence, indicating synchronous affective interaction during the still-face (d = 0.63). Higher levels of affect interaction during the still-face episode were detected in infants later classified as disorganised compared to securely attached (d = 0.97). Findings underscore the importance of testing for still-face interaction and suggest that this interaction may be an unappreciated predictor of infant attachment outcomes.

在 "面对面/静止面对面"(FFSF)的 "静止面对面 "环节中,父母被要求变得毫无反应。然而,婴儿与父母之间的互动可能是不可抑制的,而且有证据表明,静止时的互动与依恋结果有关。为了探讨这些问题,我们对73名6个月大的婴儿(45名男性;36名西班牙裔/拉丁裔;38名白人、3名黑人/非裔美国人、2名亚裔、30名多种族)和他们的父母(年龄=36岁;5名男性;30名西班牙裔/拉丁裔;65名白人、3名黑人/非裔美国人、2名亚裔、2名未知)在静止面对面过程中的连续情感价位(从消极到积极)进行了独立编码,并在婴儿15个月时用陌生情境程序(n = 66)对依恋进行了评估。父母和婴儿的情感价位在瞬间之间存在平均正相关,表明在静止面孔期间存在同步情感互动(d = 0.63)。与安全依恋的婴儿相比,后来被归类为无组织依恋的婴儿在静止表情过程中的情感互动水平更高(d = 0.97)。研究结果强调了测试静止面互动的重要性,并表明这种互动可能是预测婴儿依恋结果的一个未被重视的因素。
{"title":"How still? Parent–infant interaction during the still-face and later infant attachment","authors":"Yeojin A. Ahn,&nbsp;Katherine Martin,&nbsp;Emily B. Prince,&nbsp;Sy-Miin Chow,&nbsp;Jeffrey F. Cohn,&nbsp;Jue Wang,&nbsp;Elizabeth A. Simpson,&nbsp;Daniel S. Messinger","doi":"10.1002/icd.2492","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2492","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In the still-face episode of the Face-to-Face/Still-Face (FFSF), parents are asked to become unresponsive. However, infant–parent interaction may be irrepressible, and there is some evidence that interaction during the still-face is associated with attachment outcome. To explore these questions, we independently coded the continuous affective valence (negative to positive) of seventy-three 6-month-old infants (45 males; 36 Hispanic/Latinx; 38 White, 3 Black/African-American, 2 Asian, 30 multiracial) and their parents (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 36 years; 5 males; 30 Hispanic/Latinx; 65 White, 3 Black/African-American, 2 Asian, 2 unknown) during the FFSF and assessed attachment at 15 months with the Strange Situation Procedure (<i>n</i> = 66). There was a mean positive correlation between moment-to-moment parent and infant affective valence, indicating synchronous affective interaction during the still-face (<i>d</i> = 0.63). Higher levels of affect interaction during the still-face episode were detected in infants later classified as disorganised compared to securely attached (<i>d</i> = 0.97). Findings underscore the importance of testing for still-face interaction and suggest that this interaction may be an unappreciated predictor of infant attachment outcomes.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140034972","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Understanding the relation between socioeconomic status and elementary science achievement: A quantile regression approach 了解社会经济地位与小学科学成绩之间的关系:量化回归方法
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-28 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2502
Zachary T. Barnes, Ashley A. Edwards, Susanne Strachota, Yi Feng, Jessica Logan

A student's socioeconomic status (SES) has a significant relation to their academic achievement. Much of this work has explored this in math and reading, but less is known about how SES relates to science achievement, particularly in the early grades. Using quantile regression with nationally representative data, we explored this relation in 12,676 kindergarten students (51.2% boys, 52.2% White, 21.8% Hispanic, 12.7% Black and 6.6% Asian) and 10,339 fifth-grade students (51.3% boys, 49.4 White, 27.2% Hispanic, 9.4% Black and 8.2% Asian). We found less variability in science achievement scores for those high on SES than those low on SES. The scores of the high SES students cluster together on the high end of science achievement, whilst those from low SES score across the distribution. These findings highlight the need to explore what can mitigate the relation between SES and science achievement and where resources to support science achievement are most needed.

学生的社会经济地位(SES)与他们的学业成绩有很大关系。这方面的研究主要集中在数学和阅读方面,但对于社会经济地位与科学成绩的关系,尤其是低年级学生的科学成绩,了解较少。我们利用具有全国代表性的数据,对 12,676 名幼儿园学生(51.2% 的男生、52.2% 的白人、21.8% 的西班牙裔、12.7% 的黑人和 6.6% 的亚裔)和 10,339 名五年级学生(51.3% 的男生、49.4% 的白人、27.2% 的西班牙裔、9.4% 的黑人和 8.2% 的亚裔)进行了量化回归,探讨了这种关系。我们发现,与社会经济地位低的学生相比,社会经济地位高的学生的科学成绩得分差异较小。社会经济地位高的学生的分数集中在科学成绩的高端,而社会经济地位低的学生的分数则分布不均。这些研究结果突出表明,有必要探讨是什么因素可以缓解社会经济地位与科学成绩之间的关系,以及哪些方面最需要资源来支持科学成绩的提高。
{"title":"Understanding the relation between socioeconomic status and elementary science achievement: A quantile regression approach","authors":"Zachary T. Barnes,&nbsp;Ashley A. Edwards,&nbsp;Susanne Strachota,&nbsp;Yi Feng,&nbsp;Jessica Logan","doi":"10.1002/icd.2502","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2502","url":null,"abstract":"<p>A student's socioeconomic status (SES) has a significant relation to their academic achievement. Much of this work has explored this in math and reading, but less is known about how SES relates to science achievement, particularly in the early grades. Using quantile regression with nationally representative data, we explored this relation in 12,676 kindergarten students (51.2% boys, 52.2% White, 21.8% Hispanic, 12.7% Black and 6.6% Asian) and 10,339 fifth-grade students (51.3% boys, 49.4 White, 27.2% Hispanic, 9.4% Black and 8.2% Asian). We found less variability in science achievement scores for those high on SES than those low on SES. The scores of the high SES students cluster together on the high end of science achievement, whilst those from low SES score across the distribution. These findings highlight the need to explore what can mitigate the relation between SES and science achievement and where resources to support science achievement are most needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139994803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Beliefs, practices and support needs of preschool teachers toward shy-withdrawn behaviours 学前教师对害羞行为的看法、做法和支持需求
IF 2.8 4区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1002/icd.2499
Maryse Guedes, Manuela Veríssimo, António J. Santos

Shy-withdrawn behaviours place preschoolers at increased risk of experiencing adverse developmental outcomes. Positive teacher-child relationships play a protective role against these negative socioemotional outcomes. This study aimed to understand, in-depth, the beliefs, practices and support needs of preschool teachers toward shy-withdrawn children. Thirty preschool teachers of children aged 3–5 years were interviewed, using a semistructured guide. The thematic analysis revealed that most preschool teachers were aware of the main manifestations, transactional influences and consequences of shy-withdrawn behaviours, and of the empirically validated strategies that may be useful to modify them. However, a noteworthy proportion of participants reported misconceptions about the intentionality of shy-withdrawn behaviours, negative emotions and inconsistent or less effective practices. Our findings highlight that strengthening pre-service and in-service training and providing coaching to the preschool staff may be helpful to restructure dysfunctional beliefs about shy-withdrawn behaviours, translate developmental knowledge in evidence-based classroom practices and enhance professionals' self-awareness.

Highlights

  • This study explored, in-depth, the beliefs, practices and support needs of preschool teachers toward shy-withdrawn children.
  • Most of the 30 interviewed preschool teachers were aware of the main manifestations, transactional influences and consequences of shy-withdrawn behaviours.
  • Our findings highlight the need to strengthen teachers' training, bridging theoretical knowledge with professional development.
害羞和退缩行为会增加学龄前儿童出现不良发展结果的风险。积极的师幼关系对这些负面的社会情感结果起着保护作用。本研究旨在深入了解学前教师对害羞、退缩儿童的信念、做法和支持需求。采用半结构式指南,对 30 名 3-5 岁儿童的学前教师进行了访谈。专题分析表明,大多数学前教师都了解害羞--退缩行为的主要表现形式、交易影响和后果,以及经经验验证可能有助于改变这些行为的策略。然而,也有相当一部分参与者对害羞--退缩行为的故意性、消极情绪和不一致或不太有效的做法存在误解。我们的研究结果强调,加强职前和在职培训以及为学前教育人员提供辅导,可能有助于调整对羞怯行为的功能失调信念,将发展知识转化为以证据为基础的课堂实践,并提高专业人员的自我意识。在 30 位受访的学前教师中,大多数都了解害羞、退缩行为的主要表现、交易影响和后果。我们的研究结果凸显了加强教师培训、将理论知识与专业发展相结合的必要性。
{"title":"Beliefs, practices and support needs of preschool teachers toward shy-withdrawn behaviours","authors":"Maryse Guedes,&nbsp;Manuela Veríssimo,&nbsp;António J. Santos","doi":"10.1002/icd.2499","DOIUrl":"10.1002/icd.2499","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Shy-withdrawn behaviours place preschoolers at increased risk of experiencing adverse developmental outcomes. Positive teacher-child relationships play a protective role against these negative socioemotional outcomes. This study aimed to understand, in-depth, the beliefs, practices and support needs of preschool teachers toward shy-withdrawn children. Thirty preschool teachers of children aged 3–5 years were interviewed, using a semistructured guide. The thematic analysis revealed that most preschool teachers were aware of the main manifestations, transactional influences and consequences of shy-withdrawn behaviours, and of the empirically validated strategies that may be useful to modify them. However, a noteworthy proportion of participants reported misconceptions about the intentionality of shy-withdrawn behaviours, negative emotions and inconsistent or less effective practices. Our findings highlight that strengthening pre-service and in-service training and providing <i>coaching</i> to the preschool staff may be helpful to restructure dysfunctional beliefs about shy-withdrawn behaviours, translate developmental knowledge in evidence-based classroom practices and enhance professionals' self-awareness.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This study explored, in-depth, the beliefs, practices and support needs of preschool teachers toward shy-withdrawn children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Most of the 30 interviewed preschool teachers were aware of the main manifestations, transactional influences and consequences of shy-withdrawn behaviours.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Our findings highlight the need to strengthen teachers' training, bridging theoretical knowledge with professional development.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47820,"journal":{"name":"Infant and Child Development","volume":"33 4","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139976675","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infant and Child Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1